A Corpus-Based Study of Spatial Frames of References in Early Child Mandarin Chinese
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Definition of Spatial Frames of References
1.1.1. Taxonomy of FoRs
1.1.2. The Seven-Class FoRs
1.2. Acquisition of Spatial Frames of References in Early Years
1.3. Spatial FoRs: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet
1.4. The Context of This Study
- What are the basic linguistic features of spatial FoRs in early childhood Mandarin Chinese?
- What are the developmental patterns of spatial FoRs in early childhood Mandarin Chinese?
- Are the seven-class FoRs applicable to early childhood Mandarin Chinese?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. The Corpus
2.1.1. The Sample
2.1.2. Communication Task
2.2. Coding
2.2.1. The Seven-Class FoRs Coding Scheme
2.2.2. Coding Procedure
2.2.3. Inter-Rater Reliability
2.3. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics of the Language Materials
Example a. Child 1 at ages 2–6 | |||
(某物)在 | 我 | 面前 | |
(sth) be | my | front | |
(sth) is in front of me. | |||
Missing component: subject | |||
FoRs category: DR |
Example b. Child 2 at ages 4–6 | |||
(某物)在 | 蛋糕 | 后面 | |
(sth) be | cake | back | |
(sth) is at the back of the cake. | |||
Missing component: subject | |||
FoRs category: OCR |
Example c. Child 3 at ages 2–6 | |||
(某物) 向着 | 门 | ||
(sth) towards | door | ||
(sth) is towards the door. | |||
Missing component: subject | |||
FoRs category: LBR |
Example d. Child 4 at ages 3–6 | ||||
树 | 要(应该) | 在 | 上坡 | |
tree | should be | at | uphill | |
The tree should be on the uphill. | ||||
Missing component: appropriate verb | ||||
FoRs category: GR |
Example e. Child 5 at ages 5–6 | ||||
你 | 怎么 | 在门 | 右边 | (站着) |
you | why | door | right | (standing) |
Why (are) you (standing) on the right of the door? | ||||
Missing component: appropriate verb | ||||
FoRs category: RR |
3.2. Production Patterns of Spatial FoRs in Early Years
3.2.1. The Pattern of Linguistic Features
3.2.2. Developmental Patterns
3.3. Distribution Patterns of FoRs: Missing, Allocation, and Preferences
4. Discussion
4.1. Linguistic Features of Spatial FoRs in Early Child Mandarin
4.2. Developmental Trends of Spatial FoRs in Early Child Mandarin
4.3. Preferences of Using Spatial FoRs among Chinese Preschoolers
5. Conclusions, Limitations, and Implications
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. The Coding Scheme for Spatial Frames of Reference in Chinese Preschoolers
Step 1: Reference status (Diessel 2014) | ||
Coding Level (Code) | Sample Sentences | Definition |
(1) No reference (0) | (某物) 里面有巧克力。 There is chocolate inside (what). There is chocolate inside (sth). | There is no reference within the spatial sentence. |
(2) With reference (1) | 辣椒放在锅里。 Chilly put inside the pot. Chilly is put inside the pot. | There is one specific reference within the spatial sentence. |
(3) Demonstrative Reference (99) | 面包放这儿吧。 Bread put (my standpoint) here. Put the bread over here. | Demonstratives are used to refer to speaker’s standpoint serving as reference. |
Step 2: Classic level of spatial frame of reference (Levinson 2003) | ||
Coding level (code) | Sample sentences | Definition |
(0) No frame (N) [Extension of Levinson’s three level for the consideration of children’s language proficiency] | 放在大锅。 Put (be) the pot (in). Put in the pot. | There is no localizer or indicator to show the frame of reference (although there should be one). |
(1) Relative (R) | 猪在牛的右手边。 The pig is to the right of the cow. | Use the subject (speaker) as the reference. |
(2) Absolute (A) | 猪在牛的东边。 The pig is east of the cow. | Use the earth as the reference. |
(3) Intrinsic (I) | 猪在牛的前面。 The pig is in front of the cow. | Use the object itself as the reference. |
Step 3: Chinese spatial frame of reference (Danziger 2010; Levinson 2003; Liu 2014; O’Meara 2011; Shusterman and Li 2016;) | ||
Coding level (code) | Sample sentences | Definition |
1. Object-centred reference (OCR) | 皮球在椅子前面。 The ball is in front of the chair. | Refers to the object-based reference within the intrinsic system, the reference entity is movable and not stationary. |
2. People-centred reference (PCR) | 皮球在你的前面。 The ball is in front of you. | Refers to the coordinate that takes the others’ body position (such as the listeners or people other than the speakers themselves) as the reference. |
3. Absolute reference (AR) | 皮球在椅子的北方向。 The ball is north of the chair. | Specifically refers to the reference involved with cardinal directions (east/west/north/south). |
4. Geomorphic reference (GR) | 皮球在椅子的上游(方向)。 The ball is upstream from the chair. | Refers to a frame with one or more axes formed by the gradient of the geographical environment and could be projected onto the ground or figure. |
5. Landmark-based reference (LBR) | 皮球从椅子那儿朝着教堂。 The ball is toward the church from the chair. | Refers to a frame with the axis as a vector pointing from a ground or figure to a given landmark. |
6. Relative reference (RR) | 皮球在椅子右边。 The ball is to the right of the chair. | Relative (speaker’s viewpoint) as mentioned by Levinson’s definition. |
7. Direct reference (DR) | 皮球在我们前面。 The ball is in front of us. 皮球朝向我。 The ball is toward me. 皮球从椅子那朝向我。 The ball is toward me from the chair. | Refers to the coordinate that takes the speaker’s body position directly as the reference. |
References
- Adamou, Evangelia, and Xingjia R. Shen. 2017. Beyond language shift: Spatial cognition among the Ixcatecs in Mexico. Journal of Cognition and Culture 17: 94–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bakeman, Roger, and John M. Gottman. 1997. Observing Interaction: An Introduction to Sequential Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Berman, Phyllis W. 1976. Young children’s use of the frame of reference in the construction of the horizontal, vertical, and oblique. Child Development 47: 259–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bohnemeyer, Jürgen. 2011. Spatial frames of reference in Yucatec: Referential promiscuity and task-specificity. Language Sciences 33: 892–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bremner, Andrew J., Peter E. Bryant, and Denis Mareschal. 2006. Object-centred spatial reference in 4-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development 29: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brown, Penelope, and Stephen C. Levinson. 2000. Frames of spatial reference and their acquisition in Tenejapan Tzeltal. In Culture, Thought, and Development. Edited by L. Nucci, G. Saxe and E. Turiel. Mahwah: Erlbaum, pp. 167–97. [Google Scholar]
- Brownell, Cellia A., and Michael S. Carriger. 1991. Collaborations among toddler peers: Individual contributions to social contexts. In Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition. Edited by Lauren B. Resnick, John M. Levine and Stephanie D. Teasley. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 365–83. [Google Scholar]
- Burigo, Michele, and Holger Schultheis. 2018. The effects of direction and orientation of located objects on spatial language comprehension. Language and Cognition 10: 298–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlson, Laura A., and Shannon R. Van Deman. 2008. Inhibition within a reference frame during the interpretation of spatial language. Cognition 106: 384–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carlson-Radvansky, Laura A., and David E. Irwin. 1993. Frames of reference in vision and language: Where is above? Cognition 46: 223–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Craton, Lincoln G., James Elicker, Jodie M. Plumert, and Herbert L. Pick, Jr. 1990. Children’s use of frames of reference in communication of spatial location. Child Development 61: 1528–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danziger, Eve. 2010. Deixis, gesture, and cognition in spatial frame of reference typology. Studies in Language. International Journal sponsored by the Foundation “Foundations of Language” 34: 167–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de León, Lourdes. 1994. Exploration in the acquisition of geometric location by Tzotzil children. Linguistics 32: 857–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de León, Lourdes. 1995. They just hang out with the old fela: The development of geocentric location in young speakers of Guugu Yimithirr. In Cognitive Anthropology Research Group Working Paper 33. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute. [Google Scholar]
- DeLoache, Judy S. 1987. Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science 238: 1556–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- DeLoache, Judy S. 1989. Young children’s understanding of the correspondence between a scale model and a larger space. Cognitive Development 4: 121–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deng, Xiangjun, and Virginia Yip. 2015. The linguistic encoding of space in child Mandarin: A corpus-based study. Linguistics 53: 1079–112. [Google Scholar]
- Deng, Xiangjun, and Virginia Yip. 2016. Cognition and perception in the linguistic encoding of space in child Mandarin. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 44: 287–325. [Google Scholar]
- Diessel, Holger. 2014. Demonstratives, frames of reference, and semantic universals of space. Language and Linguistics Compass 8: 116–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galati, Gaspare, Gina Pelle, Alain Berthoz, and Giorgia Committeri. 2010. Multiple reference frames used by the human brain for spatial perception and memory. Experimental Brain Research 206: 109–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gräfenhain, Maria, Tanya Behne, Malinda Carpenter, and Michael Tomasello. 2009. Young children’s understanding of joint commitments. Developmental Psychology 45: 1430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Halliday, Michael A. 1985. Register variation. In Language, Context and Text: Aspects of Language in a Social-Semiotic Perspective. Geelong: Deakin University Press, pp. 29–43. [Google Scholar]
- Hamann, Katharina, Felix Warneken, and Michael Tomasello. 2012. Children’s developing commitments to joint goals. Child Development 83: 137–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haun, Daniel B., Christian J. Rapold, Gabriele Janzen, and Stephen C. Levinson. 2011. Plasticity of human spatial cognition: Spatial language and cognition covary across cultures. Cognition 119: 70–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika. 2006. How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26: 55–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huttenlocher, Janellen, Nora Newcombe, and Elisabeth H. Sandberg. 1994. The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology 27: 115–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Iachini, Tina, and Robert H. Logie. 2003. The role of perspective in locating position in a real-world, unfamiliar environment. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 17: 715–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janzen, Gabriele, Daniel B. Haun, and Stephen C. Levinson. 2012. Tracking down abstract linguistic meaning: Neural correlates of spatial frame of reference ambiguities in language. PloS ONE 7: e30657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koffka, Kurt. 1935. Principles of Gestalt Psychology. New York: A Harbinger Book. [Google Scholar]
- Le Guen, Oliver. 2011. Speech and gesture in spatial language and cognition among the Yucatec Mayas. Cognitive Science 35: 905–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levinson, Stephen C. 1996. Language and space. Annual Review of Anthropology 25: 353–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levinson, Stephen C. 2003. Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, vol. 5. [Google Scholar]
- Levinson, Stephen C., and David P. Wilkins. 2006. Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Peggy, and Linda Abarbanell. 2018. Competing perspectives on frames of reference in language and thought. Cognition 170: 9–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Jing, and Kan Zhang. 2009. Regional differences in spatial frame of reference systems for people in different areas of China. Perceptual and Motor Skills 108: 587–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Lijin. 2014. Spatial Frames of reference and topological descriptions in Chinese discourse. Journal of Beijing International Studies University 36: 24–32. (In Chinese). [Google Scholar]
- Lozano, Cristóbal, and Amaya Mendikoetxea. 2013. Corpus and experimental data: Subjects in second language research. In Twenty Years of Learner Corpus Research: Looking back, Moving ahead. Corpora and Language in Use—Proceedings 1. Edited by Sylviane Granger, Gaëtanelle Gilquin and Fanny Meunier. Louvain-la-Neuve: Presses Universitaires de Louvain, pp. 313–23. [Google Scholar]
- Majid, Asifa, Melissa Bowerman, Sotaro Kita, Daniel B. Haun, and Stephen C. Levinson. 2004. Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 108–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McHugh, Mary L. 2012. Interrater reliability: The kappa statistic. Biochemia Medica: Biochemia Medica 22: 276–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monaghan, Padraic, and Caroline F. Rowland. 2017. Combining language corpora with experimental and computational approaches for language acquisition research. Language Learning 67: 14–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Negen, James, and Marko Nardini. 2015. Four-year-olds use a mixture of spatial reference frames. PLoS ONE 10: e0131984. [Google Scholar]
- Newcombe, Nora, and Janellen Huttenlocher. 1992. Children’s early ability to solve perspective-taking problems. Developmental Psychology 28: 635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newcombe, Nora, Janellen Huttenlocher, Anna B. Drummey, and Judith G. Wiley. 1998. The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development 13: 185–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Meara, Carolyn. 2011. Spatial frames of reference in Seri. Language Sciences 33: 1025–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Meara, Carolyn, and Gabriela P. Báez. 2011. Spatial frames of reference in Mesoamerican languages. Language Sciences 33: 837–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pederson, Eric. 2003. How many reference frames? Spatial Cognition III: Routes and Navigation, Human Memory and Learning, Spatial Representation and Spatial Learning 2: 287–304. [Google Scholar]
- Pederson, Eric, Eve Danziger, David Wilkins, Stephen Levinson, Sotaro Kita, and Gunter Senft. 1998. Semantic typology and spatial conceptualisation. Language 74: 557–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perry, Lynn K., Larissa K. Samuelson, and John P. Spencer. 2009. Aligning body and world: Stable reference frames improve young children’s search for hidden objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102: 445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Polian, Gilles, and Jürgen Bohnemeyer. 2011. Uniformity and variation in Tseltal reference frame use. Language Sciences 33: 868–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruggiero, Gennaro, Ortensia D’Errico, and Tina Iachini. 2016. Development of egocentric and allocentric spatial representations from childhood to elderly age. Psychological Research 80: 259–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schutte, Anne R., and John P. Spencer. 2009. Tests of the dynamic field theory and the spatial precision hypothesis: Capturing a qualitative developmental transition in spatial working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 35: 1698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sekine, KazukiK. 2009. Changes in frame of reference use across the preschool years: A longitudinal study of the gestures and speech produced during route descriptions. Language and Cognitive Processes 24: 218–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shinohara, Kazuko, and Yoshihiro Matsunaka. 2004. Spatial cognition and linguistic expression: Empirical research on frames of reference in Japanese. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics 2: 261–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shusterman, Anna, and Peggy Li. 2016. Frames of reference in spatial language acquisition. Cognitive Psychology 88: 115–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Surtees, Andrew D., Matthijs L. Noordzij, and Ian A. Apperly. 2012. Sometimes losing yourself in space: Children’s and adults’ spontaneous use of multiple spatial reference frames. Developmental Psychology 48: 185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tse, Shek K., Carol Chan, Hui Li, and Sin M. Kwong. 2002. Sex differences in syntactic development: Evidence from Cantonese-speaking preschoolers in Hong Kong. International Journal of Behavioral Development 26: 509–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Dandan D., and Hui Li. 2017. Study on Children’s Use of the Mandarin Locative Prepositions: A Study Base don Corpus. Early Childhood Education (Educational Sciences), 33–36, (Peer-review Journal Articles in Chinese). [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, Zheng, and Ann E. Boehm. 2001. American and Chinese children’s knowledge of basic relational concepts. School Psychology International 22: 5–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Utterances | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
FoRs Category | Aged 2–6 | Aged 3–6 | Aged 4–6 | Aged 5–6 | ||||
[Type (MLU)] | Types (MLU) | WSR | Types (MLU) | WSR | Types (MLU) | WSR | Types (MLU) | WSR |
DR [30 (6.85)] | 4 (6.75) | 75% | 5 (6.40) | 60% | 9 (7.00) | 78% | 12 (7.25) | 83% |
GR [9 (5.80)] | 1 (3.00) | 0 | 1 (5.00) | 0 | 2 (8.00) | 50% | 5 (7.20) | 80% |
LBR [26 (8.38)] | 4 (8.25) | 50% | 7 (7.43) | 71% | 9 (8.33) | 78% | 6 (9.50) | 100% |
OCR [131 (7.45)] | 17 (7.05) | 65% | 24 (7.04) | 63% | 37 (7.54) | 65% | 53 (8.15) | 89% |
PCR [18 (8.41)] | 0 (-) | 0 | 2 (8.00) | 50% | 7 (7.57) | 57% | 9 (9.67) | 100% |
RR [41 (8.52)] | 5 (7.40) | 80% | 9 (7.33) | 67% | 10 (10.40) | 90% | 17 (8.94) | 94% |
AR [- (-)] | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Step 1. Reference Status | Step 2. General FoRs | Step 3. Seven-Class FoRs in This Study | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
No reference [0] | 306 (10.78%) | ||||
Demo reference [99] | 1735 (61.16%) | ||||
With reference [1] | 796 (28.06%) | No frame [N] | 11 (1.38%) | ||
Absolute [A] | 46 (5.78%) | Geomorphic reference [GR] | 11 (1.40%) | ||
Landmark-based reference [LBR] | 35 (4.46%) | ||||
Absolute reference [AR] | 0 (0%) | ||||
Intrinsic [I] | 435 (54.65%) | Object-centred reference [OCR] | 410 (52.23%) | ||
People-centred reference [PCR] | 25 (3.18%) | ||||
Relative [R] | 304 (38.19%) | Direct reference [DR] | 78 (9.94%) | ||
Relative reference [RR] | 226 (28.79%) | ||||
total | 2837 | total | 796 | total | 785 |
Aged 2–6 (n = 42) | Aged 3–6 (n = 42) | Aged 4–6 (n = 42) | Aged 5–6 (n = 42) | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sum | Mean | SD | Sum | Mean | SD | Sum | Mean | SD | Sum | Mean | SD | F | p | |
Static spatial sentences | 331 | 7.881 | 10.600 | 452 | 10.762 | 10.455 | 701 | 16.691 | 11.267 | 1353 | 32.214 | 20.646 | 25.554 | 0.000 |
No reference sentence | 40 | 0.952 | 1.710 | 63 | 1.500 | 2.340 | 98 | 2.333 | 2.138 | 105 | 2.500 | 2.452 | 4.665 | 0.004 |
Demonstrative reference | 206 | 4.905 | 7.999 | 255 | 6.071 | 6.961 | 390 | 9.286 | 6.992 | 884 | 21.048 | 14.002 | 25.623 | 0.000 |
Reference sentence | 85 | 2.024 | 2.434 | 134 | 3.191 | 3.549 | 213 | 5.071 | 4.571 | 364 | 8.667 | 6.842 | 16.435 | 0.000 |
Subtypes of reference sentence | ||||||||||||||
No frame sentence | 3 | 0.071 | 0.261 | 1 | 0.024 | 0.154 | 2 | 0.048 | 0.216 | 5 | 0.119 | 0.395 | 0.943 | 0.421 |
Direct reference | 15 | 0.357 | 1.144 | 17 | 0.405 | 1.037 | 20 | 0.476 | 0.890 | 26 | 0.619 | 0.882 | 0.554 | 0.646 |
Geomorphic reference | 1 | 0.024 | 0.154 | 1 | 0.024 | 0.154 | 4 | 0.095 | 0.370 | 5 | 0.119 | 0.328 | 1.386 | 0.249 |
Landmark-based reference | 5 | 0.119 | 0.328 | 10 | 0.238 | 0.617 | 7 | 0.167 | 0.437 | 13 | 0.310 | 0.680 | 1.021 | 0.385 |
Object-centred reference | 36 | 0.857 | 1.389 | 62 | 1.476 | 1.798 | 108 | 2.571 | 2.596 | 204 | 4.857 | 5.020 | 14.029 | 0.000 |
People-centred reference | 0 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 3 | 0.071 | 0.342 | 8 | 0.191 | 0.594 | 14 | 0.333 | 0.612 | 4.242 | 0.006 |
Relative reference | 25 | 0.595 | 1.127 | 40 | 0.952 | 1.481 | 64 | 1.524 | 1.838 | 97 | 2.310 | 2.342 | 7.624 | 0.000 |
Aged 2–6 (n = 42) | Aged 3–6 (n = 42) | Aged 4–6 (n = 42) | Aged 5–6 (n = 42) | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rsum | M | SD | Rsum | M | SD | Rsum | M | SD | Rsum | M | SD | F | Sig. | Msum | |
R_DR | 1.455 | 0.035 | 0.103 | 1.404 | 0.033 | 0.109 | 1.009 | 0.024 | 0.047 | 0.773 | 0.018 | 0.025 | 0.399 | 0.754 | 1.161 |
R_GR | 0.100 | 0.002 | 0.015 | 0.333 | 0.008 | 0.051 | 0.322 | 0.008 | 0.028 | 0.095 | 0.002 | 0.007 | 0.454 | 0.715 | 0.213 |
R_LBR | 0.519 | 0.012 | 0.044 | 1.033 | 0.025 | 0.070 | 0.313 | 0.007 | 0.020 | 0.396 | 0.009 | 0.022 | 1.283 | 0.282 | 0.565 |
R_OCR | 5.689 | 0.135 | 0.224 | 5.848 | 0.139 | 0.169 | 5.515 | 0.131 | 0.118 | 6.136 | 0.146 | 0.108 | 0.063 | 0.979 | 5.797 |
R_PCR | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.125 | 0.003 | 0.014 | 0.299 | 0.007 | 0.023 | 0.518 | 0.012 | 0.026 | 3.567 | 0.015 | 0.235 |
R_RR | 3.046 | 0.073 | 0.172 | 3.865 | 0.092 | 0.145 | 3.332 | 0.079 | 0.095 | 3.085 | 0.073 | 0.069 | 0.211 | 0.889 | 3.332 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wu, D.; Degotardi, S.; Li, H.; Tong, N.Y. A Corpus-Based Study of Spatial Frames of References in Early Child Mandarin Chinese. Languages 2024, 9, 318. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9100318
Wu D, Degotardi S, Li H, Tong NY. A Corpus-Based Study of Spatial Frames of References in Early Child Mandarin Chinese. Languages. 2024; 9(10):318. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9100318
Chicago/Turabian StyleWu, Dandan, Sheila Degotardi, Hui Li, and Nga Yui Tong. 2024. "A Corpus-Based Study of Spatial Frames of References in Early Child Mandarin Chinese" Languages 9, no. 10: 318. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9100318
APA StyleWu, D., Degotardi, S., Li, H., & Tong, N. Y. (2024). A Corpus-Based Study of Spatial Frames of References in Early Child Mandarin Chinese. Languages, 9(10), 318. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9100318