Next Article in Journal
Development and Validation of an Artificial Intelligence-Based Motion Analysis System for Upper Extremity Rehabilitation Exercises in Patients with Spinal Cord Injury: A Randomized Controlled Trial
Next Article in Special Issue
Association between Organizational Support and Turnover Intention in Nurses: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Structural Relationships between Socio-Cognitive Mindfulness, Everyday Creativity, and Clinical Competence in Nursing Students: Mediating Effects of Everyday Creativity
Previous Article in Special Issue
Maternal Health Care Service Utilization in the Post-Conflict Democratic Republic of Congo: An Analysis of Health Inequalities over Time
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Mental Healthcare through Cognitive Emotional Regulation Strategies among Prisoners

1
Department of Psychology, Ajou University, Suwon 16499, Republic of Korea
2
BK21 FOUR R&E Center for Psychology, Korea University, Seoul 02841, Republic of Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Healthcare 2024, 12(1), 6; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12010006
Submission received: 20 November 2023 / Revised: 12 December 2023 / Accepted: 17 December 2023 / Published: 19 December 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Health and Social Care Policy)

Abstract

:
Prisoners are exposed to a deprived environment, which triggers mental illness and psychological problems. Abundant research has reported that mental illness problems, suicide, aggression, and violent behaviors occur in incarcerated people. Although the mental healthcare system for incarcerated people is emphasized, little research has been conducted due to their limited environment. In particular, the regulation of negative emotion is significantly associated with mental illness and anti-social and violent behaviors. However, mental healthcare through cognitive emotional regulation based on cognitive behavioral therapy has not been fully investigated. This study identified four different patterns in cognitive strategies for regulating negative emotions. Cognitive emotional regulation strategies (i.e., self-blame, other-blame, rumination, catastrophizing, putting into perspective, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, acceptance, and refocus on planning) were examined and addressed their vulnerable psychological factors. We analyzed a total of 500 prisoners’ responses to the cognitive emotional regulation questionnaire (CERQ) by latent class profiling analysis. A four-class model was identified based on the responses of CERQ. In addition, the significant effect of depression on classifying the four classes was found. Furthermore, differences in the average number of incarcerations were also shown across four classes. In conclusion, Class 2 (Negative Self-Blamer) uses dysfunctional/negative strategies that may place the group at a high risk of psychological disorder symptoms, including depression and post-traumatic stress. Class 3 (Distorted Positivity) uses positive/functional strategies but seems to utilize the positive strategies in distorted manners to rationalize their convictions. Class 1 (Strong Blamer) and Class 4 (Moderator Blamer) showed similar patterns focused on the “other-blame” strategy for regulating negative emotion, but they are at different levels, indicating that they attribute incarceration to external factors. These findings provide useful information for designing mental healthcare interventions for incarcerated people and psychological therapy programs for clinical and correctional psychologists in forensic settings.

1. Introduction

Sykes’s research [1] suggested that prisoners experience five types of deprivation in prison, including freedom, goods and services, sexual relations, autonomy, and security. Prisoners are socially isolated, receive poor-quality materials and services, are prohibited from contact with the opposite sex, and must passively follow the rules. The incarcerated environment can cause mental illnesses such as depression and anxiety and anti-social behaviors such as suicide, aggression, and violence. In fact, incarcerated people suffer from mental illness with maladaptive psychological characteristics such as anxiety and depression, as well as irrational beliefs and erroneous biases [2]. The Prison Policy Initiative (2022) reported that over 40% of people suffer from mental health disorders and experience subsequent depression and bipolar disorder due to the incarcerated environment [3,4]. In addition, the Prison Policy Initiative (2022) estimated that a prisoner is three times more likely to die from suicide than the general population [5].
The features of incarceration, such as disconnection from family, loss of autonomy, boredom and lack of purpose, and unpredictability of surroundings, are linked to negative mental health outcomes [6]. In addition, researchers at the University of Georgia found that people incarcerated from home were more likely to experience depression in 214 state prisons [7]. Furthermore, the number of incarcerations of offenders who were released from prison is significantly related to poor mental health in prison. Specifically, the number of incarcerations of offenders who suffer from poor mental health in prison is higher than in the general population [8,9]. However, research has also reported that prisoners have barriers to mental health treatment [10]. Mental healthcare and related professionals are rarely available in prisons. Furthermore, prisoners find it difficult to access mental healthcare in general [10].
The experience of continuous exposure to such an incarcerated environment evokes a variety of negative emotions. Mostly, negative emotions are the basis for triggering various mental illnesses and behavioral problems. Consequently, incarceration can worsen depression and last a long time after leaving prison [6]. In addition, the prison environment has various elements that cause distorted cognitive thinking. For example, Seo et al. [11] reported that murderers in prison possess distorted cognitive thinking processes, which lead to aggressive behaviors and suicidal thoughts. Although the mental healthcare system for incarcerated people is emphasized, few research studies have been conducted due to their limited environment. Previous research suggested that mental healthcare in terms of the ability to regulate negative emotions acts as a buffer for triggering various mental illnesses and behavioral problems such as suicidal behaviors, violence, depression, etc. [12,13,14,15]. Although prisoners’ psychological problems due to the prison environment have been consistently reported as a major issue, negative emotions and distorted cognitive thinking processes among prisoners, have not been fully investigated. In addition, mental healthcare intervention regarding cognitive–emotional regulation has not been fully addressed.
Emotional regulation refers to the ability to manage one’s emotional experiences and behaviors, particularly in situations of intense emotional arousal [16]. Emotion dysregulation is related to mental health, wherein negative emotions highly influence poor mental health. In addition, research found that trauma-exposed community individuals have shown a relationship between emotion dysregulation and mental health [17]. Furthermore, difficulties in regulating emotion have strong associations with the characteristics of psychopathy, including lack of empathy, impulsivity behaviors, and criminal tendencies [12,13,14,15,16].
Research has also reported that the characteristics of psychopathy are related to criminality and incarceration [16,17,18,19,20]. In a similar vein, Garofalo, Neumann, and Velotti [20] found that emotion dysregulation plays a significant mediation role in the association between psychopathy and aggressive behaviors. Preston and Anestis [21] also found that emotion dysregulation mediated the association between self-centered impulsivity traits of psychopathy and reactive aggression. In addition, Garofalo et al. [22] reported impairments in emotional regulation influence anger in both community and offender samples. Consequently, mental healthcare in forensic settings focus on correlational interventions and psychological therapy programs related to emotional regulation for reducing anti-social and aggressive behaviors [21].
Garnesfski, Kraaij, and Spinhoven [22] suggested that people choose internal or cognitive–emotional regulation methods as they grow. Therefore, cognitive strategies for regulating emotion may explain affective and cognitive mechanisms that underlie the externalizing and anti-social behaviors of adults. Garnefski, Kraaij, and Spinhoven [22] proposed a scale for CER (CERQ: Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire) in order to assess different types of cognitive strategies for regulating emotion under stressful events. Cognitive–emotional regulation (CER) refers to a cognitive process of managing emotions to deal with stressful events [22]. The CERQ measures nine types of CER strategies, including self-blame, other-blame, rumination, catastrophizing, putting into perspective, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, acceptance, and refocusing on planning. These strategies are used for managing negative emotions in response to stressful or threatening events. In more detail, self-blame involves putting the blame on oneself for what has been experienced, while other-blame involves attributing the blame to the environment or others. Rumination is defined as repeatedly focusing on thoughts about the feelings associated with a negative event. Catastrophizing is characterized by thoughts that explicitly emphasize the severity of what has been experienced. Putting it into perspective involves downplaying the seriousness of the event and emphasizing its relative significance when comparing it to other events. Positive refocusing involves thinking about joyful and pleasant issues instead of the actual negative event. Positive reappraisal involves creating a positive meaning for the event in terms of personal growth. Lastly, acceptance is the act of accepting what has been experienced. Refocus on planning involves thoughts of resigning yourself to what has happened and reorganizing and making a plan about the events.
Abundant research regarding emotional regulation has reported robust associations with mental health. In addition, a feature of depressive disorder commonly includes cognitive changes that significantly influence the individual’s function [23]. However, the identification of different types of cognitive strategies for regulating emotion under stressful situations among incarcerated people has not been fully investigated.
In addition, different patterns of cognitive strategies in emotional regulation may be related to the frequent anti-social behaviors and crimes. Therefore, the investigation regarding the association between different types of CER strategies can provide useful information for designing a mental health intervention and psychological therapy program for caring for mental illness among incarcerated people in forensic settings.

The Present Study

The main aim of this study is to identify different patterns of CER strategies among prisoners and to see how depressive disorder would impact the classifications of CER strategies. In addition, the study examines how much the different patterns can explain the number of incarcerations as the manifest variable of frequent anti-social behaviors. To achieve this, we first analyze the prisoners’ responses to CERQ, which consists of nine different emotional strategies in response to negative events. Latent class profiling analysis is conducted to identify latent patterns of CER strategies. Afterward, Wald tests are used to examine the significant differences in the average number of incarcerations among the identified classes.

2. Method

2.1. Design and Setting

A total of 521 prisoners were selected from a prison in Pusan, South Korea, which has a moderate security level (S4). The psychological interviews were conducted by four doctoral-level clinical psychologists; basic psychiatric symptoms, including depression and anxiety, were evaluated for all participants. Additionally, a simple assessment of cognitive functioning, including memory, attention, concentration, and executive functioning, was conducted prior to the survey. Participants with severe cognitive impairment were excluded from the study. All participants were informed of the purpose and anonymity of the research. Each of the five prisoners conducted the survey in a privately blocked room. A USD 25 payment was provided to each participant. Finally, the responses of 500 prisoners were analyzed. This study received approval from the Human Subjects Review Committee at Donga University (2-1040709-AB-N-01-202001-BR-003-04).

2.2. Participant

After removing missing values and careless responses, 500 samples were analyzed in this study. The participants’ basic demographic information and psychological characteristics were computed (Table 1). Their ages ranged from 16 to 75 years (Mean = 46.69, SD = 11.59). The prisoners were incarcerated for a variety of convictions, including homicide (29.8%), violent offenses (15.4%), sexual violence offenses (30.0%), drug-related crimes (4.0%), property offenses (17.2%), and others (3.6%). Regarding education levels based on their last offense, 17.2% had only completed primary school (Year 6), 25.8% completed middle school, 44.0% finished senior high school, and 13.0% completed tertiary or above education. In terms of employment, 57% of the participants had a full-time job, 29% had a part-time job, and 14% were unemployed before being incarcerated. We also examined the basic psychological characteristics of the subjects, which were measured using the PHQ9 scale (Mean = 30.64, SD = 2.67).

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaires

CER was assessed using the Korean version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ), which was validated by Ahn, Lee, and Joo [24]. The concept and CERQ were originally proposed by Garnefski and Kraaij [25]. The CERQ consists of 64 items measuring nine sub-factors of self-blame (SB), acceptance (ACP), rumination (RM), positive refocusing (PRF), positive reappraisal (PRA), putting into perspective (PIP), catastrophizing (CAT), blaming others (BO), and refocus on planning (ROP). Participants responded to each item on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Almost Never) to 5 (Almost Always). Cronbach’s α of the total CERQ was 0.87. Cronbach’s α of each sub-factor is presented on the diagonal of Table 2, along with correlation values on the off-diagonal.

2.3.2. Number of Incarcerations

The average number of incarcerations was 2.39 (Min = 1, Max = 15), as shown in Table 2.

2.4. Analysis Procedures

The analyses were conducted based on three main hypotheses in the current study. The first hypothesis posited that the responses of prisoners would be classified into different latent classes based on their cognitive–emotional regulation (CER) strategies. The second hypothesis is that depression, which is an important factor in mental health, would have an impact on the classification of the latent classes given CER strategies. Finally, the number of incarcerations would vary across the different latent classes of CER strategies. To test these hypotheses, we applied latent class profiling (LCP) analysis to the prisoners’ responses to the CERQ and tested multinomial logistic regression to see the effect of depression on the classification of latent profiles. In addition, the Wald test was conducted to compare the four latent classes in terms of the average number of incarcerations. All analyses were conducted using Mplus 8.7 [26].
The LCP assumed unobservable latent sub-populations (i.e., latent classes) underlying the observed data [27]. In this study, we assumed that the prisoners had unique response patterns on CERQ, depending on the latent class to which they belonged. The LCP was based on the three-step method to lessen estimation bias resulting from the inclusion of the distal outcome (i.e., # of incarcerations) and to take the measurement error into account [28,29]. Therefore, two research models were analyzed in separate steps: (a) participants were classified into latent CER strategy classes based on their CERQ responses, and (b) differences in average number of incarcerations were examined across the latent classes.

3. Results

3.1. Identifying Latent Classes

Following Nylund et al.’s [30] recommendation, the bootstrap likelihood ratio test (BLRT) [31] and BIC [32] were used to decide the number of latent classes. BLRT assessed the improvement in model fit between neighboring class models by adding an additional class (i.e., comparison between k − 1 and k class models). If BLRT was significant, k classes would be preferred over the k − 1 classes. In BIC, a smaller value would indicate a better-fitting model for classification. We also considered each class size (i.e., the number of individuals in the class) and entropy as indicators of the model’s fit. If a particular class had only a few assigned individuals, the model was not preferred due to interpretability issues. We evaluated the quality of classification based on entropy, counts of individuals in each class, and the meanings of the classification to avoid data-driven decision-making. The entropy, ranging from 0 to 1, would indicate a better classification as it increased, and enough class sizes are necessary for the interpretation [33]. Table 3 presents fit indices across diverse class models. Based on the above criteria, a four-class model was selected and used for further analysis. Although a five-class model had the smallest BIC and a significant BLRT, one of the classes had a small number of individuals (n = 18) contemplating the meaningful interpretation of the corresponding class [27,33]; therefore, we decided on the four-class model, considering its interpretable latent classes and adequate entropy value.
Figure 1 displays the CERQ patterns of the four-class model, indicating the sub-factor scores, while Table 4 provides the corresponding demographic information for each class. Looking at Figure 1, we found that Class 1 and Class 4 have a particular characteristic of blaming others. These two groups had similar patterns but at different levels, in which Class 1 showed the use of a strategy exhibiting strong blame towards others, while Class 4 exhibited moderate blame towards others. Class 4 had a higher score of BO than Class 1, but Class 1 had the noticeably highest score of BO among other scores. On the other hand, Class 4 reported higher scores in other functional strategies than Class 1.
Class 2 consisted of a group of negative self-blamers. They were more likely to have CER strategies involving negative perspectives and less likely to blame others compared to other classes. Moreover, Class 2 reported higher scores in terms of dysfunctional strategies than the other classes.
Lastly, Class 3 represented a group exhibiting distorted positivity group. Participants in this class showed a similar pattern to Class 2 in terms of SB, ACP, and RM, but they had higher scores in PRF, PRA, PIP, and a lower score in CAT. Furthermore, both Class 3 and Class 4 reported higher scores in PRF, PRA, and PIP than in Class 2.
Taken together, individuals in Class 1 (strong blamers toward others) generally showed lower scores across all nine sub-factors compared to the other classes, with the highest BO sub-factor scores. Class 4 (moderate blamers to others) was similar to Class 1, but its scores on all sub-factors were higher than those of Class 1. In other words, people in C4 generally utilized all strategies. The other two classes, Class 2 and Class 3 had unique patterns. Although Class 2 had higher scores in all nine strategies than Class 1, the SB, ACP, RM, and CAT scores were higher than PRF, PRA, PIP, and BO. Notably, individuals in Class 2 had the highest CAT score among all the classes, indicating a greater tendency to employ negative strategies of SB, RM, and CAT. In addition, Class 2 showed a unique pattern with larger bumps compared to the other classes. Individuals within Class 2 had the highest scores of SB, RM, and CAT, reflecting negative perspectives towards the unpleasant situation in prison; however, they have sharp drops at PRF, PRA, and PIP related to the positive perspectives and BO. Class 3 generally used all strategies, with relatively higher use of ACP, PRF, PRA, PIP, and RFP and less use of CAT and BO, which represented negative/dysfunctional strategies. Although Class 3 employed relatively more adaptive emotional strategies than the other classes, considering the situation where prisoners were convicted and incarcerated, these strategies may not be truly adaptive emotional strategies but rather distorted positivity.

3.2. Multinomial Logistic Regression Analysis

Based on the literature [6,23], we considered depression as an important predictor to differentiate CER strategies and examined the effect of depression on the classification of the four classes using a multinomial logistic regression analysis. The results are presented in Table 5. As Class 2 was set as a reference class for the analysis, the regression effect indicates the extent to which the depressive disorder categorized the individuals into a specific latent class rather than into Class 2. The depression was a significant predictor to differentiate prisoners of Class 1/Class 3 from Class 2. The odds ratios for C1 and C3 were 0.80 and 0.85, respectively, indicating the decreased odds of classification into Class 1 and Class 3 by 20% and 15% compared to Class 2, when the level of depression increased.

3.3. Number of Incarcerations Comparisons across Classes

Given the four-class model, we investigated how the number of incarcerations varied across these classes, controlling the effect of depression. Table 6 presents the average number of incarcerations for each class: C1 (2.22); C2 (1.71); C3 (2.49); and C4 (2.46). Prisoners in Class 2, which was characterized by a higher level of CAT and a lower level of BO than the other classes, reported the lowest number of incarcerations. In addition, the proportion of first-time prisoners in Class 2 (71.1%) was the highest among the four classes: C1 (44.4%); C3 (45.2%); and C4 (50.2%). We conducted Wald tests to test statistical differences in the average number of incarcerations between classes and provided the test result in Table 6. As shown in the table, Class 2 showed statistically significant differences in the number of incarcerations compared to Class 3 and Class 4. The negative estimates for the Wald test indicate that the latter class had a higher number of incarcerations than the former; therefore, Class 3 and Class 4 had a higher average number of incarcerations than Class 2.
The identified patterns of CER strategies in the current study explain the relatively lower number of incarcerations of Class 2 compared to Class 3 and Class 4. Class 3 had higher scores in PRF, PRA, and PIP but a lower score in CAT than Class 2. On the other hand, Class 4 had higher scores in PRF, PRA, PIP, and BO compared to Class 2, while they reported lower scores in SB, ACP, RM, and CAT. In conclusion, both classes commonly showed higher scores in PRF, PRA, and PIP, and a lower score in CAT. Therefore, the prisoners with more records of incarcerations reported higher scores in PRF, PRA, and PIP, and lower scores in CAT. However, the other comparisons were not statistically significant.

4. Discussion

The main aim of this study was to identify latent classes representing different patterns of CER strategies among prisoners and examine how these patterns explained incarcerations as an indicator of anti-social behaviors. Initially, we analyzed the prisoners’ responses to CERQ, which consists of nine different strategies for regulating emotion in negative and stressful situations. LCP analysis was conducted to identify latent classes regarding different patterns of CER strategies, and the effect of depression on categorizing prisoners into specific latent classes was examined. Subsequently, Wald tests were used to examine whether the number of incarcerations varied across the latent classes and to see how the different CER strategies explained the incarcerations.
The present study identified four latent classes based on the scores of CER strategies. First, Class 2, referred to as “Negative Self-blamer,” consisted of 45 prisoners, accounting for 9% of the total sample. This class was more likely to adopt negative/dysfunctional strategies (i.e., SB, RM, and CAT) for regulating emotion under stressful events. These individuals may blame themselves for incarceration, experience negative thoughts and emotions, and perceive their incarceration experiences negatively. Given the associations between negative/dysfunctional strategies and emotional dysregulation, depression, or post-traumatic stress [8,12], Class 2 may be at a high risk of psychological disorder symptoms. The current study found that Class 2 experienced a significantly lower number of incarcerations compared to Class 3 and Class 4, which reported higher scores in PRF, PRA, and PIP strategies. Previous research has indicated that anti-social and violent behaviors may occur as a means to cope with negative emotions [26]. Therefore, cautious interventions addressing psychological symptoms may help prevent suicide in prison and the likelihood of future incarceration within this group.
Second, Class 1 (n = 36) and Class 4 (n = 231) showed behavior referred to as “Moderate blamer toward others” and “Strong blamer toward others”, respectively, and showed similar patterns of CER strategies focused on BO, but at different levels. These two classes accounted for 53.4% of the total sample, indicating that a significant proportion of prisoners attribute their anti-social behaviors or incarceration to external factors. This finding aligns with previous research showing a lack of self-criticism among a considerable number of prisoners [23]. It would be helpful to challenge their attribution and elicit self-directed behavioral changes in correction programs. Considering that placing high-risk and low-risk prisoners in the same group reduced program effects [27,28], it would be more effective to implement separate interventions in terms of emotional regulation patterns. In addition, the individuals in Class 1, with higher scores in BO and CAT and lower scores in the other sub-factors, were most likely to have low motivation to participate in correctional programs voluntarily. Therefore, behavioral approaches, such as contract agreements that enforce attendance through reduced control or incentives, may be helpful in increasing therapeutic effectiveness.
Third, Class 3 (n = 188) referred to as “Distorted Positivity,” representing 37.6% of the total sample. This class used more positive/functional strategies, such as ACP, PRF, PRA, PIP, and RFP, compared to negative/dysfunctional strategies, such as CAT. Class 4 also reported a similar pattern to Class 3; both classes reported higher scores in PRF, PRA, and PIP compared to Class 1, as well as lower scores in CAT. However, Class 3 and Class 4 differed in terms of blame strategies; SB was more prevalent in Class 3, while BO was more prevalent in Class 4. Positive/functional strategies are generally helpful for psychological symptoms in the normal population. However, prisoners in Class 3 and Class 4 seemed to utilize these positive strategies in distorted manners to rationalize their convictions and crimes, given the statistically higher average number of incarcerations compared to Class 2. Although Class 3 and Class 4 experienced repeated incarcerations, they appeared to be well-adjusted in prison through their adaptive/functional emotional regulation skills.

5. Implications and Limitations

The findings of this study have several implications. First, to our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate different patterns of Cognitive–Emotional Regulation (CER) strategies among prisoners, providing insights into their intricate emotional and cognitive mechanisms for managing negative emotions. Moreover, recognizing the association between incarceration and poor mental health of offenders in prison [7], investigating patterns of emotional dysregulation as precursors to psychopathy [15,16,20,34,35], aggression [20,21], or depression [36,37] may serve as grounded evidence to develop rehabilitation intervention targeting mental health.
Second, given that the amalgamation of high-risk prisoners and low-risk prisoners in the same intervention group resulted in diminished effectiveness [38], our findings emphasize the importance of tailed interventions based on risk levels. Despite Class 2 (i.e., Negative Self-blamer) showing a lower incarceration rate compared to Class 3 and Class 4, this group is more susceptible to experiencing psychological disorder, potentially leading to suicide. One recent study suggests that self-blame is a more significant factor in predicting suicidal ideations than factors such as age, gender (male), and high perceived stress [39]. Moreover, while positive/functional strategies are typically recommended in normal populations for their psychological symptoms [40], our results indicate that Class 3, with higher use of adaptive emotional strategies, indicates repeatedly incarcerated for offenses. In addition, Class 1 indicates lower motivation to participate in correctional programs. Thus, recognizing each class suggests the necessity for different approaches intervening in each group.
Third, significantly different levels of incarceration across latent classes driven by CER strategies among prisoners highlight the importance of individual differences in CER when designing correction programs. Despite the general efficacy of cognitive–behavioral approaches in reducing criminal behaviors among offenders [41], research indicates differing responsiveness to interventions among offenders [38,42,43]. In terms of correctional intervention effectiveness, understanding how individuals regulate their emotions leading to incarceration can inform program design adjusting to specific cognitive and emotional processes. For example, improvement in terms of emotional and cognitive regulation at a different level was associated with juvenile recidivism [44]. Consequently, rehabilitation programs within prisons could benefit from personalized interventions imparting adaptive emotional regulation skills, with the aim of mitigating the risk of post-release anti-social behaviors. Thus, tailoring interventions to specific patterns regarding cognitive emotion strategies may enhance their effectiveness and reduce the risk of anti-social behaviors and recidivism.
Despite its contribution, this study has several limitations. First, the use of self-report measures may bring related bias and inaccuracy. Future research could benefit from incorporating objective measures or observational data to provide a comprehensive understanding of these strategies. Second, the sample in this study consisted of a specific group of prisoners, which limits the generalizability of the findings to other prisoners or contexts. Future studies with diverse prisoners or different contexts would strengthen and expand upon these results. Lastly, the cross-sectional study design prevents the establishment of causal relationships between CER and frequent anti-social behaviors. Further studies are needed to examine the temporal relations and predictive validity of these patterns in relation to reoffending behaviors.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and data collection: Y.C.; methodology: M.K.; writing—original draft preparation: Y.C., M.K. and J.P. All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korean government (MSIT) (NRF-2019R1F1A1061251) and BK21 FOUR R&E Center for Psychology at Korea University.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was approved on 18 March 2020 by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at Human Subjects Review Committee at Donga University (approval No. 2-1040709-AB-N-01-202001-BR-003-04).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The derived data supporting the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author on request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Sykes, G. The pains of imprisonment. In The Society of Captives: A Study of a Maximum Security Prison; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1958; pp. 63–78. [Google Scholar]
  2. Mandracchia, J.T.; Morgan, R.D.; Garos, S.; Garland, J.T. Prisoner Thinking Patterns: An Empirical Investigation. Crim. Justice Behav. 2007, 34, 1029–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Katie, R.; Alexi, J. Research Roundup: Incarceration Can Cause Lasting Damage to Mental Health; Prison Policy Initiative: Northampton, MA, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  4. Wang, L. Chronic Punishment: The Unmet Health Needs of People in State Prisons; Prison Policy Initiative: Northampton, MA, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  5. Morgan, E.R.; Rivara, F.; Ta, M.; Crossman, D.; Jones, K.; Rowhani-Rahbar, A. Incarceration and subsequent risk of suicide: A statewide cohort study. Suicide Life-Threat. Behav. 2022, 52, 467–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Stringer, H. Improving mental health for inmates. Monit. Psychol. 2019, 50, 46. Available online: https://www.apa.org/monitor/2019/03/mental-heath-inmates (accessed on 10 December 2023).
  7. Bronson, J.; Berzofsky, M. Indicators of Mental Health Problems Reported by Prisoners and Jail Inmates, 2011–2012; U.S. Department of Justics Office of Justics, Programs Bureau of Justice Statistics: Washington, DC, USA, 2017.
  8. Alexi, J.; Sawyer, W. Arrest, Release, Repeat: How Police and Jails Are Misused to Respond to Social Problems; Prison Policy Initiative: Northampton, MA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  9. Wallace, D.; Wang, X. Does in-prison physical and mental health impact recidivism? SSM Pop. Health 2020, 11, 100569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Reingle Gonzalez, J.M.; Connell, N.M. Mental health of prisoners: Identifying barriers to mental health treatment and medication continuity. Am. J. Public Health 2014, 104, 2328–2333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Seo, J.H.; Kim, K.; Lee, K.; Kim, G. A study on linguistic and psychological characteristics and personality profiling in the writings of murders. J. Korean Data Anal. Soc. 2012, 14, 1355–1371. [Google Scholar]
  12. Hare, R.D. Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, 2nd ed.; Multi-Health Systems: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hare, R.D.; Neumann, C.S. The PCL-R assessment of psychopathy: Development, structural properties, and new directions. In Handbook of Psychopathy; Patrick, C.J., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 58–88. [Google Scholar]
  14. Williams, K.M.; Paulhus, D.L.; Hare, R.D. Capturing the four-factor structure of psychopathy in college students via self-report. J. Pers. Assess. 2007, 88, 205–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Hare, R.D. Psychopathy, affect and behavior. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research and Implications for Society; Cooke, D.J., Forth, A.E., Hare, R.D., Eds.; Kluwer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1998; pp. 105–137. [Google Scholar]
  16. Garofalo, C.; Neumann, C.S. Psychopathy and emotion regulation: Taking stock and moving forward. In Routledge International Handbook of Psychopathy and Crime; DeLisi, M., Ed.; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2018; pp. 58–79. [Google Scholar]
  17. Coid, J.; Yang, M.; Ullrich, S.; Roberts, A.; Hare, R.D. Prevalence and correlates of psychopathic traits in the household population of Great Britain. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 2009, 32, 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Hare, R.D.; Neumann, C.S. Psychopathy: Assessment and forensic implications. Can. J. Psychiatry 2009, 54, 791–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Olver, M.E.; Neumann, C.S.; Wong, S.C.P.; Hare, R.D. The structural and predictive properties of the Psychopathy Checklist–Revised in Canadian Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal offenders. Psychol. Assess. 2013, 25, 167–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Garofalo, C.; Neumann, C.S.; Velotti, P. Psychopathy and aggression: The role of emotion dysregulation. J. Interpers. Violence 2021, 36, NP12640–NP12664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Preston, O.C.; Anestis, J.C. The indirect relationships between psychopathic traits and proactive and reactive aggression through empathy and emotion dysregulation. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2020, 42, 409–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Garnefski, N.; Kraaij, V.; Spinhoven, P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2001, 30, 1311–1327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Weiss, N.H.; Forkus, S.R.; Contractor, A.A.; Dixon-Gordon, K.L. The interplay of negative and positive emotion dysregulation on mental health outcomes among trauma-exposed community individuals. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 2020, 12, 219–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Ahn, H.N.; Lee, N.; Joo, H. Validation of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire in a Korean population. Korean J. Couns. 2013, 14, 1773–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Garnefski, N.; Kraaij, V. The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2007, 23, 141–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O. Mplus User’s Guide, 8th ed.; Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  27. Lubke, G.H.; Muthén, B. Investigating population heterogeneity with factor mixture models. Psychol. Methods 2005, 10, 21–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Vermunt, J.K. Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Political Anal. 2017, 18, 450–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Wang, J.; Wang, X. Structural Equation Modeling: Applications Using Mplus, 2nd ed.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  30. Nylund, K.L.; Asparouhov, T.; Muthén, B.O. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct. Equ. Model. 2007, 14, 535–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. McLachlan, G.J.; Peel, D. Finite Mixture Models; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  32. Schwartz, G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 1978, 6, 461–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Jung, S.; Shin, M.; Lee, Y.R. An application of latent class analysis to classifying change trajectory of school achievement improvement of general high school and testing determinants of the classification. J. Educ. Eval. Res. 2015, 28, 1277–1299. [Google Scholar]
  34. Garnefski, N.; Kraaij, V. Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: A comparative study of five specific samples. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2006, 40, 1659–1669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Zhou, J.; Witt, K.; Cao, X.; Chen, C.; Wang, X. Predicting reoffending using the Structured Assessment of Violence Risk in Youth (SAVRY): A 5-year follow-up study of male juvenile offenders in Hunan Province, China. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0169251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Ehring, T.; Tuschen-Caffier, B.; Schnulle, J.; Fisher, S.; Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: Spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion 2010, 10, 563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Walker, S.; Worrall, A. Life as a woman: The gendered pains of indeterminate imprisonment. Prison. Serv. J. 2000, 132, 27–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lipsey, M.W.; Cullen, F.T. The effectiveness of correctional rehabilitation: A review of systematic reviews. Annu. Rev. Law Soc. Sci. 2007, 3, 297–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Okeke, A.O.; Obiora, A.; Ezeokaana, J.O.; Abamara, N.C. Age, gender, stress and self-blame as predictors of suicidal-ideation among prison inmates in South East Nigeria. Soc. Sci. Res. 2022, 6, 1. [Google Scholar]
  40. Carl, J.R.; Soskin, D.P.; Kerns, C.; Barlow, D.H. Positive emotion regulation in emotional disorders: A theoretical review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2013, 33, 343–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Pearson, F.S.; Lipton, D.S.; Cleland, C.M.; Yee, D.S. The effects of behavioral/cognitive-behavioral programs on recidivism. Crime Delinq. 2022, 48, 476–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Antonowicz, D.H.; Ross, R.R. Essential components of successful rehabilitation programs for offenders. Int. J. Offender. Ther. Comp. Criminol. 1994, 38, 97–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gendreau, P. Offender rehabilitation: What we know and what needs to be done. Crim. Justice Behav. 1996, 23, 144–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Docherty, M.; Lieman, A.; Gorden, B.L. Improvement in emotion regulation while detained predicts lower juvenile recidivism. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2021, 20, 164–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Cognitive emotion regulation scores across four classes. Note: SB—Self-blame; ACP—Acceptance; RM—Rumination; PRF—Positive refocusing; PRA—Positive reappraisal; PIP—Putting into perspectives; CAT—Catastrophizing; BO—Blaming others; RFP—Refocus on planning.
Figure 1. Cognitive emotion regulation scores across four classes. Note: SB—Self-blame; ACP—Acceptance; RM—Rumination; PRF—Positive refocusing; PRA—Positive reappraisal; PIP—Putting into perspectives; CAT—Catastrophizing; BO—Blaming others; RFP—Refocus on planning.
Healthcare 12 00006 g001
Table 1. Demographic information and psychological characteristics of the participants.
Table 1. Demographic information and psychological characteristics of the participants.
Demographic InformationCategoriesN%
CriminalsHomicide14929.8
Violent Offenses7715.4
Sexual Violence Offenses15030.0
Property Offenses8617.2
Drug-Related Crimes204.0
Others183.6
Years of EducationBelow Primary School122.4
Primary School7314.6
Middle School13326.6
High School22144.2
Above College6012
Job StatusFull-time Jobs28557.0
Part-time Jobs14529.0
Unemployed7014.0
Total 500
MeanSD
Age 46.6911.59
# of incarcerations 2.392.20
DepressionPHQ930.642.68
Table 2. Correlations, mean, and standard deviation of cognitive emotion regulation strategies.
Table 2. Correlations, mean, and standard deviation of cognitive emotion regulation strategies.
CER Strategy123456789
Self-blame0.80
Acceptance0.55 **0.64
Rumination0.47 **0.41 **0.66
Positive refocusing0.28 **0.33 **0.19 **0.75
Positive reappraisal0.26 **0.47 **0.29 **0.48 **0.74
Putting into perspectives0.23 **0.40 **0.23 **0.38 **0.64 **0.59
Catastrophizing0.41 **0.19 **0.58 **−0.02−0.05−0.080.78
Blaming others−0.27 **−0.10 *0.11 *−0.060.09 *0.13 **0.17 **0.74
Refocus on planning0.32 **0.45 **0.36 *0.40 **0.58 **0.43 **0.030.050.77
Mean13.8011.0713.1013.7413.9913.7510.769.7715.87
SD3.782.553.203.473.553.043.993.403.05
Note. CER—cognitive emotional regulation; diagonal elements indicate Cronbach’s α; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05.
Table 3. Fit indices for identifying latent classes.
Table 3. Fit indices for identifying latent classes.
Fit Index2 Classes3 Classes4 Classes5 Classes
BIC12319.2712100.9011937.3211865.87
BLRT625.19 **280.52 **225.72 **133.59 **
Class sizes143/35722/242/23636/45/188/23118/105/224/47/106
Entropy0.780.810.840.81
Note. ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Demographic information and psychological characteristics across classes.
Table 4. Demographic information and psychological characteristics across classes.
Demographic InformationCategoriesClass 1Class 2Class 3Class 4
Strong BlamerNegative Self-BlamerDistorted PositivityModerate Blamer
Frequency (%)
CriminalsHomicide11 (30.6)23 (51.1)51 (27.1)64 (27.7)
Violent Offenses4 (11.1)6 (13.3)33 (17.6)34 (14.7)
Sexual Violence Offenses16 (44.4)12 (26.7)42 (22.3)80 (34.6)
Property Offenses3 (8.3)2 (4.4)38 (20.2)43 (18.6)
Drug-Related Crimes-1 (2.2)14 (7.4)5 (2.2)
Others2 (5.6)1 (2.2)10 (5.3)5 (2.2)
Educational YearsBelow Primary School-1 (2.2)7 (3.7)4 (1.7)
Primary School8 (22.2)9 (20.0)23 (12.2)33 (14.3)
Middle School9 (25.0)11 (24.4)37 (19.7)76 (32.9)
High School17 (47.2)19 (42.2)93 (49.5)92 (39.8)
Above College2 (5.6)5 (11.1)27 (14.4)26 (11.3)
Job StatusFull-time Jobs21 (58.3)28 (62.2)111 (59.0)125 (54.1)
Part-time Jobs9 (25.0)9 (20.0)53 (28.2)67 (29.0)
No Jobs6 (16.7)8 (17.8)24 (12.8)39 (16.9)
Total 3645188231
Mean (SD)
Age 43.32 (12.46)51.16 (11.30)47.74 (10.46)45.44 (12.12)
# of recidivism 2.22 (1.82)1.71 (1.58)2.49 (2.28)2.46 (2.27)
DepressionPHQ929.94 (2.62)31.49 (2.46)30.35 (2.63)30.83 (2.72)
AnxietySTAI-S37.75 (6.59)40.56 (6.53)42.97 (6.04)40.37 (6.87)
STAI-T38.17 (7.63)40.11 (6.03)43.95 (5.70)41.12 (6.38)
Table 5. Multinomial logistic regression with depression.
Table 5. Multinomial logistic regression with depression.
Class ComparisonEstimateS.E.p-ValueOdds Ratio
Depression → C1−0.220.090.010.80
Depression → C3−0.160.060.010.85
Depression → C4−0.080.060.100.91
Note. Reference class = C2; bold indicates significant test statistics.
Table 6. Wald tests across latent classes for the number of incarcerations.
Table 6. Wald tests across latent classes for the number of incarcerations.
Class ComparisonEstimateS.E.p-Value
C1 vs. C20.500.380.18
C1 vs. C3−0.270.340.43
C1 vs. C4−0.240.340.48
C2 vs. C3−0.780.290.01
C2 vs. C4−0.740.280.01
C3 vs. C40.030.230.88
Note. Bold indicates significant test statistics.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Choi, Y.; Kim, M.; Park, J. Mental Healthcare through Cognitive Emotional Regulation Strategies among Prisoners. Healthcare 2024, 12, 6. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12010006

AMA Style

Choi Y, Kim M, Park J. Mental Healthcare through Cognitive Emotional Regulation Strategies among Prisoners. Healthcare. 2024; 12(1):6. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12010006

Chicago/Turabian Style

Choi, Younyoung, Mirim Kim, and Jeongsoo Park. 2024. "Mental Healthcare through Cognitive Emotional Regulation Strategies among Prisoners" Healthcare 12, no. 1: 6. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12010006

APA Style

Choi, Y., Kim, M., & Park, J. (2024). Mental Healthcare through Cognitive Emotional Regulation Strategies among Prisoners. Healthcare, 12(1), 6. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12010006

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop