4.1. Correction Degree of Problems Answers
Table 1 shows the percentages regarding the degree of correctness of the students’ answers to each of the five questions or problems of the task.
In total, four degrees of correctness of the answers are considered: correct, partially correct, incorrect, and does not answer/justify, that is, does not answer or only answers yes or no.
Questions 2–4 have not given rise to partially correct answers; therefore, this category is not considered for them.
As can be seen, only the correct answers predominate in questions Q2 and Q4, although with nuances.
In question Q2, more than half of the students answered correctly; however, in Q4, the percentage of correct answers did not reach 50%. In fact, this percentage is lower than that of incorrect answers, and they do not answer together.
On the contrary, in question Q3, as well as in those that require an argument, that is, in Q1 and Q5, incorrect answers predominate. Moreover, in Q5, practically one in four students did not answer.
Therefore, Q2 has turned out to be the easiest question, with Q5 being the most difficult. It should also be noted that questions Q2 and Q3 ask the same type of qualitative question. However, as has been seen, the percentage of correct answers in Q3 is notably lower than in Q2.
In fact, thirty-two students who answered Q2 correctly answered Q3 incorrectly, and four more did not answer it directly.
We think that this is due to the way of stating the problems, since, in Q2, the statement makes explicit, one-by-one, all the elements to be combined, while, in Q3, these are implicit. Consequently, students have difficulty understanding the situation posed by question Q3; that is, they do not pass the first problem-solving phase of Polya [
41].
4.2. Combinatorial Reasoning Resolutions
To calculate the number of menus requested by Q1–Q4the students, for the most part, have not shown the resolution strategy. Now, those who show it have followed the usual strategies for solving combinatorial problems [
15,
23,
24,
25,
26,
42]: graphical, which includes the tree diagram; enumeration, both systematic and a-systematic; and of arithmetic operations, such as the product or the sum, although the sum is not an adequate strategy to solve the types of problems raised. Likewise, other unusual strategies have been observed. In addition, as expected due to the age of the participants, no combinatorial operation was observed.
Table 2 shows the number of responses in which the strategy is not made explicit, as well as the number of responses in which a specific strategy is used. No-answer responses are not included.
As can be seen, in all the questions, the answers without strategy predominate; that is, answers in which the student only presents a numerical value as a solution.
As for resolutions with strategy, the procedure that predominates the most is arithmetic and, specifically, the product or multiplication rule. In other words, these students have recognized the multiplicative structure of problems. However, in some cases, as Mulligan and Mitchelmore warns [
35], they get confused and interpret the multiplicative situation of the Cartesian product as an additive situation.
Table 2 considers these strategies separately.
Likewise, in the line of Navarro-Pelayo et al. [
17] and Roldán et al. [
26], these results confirm that few students use the tree diagram to solve combinatorics problems; although, as observed in Q4, it has been the predominant technique.
Some aspects of the resolutions and strategies in
Table 2 are described in more detail below; for example, it will be seen which strategies lead to a correct answer and which do not.
In question Q1, the correct answer has been classified as the one in which the student argues that it cannot be that all the menus have been different because there are only 18 different possible menus. The category of partially correct includes the answers in which it is stated that it is not possible that 20 different menus have been served. However, in its argument, it does not explicitly allude to the fact that there can only be 18 different menus; that is, in the answer only an implicit count is identified (
Figure 1).
In Q2, the answer in which the student does not show a strategy, but answers 18 menus, has been considered valid.
The incorrect answers to the questions Q1 and Q2 are motivated by different cognitive conflicts of the students in some of the mathematical objects involved in the task. Thus, as in Burgos and Godino [
28], conflicts are grouped into three types: conceptual, procedural, and argumentative.
CC1: Student applies a wrong strategy to find the number of menus and, specifically, uses an additive strategy.
Figure 2 shows that the student adds the options offered by each type of dish and obtains eight menus. This is an example of typical error in arithmetic operations [
19], in which a multiplicative situation of Cartesian product is interpreted as an additive situation.
In another case, the student applies a wrong arithmetical strategy that combines product and addition (“Others” strategy in
Table 2). He does not make mistakes when multiplying, but in what he must multiply. This leads to a misunderstanding of the Cartesian product (
Figure 3).
PC1: The strategy is valid, but it is not correctly applied. As in the investigations of Navarro-Pelayo et al. [
17] and Gea et al. [
21], the non-systematic enumeration error is detected. Unlike Lamana et al. [
42], it is observed that, in most of the resolutions in which this technique is followed, not all possible solutions are found. It is supposed that the reason of this difference is the dimension of the number of solutions.
AC1: There are no signs of mathematical reasoning in the argumentation.
AC2: There are signs of combinatorial reasoning, but the proposition is not relevant (
Figure 4).
It should be noted that, in most incorrect answers without a strategy, an error of erroneous intuitive response [
17] is identified. In particular, many of them give eight menus as a solution. It is intuited that this result is motivated by the previously described CC1 conflict.
Table 3 shows the number of responses to questions Q1 and Q2, depending on the correction degree and strategy followed by the student. No-answer responses are not considered.
It is observed that the strategy that provides a greater number of correct answers in questions Q1 and Q2 is multiplication, and very few students have used enumeration or a graphical strategy to find the 18 menus. Likewise, a correct strategy (“Others” strategy) that combines tree diagram and multiplication has been identified (
Figure 5).
In addition, it is observed that, in general, the students maintain the strategy when answering both questions.
Another remarkable aspect is that, in both Q1 and Q2, there are more correct answers without strategy than with strategy: 34 vs. 27 in Q1 and 75 vs. 28 in Q2. However, it is also true that, proportionally, more correct answers are observed in those resolutions with a strategy than in those that do not follow a strategy (
Table 4).
Table 5 shows the number of responses to Q3, based on the correction degree and strategy followed, as well as Q2 to be able to compare, given that they pose the same type of problem. No-answer responses are not considered.
As can be seen in
Table 5, no student has followed the enumeration strategy to solve Q3. However, the decomposition strategy is identified. As in the studies of Roa et al. [
24] and Lamana et al. [
42], this strategy is unusual.
In the same way as in the previous questions, it is observed that the percentage of students with a correct answer in Q3 is greater in the group that shows strategy than in the group that does not show it (
Table 6).
If we compare the resolution strategies of Q2 and Q3, it has been seen that, for the most part, the students who reach a correct answer, following the strategy in both questions, have applied the same strategy in both cases. However, there are two students who have used multiplication to find the different possible menus in Q2; however, in Q3, instead of using this operation, what they apply is the decomposition strategy into sub-problems (
Figure 6). Specifically, to the 18 combinations that they already had, they add the 9 new combinations that can be made with banana for dessert.
On the other hand, the students who had an incorrect answer in Q2 and solved it with enumeration or with the product did not repeat the strategy to solve question Q3. It was only maintained by the one who had resolved it with a sum.
Regarding the cognitive errors that lead to an incorrect answer in Q3, only two have been identified.
On the one hand, an error referring to the situation–problem posed is detected, the SPC1, which consists in the misunderstanding of statement Q3 (
Figure 7).
On the other hand, in the same way as in the previous questions, the CC1 error is explicitly observed or deduced in those answers that only show a number. Specifically, the students add a certain amount, which can be 1 or 3, to the number of possible menus they have found in Q2.
While the 1 refers exclusively to the banana dessert, the 3 is the cardinal of the dessert dishes that there would be when adding this third option.
It should be noted that this strategy has also been used in some cases in which the answer to Q2 is correct.
There are other incorrect numerical answers in which the strategy does not appear explicitly, but it is intuited that, behind it, there is an error of the type PC1, in which the student makes a mistake when multiplying and reaches 28 menus, instead of 27.
Table 7 shows the number of responses to question Q4, based on the correction degree and strategy followed, as well as Q3, given that both pose the same type of problem.
Unlike questions Q2 and Q3, which present the same combinatorial model as Q4, the strategy that has been followed the most in the correct answers to this question is the graphical. This aspect may be because the number of combinations to be made in Q4 is smaller than that of the previous ones. Consequently, they find it easier to represent the solutions with a tree diagram (
Figure 8).
In the same way as in the other combinatoric questions, the percentage of correct answers is higher in the resolutions that show strategy than in those that do not (
Table 8).
Regarding the incorrect answers of Q4, most of the students who do not show strategy conclude that there are five menus for people who do not eat meat. This amount coincides with the number of dishes that do not contain meat: broccoli, beans, omelet, fruit salad, and yogurt; therefore, it is an intuitive response error, although it seems to be motivated by a conceptual error of the type CC1.
In this line, there is an exceptional case in which the student subtracts the three dishes that do not have meat from the total number of dishes (
Figure 9).
Likewise, it should be noted that five students responded with six menus.
In two of the cases, this answer has been considered valid, since it clarifies that there are six possible menus if the banana is taken into account and 4 in case that it is not.
The other three answers, which do not show a strategy, have been categorized as incorrect, since, in Q2, they state that there are eight possible menus. This leads us to think that six comes out of listing all meatless dishes, including banana.
The PC1 error has also been identified in the incorrect answers.
Figure 10 shows a resolution in which the enumeration strategy is followed, but the student does not find all the possible combinations.
4.3. Proportional Reasoning Resolutions
Regarding question Q5, the correct answer is considered when the student calculates ¼ of the possible menus and compares, appropriately, this amount with the number of menus for people who do not eat meat, that is, with the result obtained in the Q4.
Resolutions that follow an adequate procedure, but when calculating ¼ of 18, the students do not use decimals and, therefore, lead to be in disagreement with the client because precisely the number of menus for people who do not eat meat coincides with ¼ of the total menus, which has been considered partially correct (
Figure 11), as well as answers in which the student compares the number of menus without meat, four, with ¼ of 18, but does not explicitly calculate this as 25% (
Figure 12). This category also includes resolutions in which the student has not found the correct number of menus in Q2 or Q4 but has, nevertheless, followed a correct procedure.
Finally, the answers with an incorrect procedure (
Figure 13), with irrelevant qualitative arguments, or in which the student limits himself to affirming or denying the statement, copying it practically literally, have been considered incorrect.
Table 9 shows the number of responses to question Q5, according to the strategy and degree of correction.
Surprisingly, only eight students got the question Q5 right.
Regarding the strategies that lead to a partially correct answer, they indicate that the students understand the problem; although, they may not know how to divide with decimals or apply the operator ¼.
Thus, these strategies are motivated by procedural cognitive conflicts and, specifically, by conflicts in the application of arithmetic strategies.
Something similar happens in the wrong answers. In some cases, it has been observed that the resolution strategy consists of relating ¼ to 25%, but they do not arrive at a correct answer. This means that they are still in the process of developing proportional reasoning.
4.4. Difficulty of the Problems
Mauchly’s sphericity test indicates that the assumption of sphericity does not hold (); therefore, the degrees of freedom have been corrected with the Huynh-Feldt sphericity estimate ().
The result of the repeated measures ANOVA indicates that there are statistically significant differences between the scores of the questions,.
Table 10 shows the mean scores for each question, as well as the standard deviations.
Regarding the pairwise comparisons, there are no statistically significant differences between the scores of Q1 and Q3 (9) , Q1 and Q4 scores (), and Q2 and Q4 (). However, there are significant differences between the scores of Q1 and Q2 , Q1 and Q5 , Q2 and Q3 , Q2 and Q5 , Q3 and Q4 , Q3 and Q5 , and Q4 and Q5 .
Figure 14 shows a diagram that lists the questions between which there are statistically significant differences in scores.
Thus, it is confirmed that questions Q2 and Q4 have been the easiest, while Q5 has turned out to be the most difficult.