The Potential of Narrative for Understanding Protein Biosynthesis in the Context of Viral Infections
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Theoretical Framework
1.1.1. What Is a Story? More Than Just a Text Style
1.1.2. Narrative Realities, and their Consequences for Learning Science
1.2. State of Research
1.2.1. The Impact of (the Production of) Narrative Texts on the Understanding of Biological Phenomena
1.2.2. Students’ Ideas about Protein Biosynthesis and Virus Reproduction
1.2.3. Research Questions
- (1)
- To what extent does the task of writing a narrative or expository text really lead to the students producing the required text type?
- (2)
- What is the relationship between the produced text type (narrative or expository text) and the degree of scientific correctness of the written texts regarding eight concepts of protein biosynthesis in the context of virus reproduction?
- (3)
- How do the students evaluate their own writing process, i.e., to what extent did the respective intervention help them understand the biological topic and its different aspects?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Sample and Context of the Study
2.2. Sampling Procedure
2.3. Intervention (Text Production)
2.4. Data Collection and Data Analysis
2.4.1. Data Collection
2.4.2. Measurement
- Narrativity of the texts
- Scientific Correctness
- scientifically correct certifies that the student has a comprehensive understanding of the specific concept in accordance with the scientific ideas taught in the lesson. It means that they not only mention the concept but explain all the different aspects of the specific concept coherently.
- scientifically imprecise describes a basic understanding of the specific concept compared to the scientific ideas; however, with limitations in the detailed understanding or in the ability of presentation. It means that they not only mention the concept but at least describe some of the different aspects of the specific concept.
- incorrect certifies a lack of or a deficient understanding of the specific concept in comparison with the scientific ideas or indicates that the concept is not part of the learners’ written text. It means that they only mention the concept without further detailed description of aspects of the specific concept or do not mention the concept at all.
- Post-test questionnaire
2.4.3. Data Analysis
- Scientific Correctness
- Post-test questionnaire
3. Results
3.1. Text Analysis for Narrativity
3.2. Text Analysis for Scientific Correctness
3.2.1. Descriptive Statistics Suggest Differences between the Concepts Rather Than between the Interventions
3.2.2. In a Score Analysis, the Two Interventions Do Not Differ as to Overall Scientific Correctness
3.2.3. In a Descriptive View, There Are Considerable Differences between the Concepts Overall, but Much Less between the Interventions within These Concepts
3.2.4. For Some Concepts, the Frequency of Scientifically Correct or Scientifically Imprecise Descriptions Differs Statistically between the Interventions
3.3. Analysis of Post-Test Questionnaires
3.3.1. The Text Authors Estimate That Stories and Expository Texts Promoted Their Comprehension to Roughly the Same Extent
3.3.2. According to the Participants, 7 out of 16 Aspects of the Topic Were Addressed More Frequently in Expository Texts, Only 1 More Frequently Than in Narrative Texts
3.3.3. The Students’ Metacognition Does Not Indicate Any Advantage for One of the Two Text Types, unless They Are Compared Directly
3.4. Summary of Results
3.4.1. Narrativity and Scientific Correctness of the Texts
3.4.2. Post-Test Questionnaires
4. Discussion
4.1. Research Gaps and Challenges of the Field
4.2. The Biological Topic and Methodology Used in This Study
4.3. Discussion of the Results, Related to the Research Questions 1–3
4.3.1. To What Extent Does the Task of Writing a Narrative or Expository Text Really Lead to the Students Producing the Required Text Type?
4.3.2. What Is the Relationship between the Produced Text Type (Narrative or Expository Text) and the Degree of Scientific Correctness of the Written Texts Regarding Eight Concepts of Protein Biosynthesis in the Context of Virus Reproduction?
4.3.3. How do the Students Evaluate Their Own Writing Process, i.e., to What Extent Did the Respective Intervention Help Them Understand the Biological Topic and Its Different Aspects?
4.4. Implications for Research on Narrative in Science Teaching, and for Teaching Practice (RQ 4)
4.5. Limitations
Author Contributions
Funding
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Norris, S.P.; Guilbert, S.M.; Smith, M.L.; Hakimelahi, S.; Phillips, L.M. A theoretical framework for narrative explanation in science. Sci. Educ. 2005, 89, 535–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruner, J.S. The Culture of Education; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Bruner, J.S. Actual Minds, Possible Worlds; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Millar, R.; Osborne, J. Beyond 2000, Science Education for the Future. The Report of a Seminar Series Funded by the Nuffield Foundation; King’s College London, School of Education: London, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Rumelhart, D.E. Notes on a Schema for Stories. In Representation and Understanding; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1975; pp. 211–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mandler, J.M.; Johnson, N.S. Remembrance of Things Parsed: Story Structure and Recall. Cogn. Psychol. 1977, 9, 111–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stein, N.L.; Nezworski, T. The effects of organization and instructional set on story memory∗. Discourse Process 1978, 1, 177–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mandler, G. Mind and Body: Psychology of Emotion and Stress; W W Norton & Co Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Gropengießer, H. Lernen und Lehren. In Gehirn Und Lernen; Siebert, H., Ed.; Bertelsmann: Bielefeld, Germany, 2003; pp. 29–39. [Google Scholar]
- Lakoff, G. The Invariance Hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image-schemas? Cogn. Linguist. 1990, 1, 39–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lakoff, G.; Johnson, M. Metaphors We Live By; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Zabel, J. Narrative and Metaphor as Conceptual Tools for Understanding Evolution Theory. In Innovazione Nella Didattica Delle Scienze Nella Scuola Primaria e Dell’infanzia’; Universitas Studiorum: Mantova, Italy, 2015; Volume 3. [Google Scholar]
- Echterhoff, G.; Straub, J. Narrative Psychologie: Facetten eines Forschungsprogramms (Teil 1). Handl. Kult. Interpret. 2003, 12, 317–342. [Google Scholar]
- Bruner, J.S. Acts of Meaning: Four Lectures on Mind and Culture; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1990; Volume 3. [Google Scholar]
- Kurth, L.A.; Kidd, R.; Gardner, R.; Smith, E.L. Student use of narrative and paradigmatic forms of talk in elementary science conversations. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2002, 39, 793–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruner, J.S. Search of Pedagogy Volume I; Routledge: London, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lhoste, Y.; Boiron, V.; Jaubert, M.; Orange, C.; Rebière, M. Le récit: Un outil pour prendre en compte le temps et l’espace et construire des savoirs en sciences ? RDST 2011, 4, 57–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Orange, C.; Orange Ravachol, D. Problématisations scientifiques fonctionnalistes et historiques en éducation relative à l’environnement et au développement durable: Le cas de l’évolution climatique. Rev. Des. Hautes Écoles Pédagogiques Inst. Assim. Suisse Rom. Du. Tessin 2017, 22, 21–38. [Google Scholar]
- Zabel, J. Biologie Verstehen: Die Rolle der Narration beim Verstehen der Evolutionstheorie. 2009. Available online: https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:bsz:15-qucosa2-796484 (accessed on 3 May 2024).
- Zabel, J.; Gropengießer, H. What can Narrative contribute to Students’ Understanding of Scientific Concepts, e.g., Evolution Theory? J. Eur. Teach. Educ. Netw. 2015, 10, 136–146. Available online: https://etenjournal.com/2020/02/07/what-can-narrative-contribute-to-students-understanding-of-scientific-concepts-e-g-evolution-theory/ (accessed on 3 May 2024).
- Paulin, F.; Bruguière, C.; Zabel, J. La sélection naturelle racontée: étude de cas chez des lycéens. In ACTES XIIe Rencontres Scientifiques ARDIST.; Venturini, P., Pélissier, L., Eds.; ARDIST: Toulouse, France, 2022; pp. 97–102. [Google Scholar]
- Soares, S.; Gonçalves, M.; Jerónimo, R.; Kolinsky, R. Narrating science: Can it benefit science learning, and how? A theoretical review. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2023, 60, 2042–2075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Avraamidou, L.; Osborne, J. The Role of Narrative in Communicating Science. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2009, 31, 1683–1707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Altun, D. From Story to Science: The Contribution of Reading Fiction and Hybrid Stories to Conceptual Change with Young Children. Child. Soc. 2019, 33, 453–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faria, C.; Guilherme, E.; Gaspar, R.; Boaventura, D. History of Science and Science Museums. Sci. Educ. 2015, 24, 983–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomas, L.; Ritchie, S.M.; Tones, M. Attitudinal impact of hybridized writing about a socioscientific issue. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2011, 48, 878–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ritchie, S.M.; Tomas, L.; Tones, M. Writing Stories to Enhance Scientific Literacy. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2011, 33, 685–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Csikar, E.; Stefaniak, J.E. The Utility of Storytelling Strategies in the Biology Classroom. Contemp. Educ. Technol. 2018, 9, 42–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Negrete, A.; Lartigue, C. The science of telling stories: Evaluating science communication via narratives (RIRC method). J. Media Commun. Stud. 2010, 2, 98–110. [Google Scholar]
- Prins, R.; Avraamidou, L.; Goedhart, M. Tell me a Story: The use of narrative as a learning tool for natural selection. EMI Educ. Media Int. 2017, 54, 20–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duit, R.; Treagust, D.F.; Widodo, A. Teaching Science for Conceptual Change: Theory and Practise. In International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change; Vosniadou, S., Ed.; Taylor & Francis: London, UK, 2013; pp. 487–503. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, F.; Kampourakis, K. Teaching for genetics literacy in the post-genomic era. Stud. Sci. Educ. 2017, 53, 193–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jördens, J.; Asshoff, R.; Kullmann, H.; Hammann, M. Providing vertical coherence in explanations and promoting reasoning across levels of biological organization when teaching evolution. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2016, 38, 960–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, R.G.; Reiser, B.J. Reasoning across ontologically distinct levels: Students’ understandings of molecular genetics. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2007, 44, 938–959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, J.; Kattmann, U. Traits, genes, particles and information: Re-visiting students’ understandings of genetics. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2004, 26, 195–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haskel-Ittah, M.; Yarden, A. Toward Bridging the Mechanistic Gap Between Genes and Traits by Emphasizing the Role of Proteins in a Computational Environment. Sci. Educ. 2017, 26, 1143–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gericke, N.; Wahlberg, S. Clusters of concepts in molecular genetics: A study of Swedish upper secondary science students understanding. J. Biol. Educ. 2013, 47, 73–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trauschke, M. Krieg im menschlichen Körper—Überfälle böser Viren. MNU 2008, 61, 493–499. [Google Scholar]
- Simonneaux, L. A study of pupils’ conceptions and reasoning in connection with “microbes”, as a contribution to research in biotechnology education. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2000, 22, 619–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rönner, A.C.; Jakobsson, A.; Gericke, N. “Bacteria are not viruses; viruses are more malicious”—Young pupils’ understanding of bacteria and viruses in the aftermath of COVID-19. J. Biol. Educ. 2023, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Staatsministerium für Kultus des Freistaates Sachsen, Ed.; Lehrplan Gymnasium Biologie; 2019. Available online: https://www.schulportal.sachsen.de/lplandb/index.php?lplanid=83&lplansc=aUFARA6HvFKFs2nxWtO3&token=f6612663b50335ac23f99c83f437d1c7 (accessed on 3 May 2024).
- Zohar, A.; Barzilai, S. A review of research on metacognition in science education: Current and future directions. Stud. Sci. Educ. 2013, 49, 121–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinez, M.; Scheffel, M. Einführung in die Erzähltheorie; Beck: München, Germany, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Schilcher, A.; Krauss, S.; Kirchhoff, P.; Katharina, A.-M.; Christina, E.; Michael, E.; Mario, F.; Lisa, G.; Maria, G.-P.; Eileen, G.; et al. FALKE: Experiences From Transdisciplinary Educational Research by Fourteen Disciplines. Front. Educ. 2021, 5, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Labov, W.; Waletzky, J. Erzählanalyse: Mündliche Versionen persönlicher Erfahrung. In Literaturwissenschaft Und Linguistik. Eine Auswahl. Texte Zur Theorie Der Literaturwissenschaft; Athenäum Fischer: Frankfurt/Main, Germany, 1973; Volume 2. [Google Scholar]
- Zabel, J. Geschichten für das Lernen nutzen. In Biologie-Methodik. Handbuch Für Die Sekundarstufe I Und II; Spörhase-Eichmann, U., Ruppert, W., Eds.; Cornelsen Verlage: Berlin, Germany, 2016; Volume 3, pp. 63–67. [Google Scholar]
- Levi, P.; Rosenthal, R. The Periodic Table; Penguin Books: London, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Egan, K. Primary Understanding: Education in Early Childhood; Routledge: London, UK, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Hadzigeorgiou, Y. Imaginative Science Education. The Central Role of Imagination in Science Education; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2016. [Google Scholar]
Concept | The Concept Contains the Description of… |
---|---|
Virus Structure | …viruses as organic structures with an outer protein coat with spike proteins containing DNA with genes in an inner protein coat (capsid). |
Virus Reproduction | …virus reproduction as a complex process with multiple steps: virus’ attachment on the host cells’ surfaces (adsorption), penetration and the release of the DNA virus genome into the host cell, followed by the replication and protein biosynthesis of virus’ genome into proteins, and assembly and release of new viruses. |
Flow of Genetic Information | …genetic information flow as information transfer from virus DNA to mRNA to their realization into amino acids of virus proteins. |
(Relevance of) DNA Replication | …DNA replication as continuously identical duplication of virus DNA by enzymes of the host cell. |
(Relevance of) Transcription | …transcription as transfer of genetic information from virus DNA to functional mRNA by enzymes of the host cell. |
(Relevance of) Translation | …translation as realization of mRNA into amino acids of virus proteins. |
Compartmentalization | …compartmentalization as involvement of different cell components during the process of virus reproduction (cell membrane, cell nucleus, cytoplasm, ribosomes). |
Interaction between Organization Levels | …interaction between organization levels as change of perspective to explain processes at subcellular, cellular, or higher levels of organization. |
Category for Narrativity | Anchor Example | |
---|---|---|
1 | Connections between causal and temporal events | “He went to a mucosal cell and made himself comfortable on it with his protein spikes. When it was no longer cozy enough, it penetrated the mucosal cell […]” |
2 | Causal connections that are rooted in underlying reasons or motives | “When he [virus] saw this, he became angry and wanted revenge. He devised a plan to do more harm to the human than the cell did to the virus. He began to reprogram it like a hacker so that the cell would do something very stupid for humans. Because suddenly the cell started to work and with the help of the enzymes the cell multiplied the virus DNA […]” |
3 | Problems that become the focus of attention in the plot | “This shock unexpectedly dissolved the virus until only his DNA was left. When he saw this, he became angry and wanted revenge. He made himself a plan […]” |
4 | Fictional elements | “Once upon a time there was a herpes virus and his name was Tim […]” |
Intervention “Narrative” (N = 46) Rel. Freq. (Abs. Freq.) | Intervention “Expository Text” (N = 22) Rel. Freq. (Abs. Freq.) | Total Sample (N = 68) Rel. Freq. (Abs. Freq.) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Concepts | Corr | Impr | inc/m | Corr | Impr | inc/m | Corr | Impr | inc/m |
Virus structure | 65.2 (30) | 10.9 (5) | 23.9 (11) | 77.3 (17) | 4.5 (1) | 18.2 (4) | 69.1 (47) | 8.8 (6) | 22.1 (15) |
Virus replication | 52.2 (24) | 19.6 (9) | 28.3 (13) | 27.3 (6) | 22.7 (5) | 50.0 (11) | 44.1 (30) | 20.6 (14) | 35.3 (24) |
Genetic information flow | 19.6 (9) | 30.4 (14) | 50.0 (23) | 22.7 (5) | 22.7 (5) | 54.5 (12) | 20.6 (14) | 27.9 (19) | 51.5 (35) |
DNA replication | 32.6 (15) | 47.8 (22) | 19.6 (9) | 36.4 (8) | 45.5 (10) | 18.2 (4) | 33.8 (23) | 47.1 (32) | 19.1 (13) |
Transcription | 26.1 (12) | 45.7 (21) | 28.3 (13) | 27.3 (6) | 50.0 (11) | 22.7 (5) | 26.5 (18) | 47.1 (32) | 26.5 (18) |
Translation | 34.8 (16) | 47.8 (22) | 17.4 (8) | 31.8 (7) | 27.3 (6) | 40.9 (9) | 33.8 (23) | 41.2 (28) | 25.0 (17) |
Compartmentalization | 78.3 (36) | 8.7 (4) | 13.0 (6) | 59.1 (13) | 36.4 (8) | 4.5 (1) | 72.1 (49) | 17.6 (12) | 10.3 (7) |
Levels of Organization | 41.3 (19) | 30.4 (14) | 28.3 (13) | 9.1 (2) | 54.5 (12) | 36.4 (8) | 30.9 (21) | 38.2 (26) | 30.9 (21) |
Total | 44 (161) | 30 (111) | 26 (96) | 36 (64) | 33 (58) | 31 (54) | 41 (225) | 31 (169) | 27 (150) |
Category for Scientific Correctness | Anchor Example…for Compartmentalization | |
---|---|---|
1 | Scientifically correct | “A long, long time ago, a lonely little virus called Herpes lived in a small hut on the outskirts of town. […] He was astonished when he stopped in front of a large building, the Host Cell. […] The door was locked from the outside, but his hand, one of his spike proteins, fitted perfectly into the notch in the door. The moment Herpes touched the structure, he found himself inside the cell […]. When one of these enzymes discovered him, he heard it call out loudly: “DNA, what are you doing here, you must be in the cell nucleus. I’ll take you there.” […] The employees told Herpes to go to the employee named RNA Polymerase who was in the same section of the building […] The employee sticks a sticker with the words mRNA on him and sends him out of the middle building. […] Little Herpes arrives in the room where a doctor called Ribosome is apparently in charge. Unfortunately, Herpes couldn’t see what he was doing there, but suddenly the employees brought her back to the main building.” (story by Meike) |
2 | Scientifically imprecise | “Welcome to Operation DNA 3 […] You are here to let us infect you with a DNA virus. Escape is not an option. […] In 43 min, everyone in this room will have a DNA virus developed by us in their bodies. The DNA viruses then introduce their genetic material into the cell nucleus, where they multiply. This is called replication. In the cell nucleus, they produce new viruses, known as transcription. […] Translation is the final step […] [and] takes place on ribosomes […]” (story by Hilde) |
3 | Scientifically incorrect | “They [DNA viruses] just go into my neighboring cells. […] He said that they use the cells as so-called host cells […] They enter the cell and release their genetic material. Many identical DNAs are formed there, but they still need a protective coating. They also get this and for this they produce mRNAs, which are then translated into virus proteins. The virus DNAs are then coated with their protective coating in the cell nucleus and then it’s off to the next cell […]” (story by Ben) |
Writing the Story/the Expository Text Has Improved My Understanding of the … | Mean NG | Mean EG | SD NG | SD EG | t-Value | p-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| 3.37 | 3.77 | 1.07 | 0.79 | 1.549 | 0.063 |
| 3.48 | 3.59 | 0.85 | 1.03 | 0.468 | 0.320 |
| 3.59 | 3.73 | 0.92 | 0.91 | 0.580 | 0.282 |
| 3.00 | 3.48 | 0.76 | 0.91 | 2.232 * | 0.014 |
| 2.43 | 2.91 | 0.80 | 1.28 | 1.566 | 0.061 |
| 3.09 | 3.00 | 0.95 | 1.00 | 0.342 | 0.367 |
| 2.74 | 3.09 | 1.03 | 1.20 | 1.227 | 0.112 |
| 3.37 | 3.55 | 0.96 | 0.89 | 0.711 | 0.234 |
| 4.24 | 4.27 | 0.91 | 0.91 | 0.140 | 0.444 |
| 4.30 | 4.36 | 0.83 | 0.83 | 0.272 | 0.393 |
| 3.38 | 3.71 | 1.02 | 1.08 | 1.233 | 0.111 |
| 3.52 | 3.68 | 0.88 | 0.82 | 0.708 | 0.241 |
Thematic Area | NG Abs. Freq. | EG Abs. Freq. | χ2 | p-Value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Yes | No | Yes | No | |||
Cell nucleus (structure) | 23 | 23 | 5 | 17 | 4.57 * | 0.033 |
Enzymes of DNA replication | 27 | 19 | 14 | 8 | 0.15 | 0.699 |
RNA replication (process) | 31 | 15 | 20 | 2 | 4.39 * | 0.036 |
Genetic information flow | 21 | 25 | 17 | 5 | 6.04 * | 0.014 |
Transcription (process) | 22 | 24 | 18 | 4 | 7.10 ** | 0.008 |
mRNA | 28 | 18 | 18 | 4 | 2.98 | 0.084 |
Translation (process) | 22 | 24 | 16 | 6 | 3.74 | 0.053 |
Ribosomes | 22 | 24 | 13 | 9 | 0.76 | 0.383 |
tRNA | 6 | 40 | 9 | 13 | 6.72 ** | 0.009 |
Amino acids and protein | 16 | 30 | 11 | 11 | 1.44 | 0.230 |
Codon and anticodon | 5 | 41 | 9 | 13 | 8.21 ** | 0.004 |
Start codon, stop codon | 3 | 43 | 6 | 16 | 5.58 * | 0.018 |
Genetic Code | 6 | 40 | 7 | 15 | 3.39 | 0.066 |
Structure of viruses | 35 | 11 | 19 | 3 | 0.96 | 0.327 |
Reproduction of viruses | 43 | 2 | 21 | 1 | 0.10 | 0.752 |
Change of organization levels | 16 | 30 | 9 | 13 | 0.24 | 0.624 |
Item | Mean NG | Mean EG | SD NG | SD EG | t-Value | p-Value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| 3.26 | 3.09 | 1.13 | 1.08 | 0.579 | 0.282 | |
| 3.37 | 3.23 | 1.09 | 1.28 | 0.469 | 0.320 | |
| 3.54 | 2.73 | 1.19 | 1.01 | 2.730 ** | 0.004 | |
| 2.85 | 2.64 | 1.41 | 1.30 | 0.583 | 0.281 | |
| 3.16 | 3.50 | 1.25 | 0.90 | 1.144 | 0.128 | |
(34/35) | By writing the [own text type], I have dealt with the topic in depth. | 3.78 | 3.71 | 1.14 | 0.88 | 0.244 | 0.404 |
(34 NG) | When learning scientific content, I have already occasionally used stories (story-like elements) (also mentally). | 2.27 | - | 1.31 | - | - | - |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zabel, J.; Averdunk, C. The Potential of Narrative for Understanding Protein Biosynthesis in the Context of Viral Infections. Educ. Sci. 2024, 14, 521. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14050521
Zabel J, Averdunk C. The Potential of Narrative for Understanding Protein Biosynthesis in the Context of Viral Infections. Education Sciences. 2024; 14(5):521. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14050521
Chicago/Turabian StyleZabel, Jörg, and Cornelia Averdunk. 2024. "The Potential of Narrative for Understanding Protein Biosynthesis in the Context of Viral Infections" Education Sciences 14, no. 5: 521. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14050521
APA StyleZabel, J., & Averdunk, C. (2024). The Potential of Narrative for Understanding Protein Biosynthesis in the Context of Viral Infections. Education Sciences, 14(5), 521. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14050521