The Impact of Working Memory on the Development of Social Play in Japanese Preschool Children: Emotion Knowledge as a Mediator
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Working memory is positively correlated with emotion recognition and emotion situation knowledge.
- Emotion situation knowledge is more sophisticated than emotion recognition and, thus, is more strongly associated with social play.
- Emotion knowledge mediates the relationship between working memory and social play.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Social Play
2.3.2. Emotion Knowledge
2.3.3. Working Memory
2.3.4. Verbal Ability
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
3.2. Mediational Analysis
Social Play and Working Memory
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- White, R.E.; Carlson, S.M. Pretending with realistic and fantastical stories facilitates executive function in 3-year-old children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2021, 207, 105090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bauer, R.H.; Gilpin, A.T. Imaginative children in the classroom: Mixed-methods examining teacher reported behavior, play observations and child assessments. Early Edu. Dev. 2022, 34, 449–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colliver, Y.; Harrison, L.J.; Brown, J.E.; Humburg, P. Free play predicts self-regulation years later: Longitudinal evidence from a large Australian sample of toddlers and preschoolers. Early Child. Res. Q. 2022, 59, 148–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, M.M.; Thibodeau, R.B.; Pierucci, J.M.; Gilpin, A.T. Supporting the development of empathy: The role of theory of mind and fantasy orientation. Soc. Dev. 2017, 26, 951–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalkusch, I.; Jaggy, A.K.; Bossi, C.B.; Weiss, B.; Sticca, F.; Perren, S. Fostering children’s social pretend play competence and social skills through play tutoring: What is the mechanism of change. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2022, 46, 461–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coolahan, K.; Fantuzzo, J.; Mendez, J.; McDermott, P. Preschool peer interactions and readiness to learn: Relationships between classroom peer play and learning behaviors and conduct. J. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 92, 458–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ioannou, S.; Key, A.P.; Muscatello, R.A.; Klemencic, M.; Corbett, B.A. Peer actors and theater techniques play pivotal roles in improving social play and anxiety for children with autism. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weiss, E.M.; McDermott, P.A.; Rovine, M.J.; Oh, J. Latent growth trajectories of peer context behavior problems across preschool, kindergarten and first grade. Early Educ. Dev. 2022, 33, 939–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, J.A. Contributions of teacher-child relationships to positive school adjustment during elementary school. J. Sch. Psychol. 2006, 44, 211–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bassett, H.H.; Denham, S.; Mincic, M.; Graling, K. The structure of preschoolers’ emotion knowledge: Model equivalence and validity using a structural equation modeling approach. Early Educ. Dev. 2012, 23, 259–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buhs, E.S.; Ladd, G.W. Peer rejection as an antecedent of young children’s school adjustment: An examination of mediating processes. Dev. Psychol. 2001, 37, 550–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Denham, S.A.; Bassett, H.H.; Zinsser, K.; Wyatt, T.M. How preschoolers’ social-emotional learning predicts their early school success: Developing theory-promoting, competency-based assessments. Infant Child Dev. 2014, 23, 426–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeRosier, M.E.; Lloyd, S.W. The impact of children’s social adjustment on academic outcomes. Read. Writ. Q. 2011, 27, 25–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bauer, R.H.; Gilpin, A.T.; Thibodeau-Nielsen, R.B. Executive functions and imaginative play: Exploring relations with prosocial behaviors using structural equation modeling. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2021, 25, 100165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farrell, C.B.; Gilpin, A.T. Longitudinal bidirectionality of emotion knowledge and inhibitory control in low-income children using cross-lagged panels. Soc. Dev. 2021, 30, 1006–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fisher, K.R.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Gryfe, S.G. Conceptual split? Parents’ and experts’ perceptions of play in the 21st century. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 29, 305–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Howes, C.; Matheson, C.C. Sequences in the development of competent play with peers: Social and social pretend play. Dev. Psychol. 1992, 28, 961–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bailey, J.; Bulotsky-Shearer, R.J. Learning behaviors mediate associations between parent-reported peer play skills and literacy and mathematics skills for low-income preschool children. Early Educ. Dev. 2022, 33, 523–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thibodeau-Nielsen, R.B.; Gilpin, A.T.; Palermo, F.; Nancarrow, A.F.; Farrell, C.B.; Turley, D.; DeCaro, J.A.; Lochman, J.E.; Boxmeyer, C.L. Pretend play as a protective factor for developing executive functions among children living in poverty. Cogn. Dev. 2020, 56, 100964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, R.E.; Thibodeau-Nielsen, R.B.; Palermo, F.; Mikulski, A.M. Engagement in social pretend play predicts preschoolers’ executive function gains across the school year. Early Child. Res. Q. 2021, 56, 103–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hashmi, S.; Vanderwert, R.E.; Price, H.A.; Gerson, S.A. Exploring the Benefits of Doll Play Through Neuroscience. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 560176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hashmi, S.; Vanderwert, R.E.; Paine, A.L.; Gerson, S.A. Doll play prompts social thinking and social talking: Representations of internal state language in the brain. Dev. Sci. 2022, 25, e13163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goldstein, T.R.; Lerner, M.D. Dramatic pretend play games uniquely improve emotional control in young children. Dev. Sci. 2018, 21, e12603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ladd, G.W. Effectiveness of a social learning method for enhancing children’s social interaction and peer acceptance. Child Dev. 1981, 52, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mize, J.; Ladd, G.W. A cognitive-social learning approach to social skill training with low-status preschool children. Dev. Psychol. 1990, 26, 388–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gresham, F.M. Utility of cognitive-behavioral procedures for social skills training with children: A critical review. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 1985, 13, 411–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parker, J.G.; Low, C.M.; Walker, A.R.; Gamm, B.K. Friendship jealousy in young adolescents: Individual differences and links to sex, self-esteem, aggression, and social adjustment. Dev. Psychol. 2005, 41, 235–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubin, K.H.; Coplan, R.J.; Bowker, J.C. Social withdrawal in childhood. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 141–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gibb, R.; Coelho, L.; Van Rootselaar, N.A.; Halliwell, C.; MacKinnon, M.; Plomp, L.; Gonzalez, C. Promoting executive function skills in preschoolers using a play-based program. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 720225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moriguchi, Y.; Hiraki, K. Prefrontal cortex and executive function in young children: A review of NIRS studies. Front. Human Neurosci. 2013, 7, 867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Romero-Lopez, M.; Pichardo, M.C.; Justicia-Arraez, A.; Bembibre-Serrano, J. Reducing Aggression by Developing Emotional and Inhibitory Control. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2021, 18, 5263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moriguchi, Y. The early development of executive function and its relation to social interaction: A brief review. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Howard, S.J.; Vasseleu, E.; Neilsen-Hewett, C.; de Rosnay, M.; Chan, A.Y.C.; Johnstone, S.; Mavilidi, M.; Paas, F.; Melhuish, E.C. Executive function and self-regulation: Bi-directional longitudinal associations and prediction of early academic skills. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 733328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Austin, G.; Groppe, K.; Elsner, B. The reciprocal relationship between executive function and theory of mind in middle childhood: A 1-year longitudinal perspective. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Shahaeian, A.; Haynes, M.; Frick, P.J. The role of language in the association between theory of mind and executive functioning in early childhood: New longitudinal evidence. Early Child. Res. Q. 2023, 62, 251–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fan, L.H.; Qing, W.J.; Wang, Y.L.; Zhan, M.C. Family socioeconomic status and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool children: The mediating role of executive function. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2022, 19, 11608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moriguchi, Y.; Chevalier, N.; Zelazo, P.D. Editorial: Development of executive function during childhood. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wagar, T. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Morra, S.; Panesi, S.; Traverso, L.; Usai, M.C. Which tasks measure what? Reflections on executive function development and a commentary on Podjarny, Kamawar, and Andrews (2017). J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 167, 246–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chalmers, K.A.; Freeman, E.E. A comparison of single and multi-test working memory assessments in predicting academic achievement in children. J. Psychol. Interdiscip. Appl. 2018, 152, 613–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chalmers, K.A.; Freeman, E.E. Working memory power test for children. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2019, 37, 105–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gray, S.; Green, S.; Alt, M.; Hogan, T.; Kuo, T.; Brinkley, S.; Cowan, N. The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence. J. Mem. Lang. 2017, 92, 183–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Mutter, B.; Alcorn, M.B.; Welsh, M. Theory of mind and executive function: Working memory capacity and inhibitory control as predictors of false-belief task performance. Percept. Mot. Skills 2006, 102, 819–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morra, S.; Parrella, I.; Camba, R. The role of working memory in the development of emotion comprehension. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 29, 744–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thibodeau-Nielsen, R.B.; Gilpin, A.T.; Brown, M.M.; Meyer, B.A. The effects of fantastical pretend-play on the development of executive functions: An intervention study. J. Exp. Psychol. 2016, 145, 120–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. Working memory and emotion: Ruminations on a theory of depression. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2013, 17, 20–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Q.; Wang, Q.; Xin, Z.; Gu, H. The impact of gross motor skills on the development of emotion understanding in children aged 3-6 years: The mediation role of executive functions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 22, 14807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pons, F.; Harris, P.L.; Doudin, P.A. Teaching emotion understanding. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2002, 17, 293–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deneault, J.; Ricard, M. Are emotion and mind understanding differently linked to young children’s social adjustment? Relationships between behavioral consequences of emotions, false belief, and SCBE. J. Genet. Psychol. 2013, 174, 88–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Widen, S.C.; Russell, J.A. Children acquire emotion categories gradually. Cogn. Dev. 2008, 23, 291–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sette, S.; Spinrad, T.L.; Baumgartner, E. The relations of preschool children’s emotion knowledge and socially appropriate behaviors to peer likability. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2017, 41, 532–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Widen, S.C.; Russell, J.A. A closer look at preschoolers’ freely produced labels for facial expressions. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 39, 114–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fine, S.E.; Izard, C.E.; Trentacosta, C.J. Emotion situation knowledge in elementary school: Models of longitudinal growth and preschool correlates. Soc. Dev. 2006, 15, 730–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heinze, J.E.; Miller, A.L.; Seifer, R.; Locke, R. Emotion knowledge, loneliness, negative social experiences, and internalizing symptoms among low-income preschoolers. Soc. Dev. 2015, 24, 240–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Denham, S.A.; Couchoud, E.A. Young preschoolers understanding of emotions. Child Study J. 1990, 20, 171–192. [Google Scholar]
- Denham, S.A.; McKinley, M.; Couchoud, E.A.; Holt, R. Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Dev. 1990, 61, 1145–1152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Widen, S.C.; Russell, J.A. Differentiation in preschooler’s categories of emotion. Emotion 2010, 10, 651–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gal-Szabo, D.E.; Spinrad, T.L.; Eisenberg, N.; Sulik, M.J. The relations of children’s emotion knowledge to their observed social play and reticent/uninvolved behavior in preschool: Moderation by effortful control. Soc. Dev. 2017, 28, 57–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howes, C.; Unger, O.; Seidner, L.B. Social pretend play in toddlers: Parallels with social play and with solitary pretend. Child Dev. 1989, 60, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ursache, A.; Dawson-McClure, S.; Siegel, J.; Brotman, L.M. Predicting early emotion knowledge development among children of color living in historically disinvested neighborhoods: Consideration of child pre-academic abilities, self-regulation, peer relations and parental education. Cogn. Emot. 2019, 33, 1562–1576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lucas-Molina, B.; Quintanilla, L.; Sarmento-Henrique, R.; Babarro, J.M.; Gimenez-Das, M. The relationship between emotion regulation and emotion knowledge in preschoolers: A longitudinal study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rademacher, A.; Goagoses, N.; Schmidt, S.; Zumbach, J.; Koglin, U. Preschoolers’ profiles of self-regulation, socialemotional and behavior skills and its prediction for a successful behavior adaptation during the transitional period from preschool to elementary school. Early Educ. Dev. 2021, 33, 1137–1151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watanabe, N.; Denham, S.A.; Jones, N.M.; Kobayashi, T.; Bassett, H.H.; Ferrier, D.E. Working toward cross-cultural adaptation: Preliminary psychometric evaluation of the Affect Knowledge Test in Japanese preschoolers. SAGE Open. 2019, 9, 2158244019846688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zahn-Waxler, C.; Friedman, R.J.; Cole, P.M.; Mizuta, I.; Hiruma, N. Japanese and United States preschool children’s responses to conflict and distress. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 2462–2477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nakamura, M.; Buck, R.; Kenny, D.A. Relative contributions of expressive behavior and contextual information to the judgment of the emotional state of another. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 59, 1032–1039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coplan, R.J.; Rubin, K.H. Exploring and assessing nonsocial play in the preschool: The development and validation of the preschool play behavior scale. Soc. Dev. 1998, 7, 72–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubin, K.H. The Play Observation Scale (POS); University of Waterloo: Waterloo, ON, Canada, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Denham, S.A.; Caverly, S.; Schmidt, M.; Blair, K.; DeMulder, E.; Caal, S.; Hamada, H.; Mason, T. Preschool understanding of emotions: Contributions to classroom anger and aggression. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2014, 43, 901–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denham, S.A. Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Dev. 1986, 57, 194–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borke, H. Interpersonal perception of young children: Egocentrism or empathy? Dev. Psychol. 1971, 5, 263–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlson, S.M.; Moses, L.J.; Breton, C. How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant Child Dev. 2002, 11, 73–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ueno, K.; Nagoshi, N.; Konuki, S. PVT-R Manual; Nihon Bunka Kagakusha: Tokyo, Japan, 2008. (In Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, R.M.; Kenny, D.A. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 1173–1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nook, E.C.; Sasse, S.F.; Lambert, H.K.; McLaughlin, K.A.; Somerville, L.H. Increasing verbal knowledge mediates development of multidimensional emotion representations. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2017, 1, 881–889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Masuda, T.; Ellsworth, P.C.; Mesquita, B.; Leu, J.; Tanida, S.; Van de Veerdonk, E. Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 94, 365–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kitayama, S. Culture and emotion. In International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences; Smelser, N.J., Baltes, P.B., Eds.; Elsevier Science: Oxford, UK, 2001; pp. 3134–3139. [Google Scholar]
- Trommsdorff, G.; Heikamp, T. Socialization of emotions and emotion regulation in cultural context. In Cultural Variations in Psychopathology: From Research to Practice; Barnow, S., Balkir, N., Eds.; Hogrefe: Cambridge, MA, 2013; pp. 67–92. [Google Scholar]
- Kosokabe, T.; Mizusaki, M.; Nagaoka, W.; Honda, M.; Suzuki, N.; Naoi, R.; Moriguchi, Y. Self-directed dramatic and music play programs enhance executive function in Japanese children. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2021, 24, 100158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Q.; Liu, P.; Yan, N.; Feng, T. Executive function training improves emotional competence for preschool children: The roles of inhibition control and working memory. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tang, Y.; Harris, P.; Zou, H.; Wang, J.; Zhang, Z. The relationship between emotion understanding and social skills in preschoolers: The mediating role of verbal ability and the moderating role of working memory. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 18, 593–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brock, L.L.; Kim, H.; Kelly, C.L.; Mashburn, A.J.; Grissmer, D.W. Theory of mind, directly and indirectly, facilitates kindergarten adjustment via verbal ability, executive function, and emotion knowledge. Psychol. Sch. 2019, 56, 176–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Measure | n | M | SD | Range |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 47 | 61.89 | 6.00 | 51.0–72.0 |
Verbal ability | 47 | 19.91 | 9.37 | 5.0–44.0 |
Working memory | 47 | 2.53 | 0.69 | 1.0–4.0 |
Emotion recognition | 47 | 2.19 | 0.88 | 0.0–4.0 |
Situation knowledge | 47 | 3.53 | 0.72 | 2.0–4.0 |
Social play | 47 | 0.40 | 0.25 | 0.00–0.90 |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Gender | - | |||||
2. Age | −0.25 | - | ||||
3. Verbal ability | 0.02 | 0.51 ** | - | |||
4. Working memory | 0.21 | 0.16 | 0.42 ** | - | ||
5. Emotion recognition | −0.06 | 0.25 | 0.28 * | 0.37 * | - | |
6. Situation knowledge | −0.04 | 0.22 | −0.03 | −0.06 | 0.25 † | - |
7. Social play | −0.08 | 0.40 ** | 0.28 * | 0.40 ** | 0.66 ** | 0.41 ** |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Shimizu, H. The Impact of Working Memory on the Development of Social Play in Japanese Preschool Children: Emotion Knowledge as a Mediator. Children 2023, 10, 524. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10030524
Shimizu H. The Impact of Working Memory on the Development of Social Play in Japanese Preschool Children: Emotion Knowledge as a Mediator. Children. 2023; 10(3):524. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10030524
Chicago/Turabian StyleShimizu, Hisayo. 2023. "The Impact of Working Memory on the Development of Social Play in Japanese Preschool Children: Emotion Knowledge as a Mediator" Children 10, no. 3: 524. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10030524
APA StyleShimizu, H. (2023). The Impact of Working Memory on the Development of Social Play in Japanese Preschool Children: Emotion Knowledge as a Mediator. Children, 10(3), 524. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10030524