Next Article in Journal
Incidence of and Risk Factors for Subsequent Lower Respiratory Tract Infection Following an Infant RSV Hospitalization
Previous Article in Journal
Health Preferences in Transition: Differences from Pandemic to Post-Pandemic in Valuation of COVID-19 and RSV Illness in Children and Adults
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Case Report

Effectiveness of Intensive Linguistic Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Case Study

by
Esther Moraleda-Sepulveda
*,
Noelia Pulido-García
,
Nadia Loro-Vicente
and
Noelia Santos-Muriel
Faculty of Psychology, University Complutense of Madrid Campus de Somosaguas, 28223 Pozuelo de Alarcón, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2025, 12(2), 182; https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020182
Submission received: 17 December 2024 / Revised: 20 January 2025 / Accepted: 24 January 2025 / Published: 31 January 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Neurology & Neurodevelopmental Disorders)

Abstract

:
Background: Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is currently classified as a neurodevelopmental disorder with increasing prevalence year by year. One of the key characteristics of this population is the persistent and variable difficulty they present in the development of functional language. For this reason, most individuals with ASD are candidates for linguistic treatment, especially during the early stages of development. The aim of this study was to assess the effectiveness of an individualized and intensive oral language and communication intervention. Method: This research was conducted through a case study of a 5-year-old Spanish-speaking child diagnosed with ASD. The child’s family sought intensive speech therapy to address articulation difficulties that were affecting speech intelligibility. However, a linguistic intervention program was proposed that would cover work in all areas of language. A comprehensive assessment of the child’s language and communication skills was carried out by a team of five professionals. Following this, an individualized intervention was implemented for 27 h per week over a period of 4 weeks. After this period, the child’s linguistic skills were reassessed. Results: The results show that the proposed intervention not only improved articulation skills. Conclusion: It is important to conduct a detailed analysis of each case and design individualized interventions that directly impact the effectiveness of treatments.

1. Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that manifests in a variable range of alterations, primarily in social interaction, which in turn affects communication and language [1,2,3]. It develops at an early age, and failure to diagnose it early can impact an individual’s life for the long term [4]. Historically, ASD has been considered a disorder predominantly affecting the male population [5], with a prevalence four times higher in males than in females [3,6]. However, current studies suggest that this may be due to underdiagnosis in females [7].
Regardless of gender, it can be stated that individuals with ASD present communication difficulties, and some authors argue that a significant percentage of them never develop functional language [8,9,10]. A great deal of heterogeneity has also been documented regarding linguistic and communicative abilities in individuals with ASD, resulting in a spectrum ranging from the complete absence of conventional communicative behaviors to the use of complex structures, both in form and function [11,12,13]. Additionally, language difficulties appear at early ages and can affect all components of language [14].
Regarding the preverbal stage of language, it has been suggested that children with ASD are capable of requesting objects, actions, and social routines, persisting until they achieve their goal [15]. However, they seem to perform these communicative functions through protoimperative gestures, such as offering, showing, or pointing [16]. This has led some authors to suggest a dissociation between protoimperative and protodeclarative gestures [17], considering it an early sign of impaired social communication in children with ASD [18]. On the other hand, some authors have hypothesized that individuals with ASD show alterations in perceiving communicative acts, which could explain difficulties in responding to their own name [19,20]. Miller et al. [21] found that around 52% of children with ASD did not respond to their name at 12 months. It has also been documented that some individuals with ASD lack eye contact or make it in an atypical manner, leading to deficits in establishing joint attention later on and making the use of gestures as a primary form of communication difficult [22,23,24].
In relation to the phonetic–phonological area of language development, the speech of children with ASD is characterized by poor vocalization, which makes it difficult to understand, especially for individuals unfamiliar with their way of speaking [25,26,27]. In this regard, authors such as Cleland et al. [28] and Shriberg et al. [29] reported that approximately 41% of verbal children with ASD present phonological simplification processes, such as the absence of fricative consonants, affricates, velars, and liquids, and distortion or inconsistent use of nasals and alveolars [30].
In terms of morphosyntactic components, it is important to highlight the difficulties individuals with ASD face in understanding and using object pronouns, relative pronouns, and reflexive pronouns [31,32,33]. Moreover, their speech often includes prohibitive phrases, with negative or defiant statements [34]. The lack of logical sentence structure is another characteristic of their language, leading to a greater predominance of the present tense and difficulty in using and understanding verb tenses [35,36]. However, perhaps the most distinctive morphological feature is the use of pronominal inversion, understood as the use of the second or third person singular instead of the first, and using one’s own name to refer to oneself [37,38,39].
Regarding the semantic aspect of language, individuals with ASD are characterized by a discrepancy in expressive and receptive vocabulary scores, with expressive vocabulary scores being higher than receptive vocabulary scores [40,41]. Terms directed at social and emotional use are scarce, and they produce few statements aimed at conversation [42]. In addition to deficits in standardized vocabulary scores, children with ASD present unique deficits in specific subdomains of vocabulary, including the production and comprehension of personal pronouns, mental state terms, and prepositions [43]. They also face difficulties in understanding polysemy, as they struggle to understand why a word has multiple meanings [34].
Pragmatic language is undoubtedly the most altered aspect in individuals with ASD [44]. Among the most documented difficulties, it is important to highlight the use of echolalia and the limited creative language [45,46,47]. Echolalia refers to the repetition of decontextualized utterances made by individuals with ASD [44], which serve various communicative functions and regulate the nervous system [44,46]. Other authors have also reported difficulties in understanding indirect speech acts, metaphors, and jokes [48,49,50]. Additionally, they tend to ask repetitive or inappropriate questions [51,52,53], as well as engage in monolithic discourse and storytelling due to the cognitive inflexibility characteristic of this population [54].
In summary, it is important to consider not only the direct communication of individuals with ASD, but also ensure that different interlocutors are able to communicate with them and respond to their communicative intentions [10,55,56]. Therefore, communication and language should be approached as natural processes through more conscious interactions, promoting these processes as fundamental aspects for the development and learning of individuals with ASD [57,58,59]. For this reason, the objective of this work is to evaluate the effectiveness of individualized and intensive linguistic intervention in a child with ASD, taking into account their needs, interests and particularities and taking into account all areas of language and communication together [60,61,62].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Case Description

X.G. is a 5-year-old male, born in 2019, previously diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). He sought speech therapy in July 2024 for intensive therapy to improve his linguistic skills, particularly in the phonetic–phonological area and speech intelligibility.
An initial interview with the parents was conducted, during which they provided all existing reports to date. There were no abnormalities observed during the pregnancy. The pregnancy was full-term, and delivery was via cesarean section due to the baby’s position. He was breastfed until 8 months.
Motor development progressed normally. He sat up at 6 months, began crawling at 9 months, and walked independently at 12 months. His parents were concerned that he sometimes ran while looking sideways and on tiptoe. Additionally, the school recommended fine motor skill reinforcement.
Regarding language development, he began producing his first words after 24 months and started using 2- to 3-word phrases after 36 months, with speech characterized mainly by omissions and articulatory substitutions.
In terms of feeding, he is a selective eater and has stopped eating foods such as fruits and vegetables. Sleep-wise, he does not show alterations and sleeps well, taking a nap. By age three, he still did not show sphincter control.
X.G. started preschool at 13 months. During the first year, his socialization with other children was minimal. He was described as a very restless child, with difficulties staying seated and paying attention. This evolved into more regulated behavior, allowing him to follow routines in the classroom. He enjoys games like puzzles, playing with cars, and modeling clay.
In 2022, the family sought a psychological evaluation due to concerns about a possible neurodevelopmental disorder, referred by the pediatric service. The pediatrician noted delays in language development, inconsistent eye contact, limited response to his name, difficulty following instructions, frequent walking on tiptoe, and concerns about frequent touching of his neck.
The psychological evaluation was conducted using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2 [62], the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-3rd Edition [63], the Adaptive Behavior Assessment System-3 [64], and observation in natural settings, such as the school.
Results from the Bayley-3 Scales and the ABAS-3 revealed below-average cognition (16th percentile), linguistic ability well below expectations for his age (2nd percentile), impaired motor skills (12th percentile), and low general adaptive functioning (14th percentile), particularly in social (34th percentile), practical (21st percentile), and conceptual (2nd percentile) areas. The ADOS-2 scored a total raw score of 16 points, indicating symptoms consistent with Autism Spectrum Disorder, including communication difficulties, social interaction impairments, and restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests. Observations in the school context confirmed low frustration tolerance, motor stereotypies, and lack of social intent, with a strong tendency to play alone.
This information led to the diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder. After the diagnosis, and due to the presence of linguistic and communicative difficulties, X.G. began attending speech therapy twice a week for 45-min sessions. Between January 2022 and June 2024, X.G. showed improvement in communicative intent, incorporating functions such as requesting, rejecting, greeting, and saying goodbye, although not consistently. His vocabulary and Mean Length of Utterance (MLU) increased, and he was able to imitate with gestural prompts and follow simple instructions. However, by April 2023, X.G. began to show more significant speech difficulties, marked by decreased intelligibility due to difficulties articulating phonemes both in repetition and spontaneously, as well as irregular voice volume.
Following the onset of these difficulties, X.G. was referred to the audiology service, where he underwent tympanometry, otoacoustic emissions testing, and auditory brainstem response (ABR) testing. Results revealed poor middle ear function bilaterally and altered cochlear mechanisms bilaterally. These findings were consistent with moderate bilateral hearing loss, although hearing aids were not prescribed.
Additionally, in April 2023, X.G. began supplementing traditional speech therapy with Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) therapy, administered by a Certified Behavior Analyst once a week for 50-min sessions, focusing on social skills such as self-regulation strategies, personal space respect strategies, and communication strategies.
Finally, in 2024, due to persistent feeding restrictions, X.G. attended Occupational Therapy, where the Sensory Profile-2 [65] was administered. The results indicated a low auditory threshold, with high thresholds for visual, kinetic, and tactile stimuli, accompanied by a tendency for passive self-regulation, reflecting a pattern of low registration.
With all this information, the parents requested a case study to implement intensive language therapy in July 2024.

2.2. Assessment Instruments

To carry out the pre- and post-intervention assessment processes, a total of five assessment tools were employed. All tests used in the evaluation are standardized and validated instruments for the Spanish language.
First, the Objective and Criterion Language Battery Screening Revised [66] was administered. This test can be used between ages 5 and 14. This battery evaluates four different components of language: morphology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics. Its purpose is to identify the areas where the individual has the most difficulty at the linguistic level. Regarding the reliability of the BLOC, Spanish studies report that the KR-20 coefficient of the semantic module is 0.90; that of syntax is 0.98; that of morphology is 0.98; that of pragmatics is 0.97. The evaluation experience has demonstrated its usefulness in populations with special educational needs [67].
Next, given the previously mentioned auditory difficulties, the subtests of phonological discrimination and recognition from the Revised Phonetic–Phonological Evaluation [68] were administered. This test is aimed at any age range.
The evaluation of the phonetic–phonological component was conducted using the Childhood Speech Phonological Evaluation [69]. This tool is used to detect phonological development disorders in children aged between 3 years and 7 years and 11 months through a set of cards that assess articulation.
The Communication Matrix [70] was then used to assess the pragmatic component. This tool is designed to measure communicative skills in individuals of any age whose communication is in the early stages. It allows for a precise determination of how the individual communicates. The matrix provides a list of communicative functions that the individual either masters, is developing (emerging), or does not have in their repertoire, based on 7 communication levels. The Communication Matrix is appropriate for individuals of any age who are in the initial stages of communication. The Matrix adapts to any type of communicative behavior, including augmentative modalities and communication alternatives (augmentative and alternative communication, AAC) and communication presymbolic (such as gestures, facial expressions, looks and body movements). It is appropriate for people with any type or degree of disability, including severe disabilities or multiple, intellectual limitations and sensory or physical disabilities.
Finally, the Phonological Knowledge Assessment Test [71] was administered. This test aims to assess, through 6 activities, the level of phonological knowledge (both syllabic and phonemic) of preschool children, as well as those who have difficulties with the acquisition of literacy, regardless of their educational level [71].

2.3. Procedure

This study was initiated following the request of X.G.’s family for intensive and specific intervention related to their child’s language difficulties. The family contacted the principal investigator of the EITAL research group (Research Team on Language Disorders and Alterations), which is composed of a team of professionals and researchers whose main focus is research and intervention in the field of speech therapy, specifically in children.
Subsequently, a coordination process was carried out between the EITAL team members and the child’s immediate environment (family and therapists). Several meetings were held before, during, and after the intervention process. The evaluation and intervention took place in July 2024, with a total duration of four weeks, from July 1 to July 26. This was conducted by five licensed speech therapists, specialized in child language. It is important to note that prior to the intervention, the parents were provided with an informed consent form regarding the use and treatment of their child’s data and images, which was completed.
A single-case design with pre- and post-intervention assessment was employed. The initial evaluation took place during the first week of the process, where the aforementioned tests were administered to establish X.G.’s language development level and determine the intervention goals. Based on this evaluation, five basic areas of focus were identified: articulation, intelligibility, phonological awareness, syntactic and semantic skills, and pragmatics. Each of these areas was addressed by a different speech therapist. The average number of hours spent by each therapist was approximately 21.6 h. The total treatment duration amounted to 108 h of work, with a weekly schedule of 27 h (5 to 6 h per day).
At the end of the intervention, a report with the results obtained by the child was compiled and delivered to the parents.

2.4. Intervention

To carry out the intervention, a treatment plan was developed by the five speech therapists, operationalized into objectives (Table 1).

2.5. Clinical Intervention

The clinical intervention followed a methodology based on learning through play [72,73] and consisted of a total of 88 h. During these sessions, the speech therapists interacted one-on-one with X.G. As we mentioned previously, the work team considered that, after analyzing the previous information and the initial evaluation, five large areas should be the focus of intervention in this case. These areas are: articulation, intelligibility, phonological awareness, syntactic and semantic skills, and pragmatics.
The intervention on speech and language precursors, articulation, intelligibility, and phonological awareness included activities such as auditory discrimination, sound searching and identification, syllable division, articulation of isolated phonemes, phonemes in direct syllables, and phonemes in monosyllabic, bisyllabic, trisyllabic, and polysyllabic words (increasing order of difficulty), structuring and formulating sentences, and activities involving unfamiliar or low-frequency vocabulary for X.G. During these activities, the therapists used evidence-based strategies to achieve the specific objectives being worked on. Some of the strategies employed included auditory bombardment [74,75], modeling [76], gestural facilitators [77,78], visual facilitators [79], expansions [80], and extensions [81].
The pragmatic work focused on the consistent use of communicative functions for behavioral regulation (requests and rejections) and social interaction (greetings, farewells, thanking, apologizing, and requesting permission). The objectives were hierarchized according to [82] proposal, beginning with ensuring the communicative functions already present in X.G. (although not used consistently), followed by the incorporation of new communication modes, the increase in content to be communicated, and the complexification of formal aspects. Since X.G. was able to imitate and benefited from gestural support, the Total Communication—Signed Speech Program by B. Scheaffer [83,84] was chosen. Thus, the communication intervention consisted of using communication temptations (based on the indications proposed by the ACACIA test [85] and the child’s interests), as well as modeling [86], physical shaping of communication signs [83], structured waits [87], parallel speech [88,89], expansions [80], and extensions [81].

2.6. Intervention in Natural Settings

In addition to the clinic-based intervention, the therapy was complemented with intervention in natural settings such as supermarkets, parks, and cafés. A total of 20 h of intervention were aimed at working in the natural context. Prior to these outings, anticipation and planning strategies [90] were used to explain to X.G. where they were going and what they would do. In the sessions prior to the natural environment outings (conducted in the clinic), situations that might arise were anticipated, and possible actions were discussed (e.g., “What do we do if we are not understood when we speak?”) using social stories [91]. Once in the natural setting, X.G. was exposed to communication with external conversation partners, practicing previously worked-on conflict resolution and communication skills. The aim was to raise X.G.’s awareness of his communicative and articulatory difficulties, while providing him with strategies that fostered his independence despite his challenges. The communicative functions of requesting, rejecting, thanking, and apologizing were worked on during these sessions.
Some of these sessions (7 h) were also conducted with the family environment, including X.G.’s sister as a key conversation partner [92]. In these sessions, the Teach-Model-Coach-Review protocol [93] was employed, with the aim of enabling X.G.’s sister to provide the necessary support for him to communicate effectively, rather than speaking for him when he was not understood. This approach aimed to enhance his autonomy.
Regardless of whether the sessions took place in a natural or clinical setting, coordination among the professionals was daily, with feedback provided to the parents to adapt the objectives and activities to X.G.’s individual characteristics and needs. Additionally, weekly meetings of the team were held.

3. Results

The results obtained by X.G. before and after the intervention are presented below. As previously mentioned, since this was an intensive intervention program addressing different areas of language and communicative development, a thorough evaluation of X.G. was conducted to establish a baseline and allow for subsequent comparisons. The following are the results obtained by X.G. in each evaluated area.
Regarding phonological discrimination, the results indicated no improvement, as the correct answer proportion remained the same (Direct score = 11/14; 78.57%) before and after the intervention. Specifically, phonological discrimination errors were found in the pairs of phonemes /p/ and /b/, /p/ and /f/, and /ɽr/ both before and after the intervention.
On the other hand, the results did show a slight improvement in phonological recognition following the intervention. Specifically, X.G.’s score on this task increased by 9 points, rising from a raw score of 133 before the intervention to a raw score of 142 after the intervention. Additionally, the results showed that the identification of most phonemes remained stable after the intervention, except for the identification of plosive vs. fricative phonemes and front vs. posterior fricative phonemes, which showed a higher increase in the direct score (3 and 4 points, respectively).
  • Articulation and intelligibility
Moving on to articulation, the results showed a notable improvement with a significant reduction in speech simplification processes performed by X.G. Specifically, the child went from producing 16 different speech simplification processes before the intervention to only 6 after it. Therefore, as shown in Table 2, X.G. achieved correct articulation of the phonemes /ʝ/, /s/, /t/, /d/, /g/, /ñ/, as well as appropriate articulation of codas, syllabic onsets, and diphthongs after the intervention.
  • Syntactic and semantic skills
Moving on to language development, the results demonstrated a clear improvement in the syntactic component. X.G.’s direct score increased by 12 points after the intervention, moving from −3.9 Standard Deviations (SDs) below children of his same chronological age to −0.8 SD after the intervention. However, the remaining components stayed below the expected level for his chronological age (between −1.4 SD and −5 SD compared to the normative group), although they remained relatively stable when comparing post-intervention results with pre-intervention results, as shown in Table 3.
  • Phonological awareness
Continuing with phonological awareness, the results showed another improvement for X.G., as his total score increased from 9/30 before the intervention to 20/30 after it. Furthermore, Table 4 illustrates how X.G. improved in both phonemic and syllabic awareness.
  • Pragmatic
Finally, in the pragmatic area, the results obtained by X.G. showed once again an improvement after the intervention. Specifically, as shown in Table 5, X.G. overcame pre-intentional behavior after the intervention and began to develop or improve unconventional communication, conventional communication, concrete symbols for communicative purposes, abstract symbols for communicative purposes, and language use.
Thus, the results reflect the improvement experienced by X.G. after the intervention. As can be observed, X.G. now consistently uses a greater number of concrete and abstract symbols for communicative purposes, as well as an increased number of joint attention and social interaction communicative functions.

4. Discussion

The case study presented has fulfilled the main objective set at the beginning of the research. Once again, it has been demonstrated how personalized and individualized intervention in language skills, based on the child’s characteristics, offers multiple benefits in improving the abilities and skills of individuals with ASD [94,95,96], especially in the early years of life [97,98]. Furthermore, it is noteworthy how, in this case, intensive intervention proved to be beneficial for the child, as indicated by other studies [99,100]. Pérez & Pérez [101] argue that three key characteristics of an effective intervention for ASD should be: (a) early initiation, (b) a high level of structure, and (c) an intensive nature, as also described by other authors [102,103,104], and these were implemented in the intervention proposed in this study. In line with this, Ratazzi [105] assert that if a child with ASD receives early intensive intervention, they have a chance of changing the trajectory of their development [106,107], which appears to be demonstrated by the post-intervention results.
It seems important, furthermore, that interventions conducted by specialists be complemented by work within the family environment. A meta-analysis on the effectiveness of language intervention in ASD [108] proposes that three factors determine the success of the treatment. First, early intervention, which enables children with ASD to significantly improve their language. Second, interventions should include training and feedback for parents [109]. And third, interventions should be long-term, allowing for adaptive interventions that monitor progress and adjust treatment according to the child’s response, particularly for children who start with minimal verbal skills [110]. The first two aspects were addressed in this intervention, and the third is currently being worked on, with continued monitoring and follow-up of the case.
Although individuals with ASD exhibit tremendous individual variability in language, especially in the early years [111,112], it appears that, as in this clinical case, the phonetic–phonological area is altered to a greater or lesser degree [30] and phonological intensive intervention can improve speech intelligibility to a greater or lesser degree, as in this case. Thus, improving intelligibility is one of the major challenges and goals that professionals focus on [113,114,115,116], following the family’s requests in this case. However, it is important to note that the intervention proposed by the professionals considered it essential to work on those areas of language that presented the greatest difficulties, understanding communication work as a global process encompassing all its aspects [59,117].
Regarding the proposed language treatment, this intervention has highlighted the direct and directional relationship between expressive language skills. Until now, much of the reviewed studies have focused on intervention programs specifically aimed at the pragmatic subsystem [118,119,120,121,122,123], but less attention has been given to language as a whole and the interconnectivity between language systems. Although initially the parents’ demand was to specifically work on articulating all the phonemes and their generalization to spontaneous speech, the team considered it necessary to address all the language areas that were affected to varying degrees in order to improve and consolidate the proposed objectives. The improvements have not only been significant in the phonological level, but the other language subsystems also benefited from this intervention. This aligns with theoretical studies that establish the relationship between phonetics and grammar [124,125], morphology [126,127], and semantics [128]. Nonetheless, it is important to emphasize that there are still few studies focused on phonetic–phonological intervention in ASD, and, as in the case of [129], they are also single-case studies. Similarly, some research has focused on improving articulation [130].
Language interventions in natural settings are increasingly seen as a way to enhance social communication skills in familiar environments [131,132]. Under the umbrella of social communication skills lies the need to increase functional, spontaneous expressive language in young children with ASD [133]. Research has also shown that naturalistic interventions promote social development, as they typically involve interactive exchanges between the child and an adult or typically developing peer [134]. Additionally, these are “family-friendly” approaches that tend to increase both the quantity and quality of early learning experiences. Parents can easily implement these strategies in their natural environments and during ongoing activities [135]. This work also aims to move toward natural setting intervention models to promote social inclusion [85].
Finally, it is worth highlighting that communication skills positively influence the quality of life of individuals with ASD and their families throughout all stages of development [136,137,138]. Therefore, long-term linguistic intervention should continue to be emphasized [139].
Regarding the limitations of this research, as it is a single-case design, the results cannot be generalized. However, it is considered essential that interventions be tailored to the level, characteristics, and environment of the person with ASD so that the direct impact of the intervention can be observed, thereby enhancing the child’s linguistic capacities and skills. It would be essential to monitor the child over a longer period of intensive therapy (3 to 6 months) to ensure the automation of speech and language patterns. This period allows for a more realistic assessment of the effects of intensive treatment due to the unique characteristics of the child’s psychophysiological development.
In conclusion, it is important to once again emphasize the necessity of implementing and evaluating interventions conducted in clinical practice to demonstrate their efficacy and effectiveness to the scientific community. To achieve this, it is important to study clinical cases in depth and value the interventions that are proposed from an individual and personal point of view. This will allow them to be used in the future by other professionals. Language intervention will not only help improve the language and communication characteristics of individuals with ASD in the early stages, but will also contribute to improving their quality of life throughout their development.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.M.-S., N.P.-G., N.L.-V. and N.S.-M.; methodology, E.M.-S.; software, E.M.-S.; formal analysis, E.M.-S.; investigation, E.M.-S., N.P.-G., N.L.-V. and N.S.-M.; writing—original draft preparation, E.M.-S., N.P.-G., N.L.-V. and N.S.-M.; writing—review and editing, E.M.-S., N.P.-G., N.L.-V. and N.S.-M.; supervision, E.M.-S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. The Research Ethics Committee of University Complutense of Madrid, in its session on 8 February 2024, was approved with the reference CE_20240208_20_SAL.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from the participant involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are not available due to privacy restrictions on information obtained from the participant.

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the participation of this family.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Lord, C.; Elsabbagh, M.; Baird, G.; Veenstra-Vanderweele, J. Autism spectrum disorder. Lancet 2018, 392, 508–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Sharma, S.R.; Gonda, X.; Tarazi, F.I. Autism spectrum disorder: Classification, diagnosis and therapy. Pharmacol. Ther. 2018, 190, 91–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Lord, C.; Brugha, T.S.; Charman, T.; Cusack, J.; Dumas, G.; Frazier, T.; Jones, E.J.; Jones, R.M.; Pickles, A.; State, M.W.; et al. Autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2020, 6, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Buffle, P.; Naranjo, D. Identificación y Diagnóstico Tempranos Del Trastorno Del Espectro Autista: Una Revisión de La Literatura Sobre Recomendaciones Basadas En La Evidencia. Rev. Ecuat. Pediatr. 2021, 22, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Reynoso, C.; Rangel, M.J.; Melgar, V. El trastorno del espectro autista: Aspectos etiológicos, diagnósticos y terapéuticos. Rev. Med. Inst. Mex. Seguro Soc. 2017, 55, 214–222. [Google Scholar]
  6. Conlon, O.; Volden, J.; Smith, I.M.; Duku, E.; Zwaigenbaum, L.; Waddell, C.; Szatmari, P.; Mirenda, P.; Vaillancourt, T.; Bennett, T.; et al. Gender Differences in Pragmatic Communication in School-Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 49, 1937–1948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Hervás, A. Género femenino y autismo: Infra detección y mis diagnósticos. Medicina 2022, 82, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  8. Ventoso, A.; Brioso, Y. Comunicación y lenguaje en niños con Trastorno del Espectro Autista. Educ. Infancia. 2019, 8, 157–172. [Google Scholar]
  9. Paul, R.; Simmons, E. Communication and its development in autism spectrum disorders. In Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders; Volkmar, F.R., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2007; pp. 129–156. [Google Scholar]
  10. Tager-Flusberg, H.; Paul, R.; Lord, C. Language and communication in autism. In Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders: Diagnosis, Development, and Brain Mechanisms; Volkmar, F.R., Paul, R., Klin, A., Cohen, D., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 335–364. [Google Scholar]
  11. López, I.; Förster, J. Trastornos del neurodesarrollo: Dónde estamos hoy y hacia dónde nos dirigimos. Rev. Med. Clin. Condes. 2022, 33, 367–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Vogindroukas, I.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Gkotsis, K.; Elbasan, B.; Çıtak, S. Developmental Profiles of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Turk. J. Pediatr. 2022, 64, 495–503. [Google Scholar]
  13. Boucher, J. Research Review: Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder—Characteristics and causes. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatr. 2012, 53, 219–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Schaeffer, J.; Abd El-Raziq, M.; Castroviejo, E.; Durrleman, S.; Ferré, S.; Grama, I.; Tuller, L. Language in autism: Domains, profiles and co-occurring conditions. J. Neural Transm. 2023, 130, 433–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Camaioni, L.; Perucchini, P.; Muratori, F.; Parrini, B.; Cesari, A. The communicative use of pointing in autism: Developmental profile and factors related to change. Eur. Psychiatr. 2003, 18, 6–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Clements, C.; Chawarska, K. Beyond pointing: Development of the “showing” gesture in children with autism spectrum disorder. Yale Rev. Undergrad. Res. Psychol. 2010, 2, 43–63. [Google Scholar]
  17. McKern, N.; Dargue, N.; Sweller, N. Comparing gesture frequency between autistic and neurotypical individuals: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2023, 149, 724–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ye, S.; Kang, S.; Lee, K. Analysis of children’s proto-imperative and proto-declarative gestures and their developmental changes. J. Speech Hear. Res. 2021, 64, 839–851. [Google Scholar]
  19. Shumway, S.; Wetherby, A.M. Communicative acts of children with autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2009, 52, 1139–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Chiang, H.M. Expressive communication of children with autism: The use of challenging behaviour. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2008, 52, 966–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Miller, M.; Iosif, A.M.; Hill, M.; Young, G.S.; Schwichtenberg, A.J.; Ozonoff, S. Response to Name in Infants Developing Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Prospective Study. J. Pediatr. 2017, 183, 141–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Winczura, B. Interacciones sociales en niños con trastorno del espectro autista: El rol del contacto ocular. Psychol. Pol. 2014, 48, 311–327. [Google Scholar]
  23. Wiklund, M. Interactional challenges in conversations with autistic preadolescents: The role of prosody and non-verbal communication in other-initiated repairs. J. Pragmat. 2016, 94, 76–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Thorsson, M.; Sandberg, M.; Carlsson, H. Gestures as predictors of later joint attention in toddlers with ASD. J. Child. Lang. 2024, 51, 102–122. [Google Scholar]
  25. Artiles Travieso, C. El Lenguaje en Alumnos de Educación Primaria Con Trastorno Del Espectro Autista. Ph.D. Thesis, Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canarias, Las Palmas, Spain, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gates, S.S. Speech Processing in Typical and Atypical Language Development: Using Nonwords to Map the Way. Ph.D. Thesis, University College London, London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  27. Tambyraja, S.R.; Justice, L.M.; Logan, J.A. Phonological awareness intervention in preschoolers: Examining individual differences. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 107, 560–573. [Google Scholar]
  28. Cleland, J.; Gibbon, F.E.; Peppé, S.; O’Hare, A.; Rutherford, M. Phonetic and phonological errors in children with high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2010, 12, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Shriberg, L.D.; Paul, R.; McSweeny, J.L.; Klin, A.M.; Cohen, D.J.; Volkmar, F.R. Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2001, 44, 1097–1115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Wolk, L.; Edwards, M.L.; Conture, E.G. Coarticulatory organization of speech in children with phonological disorders. J. Speech Hear. Res. 2016, 36, 1048–1054. [Google Scholar]
  31. Finnegan, E.G.; Asaro-Saddler, K.; Zajic, M.C. Production and comprehension of pronouns in individuals with autism: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Autism 2021, 25, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Perovic, A.; Modyanova, N.; Wexler, K. Comprehension of reflexive and personal pronouns in children with autism: A syntactic or pragmatic deficit? Appl. Psycholinguist. 2013, 34, 813–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Novogrodsky, R.; Edelson, L.R. Ambiguous pronoun use in narratives of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Child. Lang. Teach. Ther. 2016, 32, 241–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sterponi, L.; de Kirby, K.; Shankey, J. Rethinking language in autism. Autism 2015, 19, 517–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Uclés, M. Las alteraciones morfosintácticas en el trastorno del espectro autista. Rev. Psicol. Clin. Niños Adolesc. 2015, 2, 21–30. [Google Scholar]
  36. Pérez, J.M. Autismo: Un trastorno penetrante del desarrollo. In Autismo. Un Enfoque Orientado a la Formación en Logopedia; Martos Pérez, J., Pérez Juliá, M., Eds.; Nau Llibres: Valencia, Spain, 2002; pp. 17–28. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hopkins, Z.; Yuill, N.; Keller, B. Children with autism align syntax in natural conversation. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2016, 37, 347–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lind, S.E.; Bowler, D.M. Delayed self-recognition in children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2009, 39, 643–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Whyte, E.M.; Nelson, K.E.; Scherf, K.S. Social preferences for novel objects in autism spectrum disorder: Eye-tracking reveals meaning in ambiguity. Dev. Sci. 2014, 17, 726–735. [Google Scholar]
  40. Kover, S.T.; McDuffie, A.; Hagerman, R.J.; Abbeduto, L. Receptive vocabulary in boys with autism spectrum disorder: Cross-sectional developmental trajectories. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 2696–2709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Hudry, K.; Chandler, S.; Bedford, R.; Pasco, G.; Gliga, T.; Elsabbagh, M.; Johnson, M.H.; Charman, T. Early language profiles in infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 154–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Rivière, A.; Belinchón, M. Lenguaje y autismo. In Autismo, Enfoques Actuales Para Padres Y Profesionales de la Salud Y la Educación; Valdez, D., Ed.; Fundec: Buenos Aires, Argentina, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  43. Eigsti, I.M.; de Marchena, A.B.; Schuh, J.M.; Kelley, E. Language acquisition in autism spectrum disorders: A developmental review. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2011, 5, 681–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Sterponi, L.; Shankey, J. Rethinking echolalia: Repetition as interactional resource in the communication of a child with autism. J. Child. Lang. 2014, 41, 275–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Cuña González, M.D.B. Cubriendo Las Dificultades Del Lenguaje de Los Niños Con Tea a Través Del Método Teacch: Propuesta de Intervención [Trabajo Final de Grado]; Universidad de Valladolid: Valladolid, Spain, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  46. Ryan, S.; Roberts, J.; Beamish, W. Echolalia in autism: A scoping review. Int. J. Disabil. Dev. Educ. 2024, 71, 831–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Stiegler, L.N. Examining the echolalia literature: Where do speech-language pathologists stand? Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2015, 24, 750–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Martos, J.; Ayuda, R. Comunicación y lenguaje en el espectro autista: El autismo y la disfasia. Rev. Neurol. 2002, 34 (Suppl. S1), S58–S63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Wearing, C. Autism, metaphor and relevance theory. Mind Lang. 2010, 25, 196–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Geurts, B.; Kissine, M.; van Tiel, B. Pragmatic reasoning in autism. In Thinking, Reasoning, and Decision Making in Autism; Morsanyi, K., Byrne, R.M.J., Eds.; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2019; pp. 113–134. [Google Scholar]
  51. Dudok, M. Comunicación Funcional en Niños Con Características de Tea en El áMbito Del Hogar Y Del Consultorio Fonoaudiológico [Trabajo Final de Grado]; Universidad FASTA: Mar del Plata, Argentina, 2024. [Google Scholar]
  52. Shanker, S. What Children Know When They Know What a Name Is: The Non-Cartesian View of Language Acquisition. Curr. Anthropol. 2001, 42, 481–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Davies, S.M. Patterns of Understanding of Prepositions Among Children Showing Typical Language Development and Children with Specific Language Impairment. Ph.D. Thesis, Curtin University, Bentley, WA, Australia, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  54. Young, E.C.; Diehl, J.J.; Morris, D.; Hyman, S.L.; Bennetto, L. The use of two language tests to identify pragmatic language problems in children with autism spectrum disorders. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2005, 36, 62–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Burke, M.; Kraut, R.; Williams, D. Social use of computer-mediated communication by adults on the autism spectrum. In Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work, CSCW, Savannah, GA, USA, 6–10 February 2010; pp. 425–434. [Google Scholar]
  56. Donnellan, A.M.; Hill, D.H.; Leary, M.R. Rethinking autism: Implications of sensory and movement differences for understanding and support. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Prelock, P.J.; Nelson, N.W. Language and communication in autism: An integrated view. Pediatr. Clin. N. Am. 2012, 59, 129–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Paul, R. Promoting social communication in high functioning individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2003, 12, 87–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Prizant, B.M.; Wetherby, A.M.; Rydell, P.J. Communication intervention issues for children with autism spectrum disorders. In Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Transactional Developmental Perspective; Wetherby, A.M., Prizant, B.M., Eds.; Paul, H. Brookes Publishing Co.: Baltimore, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 193–224. [Google Scholar]
  60. Mandy, W.P.; Skuse, D.H. Research review: What is the association between the social-communication element of autism and repetitive interests, behaviours and activities? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. 2008, 49, 795–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Jordan, R. Autistic spectrum disorders. In The Handbook of Intellectual Disability and Clinical Psychology Practice; Carr, A., Linehan, C., O’Reilly, G., Noonan Walsh, P., McEvoy, J., Eds.; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2014; pp. 551–578. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lord, C.; Rutter, M.; DiLavore, P.C.; Risi, S.; Gotham, K.; Bishop, S.L. Escala de Observación Para El Diagnóstico Del Espectro Autista—2 (Ados-2), 2nd ed.; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  63. Spencer-Smith, M.M.; Spittle, A.J.; Lee, K.J.; Doyle, L.W.; Anderson, P.J. Bayley-III cognitive and language scales in preterm children. Pediatrics 2015, 135, e1258–e1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. du Preez, J. Adaptive Behavior Assessment System—Third edition (ABAS-3). In The Elements of Applied Psychological Practice in Australia: Preparing for the National Psychology Examination; Pelling, N.J., Burton, L.J., Eds.; Routledge: Milton Park, UK, 2017; pp. 73–76. [Google Scholar]
  65. Dunn, W. Sensory Profile 2. User’s Manual; Pearson: Bloomington, MN, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  66. Puyuelo Sanclemente, M.; Renom Pinsach, J.; Solanas Pérez, A.; Wiig, E.H. Evaluación Del Lenguaje Bloc Screening-R. Manual de Usuario; Elsevier Masson: Barcelona, Spain, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  67. Puyuelo, M.; Salavera, C.; Wiig, E. Aportaciones a la evaluación del lenguaje mediante BLOC-C y BLOC-SR. Rev. Logop. Foniatr. Audiol. 2013, 33, 83–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Susanibar, F.; Dioses, A.; Castillo, J. Evaluación de los Trastornos de los Sonidos del Habla—TSH. In Trastornos Del Habla: De Los Fundamentos a la Evaluación; Susanibar, F., Dioses, A., Marchesan, I., Guzman, M., Leal, G., Guitar, B., Junqueira Bohnen, A., Eds.; EOS: Madrid, Spain, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  69. Bosch Galceran, L. Evaluación Fonológica Del Habla Infantil; Elsevier Masson: Barcelona, Spain, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  70. Rowland, C. Manual: Matriz de Comunicación En Internet; Oregon Health & Science University: Portland, OR, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  71. Ramos Sánchez, J.L.; Cuadrado Gordillo, I. PECO. Prueba de Evaluación de Conocimiento Fonológico. Manual; EOS: Madrid, Spain, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  72. Bernabeu Morón, N.; Goldstein, A. Creatividad Y Aprendizaje: El Juego Como Herramienta Pedagógica; Narcea Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  73. González de la Roca, C.I.; Chocoj González, M.; Guzmán Pérez, M.M.; Alfaro Méndez, I.M.; Ríos Ramírez, A. Neurociencia: El juego como conector del aprendizaje. Rev. Acad. CUNZAC 2021, 4, 47–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Encinas, D.; Plante, E. Feasibility of a recasting and auditory bombardment treatment with young cochlear implant users. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2016, 47, 157–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Plante, E.; Tucci, A.; Nicholas, K.; Arizmendi, G.D.; Vance, R. Effective use of auditory bombardment as a therapy adjunct for children with developmental language disorders. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 320–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Lovett, M.W.; Lacerenza, L.; Borden, S.L.; Frijters, J.C.; Steinbach, K.A.; De Palma, M. Components of effective remediation for developmental reading disabilities: Combining phonological and strategy-based instruction to improve outcomes. J. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 92, 263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Cravotta, A.; Busà, M.G.; Prieto, P. Effects of encouraging the use of gestures on speech. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2019, 62, 3204–3219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Kisa, Y.D.; Goldin-Meadow, S.; Casasanto, D. Do gestures really facilitate speech production? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2022, 151, 1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Meng, F. Whole Word Phonetic Displays for Speech Articulation Training. Ph.D. Thesis, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  80. Wong, T.P.; Moran, C.; Foster-Cohen, S. The effects of expansions, questions and cloze procedures on children’s conversational skills. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2012, 26, 273–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Neugebauer, S.; Coyne, M.; McCoach, B.; Ware, S. Teaching beyond the intervention: The contribution of teacher language extensions to vocabulary learning in urban kindergarten classrooms. Read. Writ. 2017, 30, 543–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Gortazar, P. Jerarquización de objetivos para el inicio del desarrollo del lenguaje y la comunicación en personas con autismo y TGD. In Comunicación Y Programas de Tránsito a la Vida Adulta en Personas Con Necesidades de Apoyo Generalizado; Departamento de Educación y Cultura: Pamplona, Spain, 1995; pp. 1–24. [Google Scholar]
  83. Rebollo Martínez, A.; Capel Sánchez, A.; Brogueras Martínez, T.; Díaz Carcelén, M.; Castellanos Niñerola, M.; Pérez Avilés, F.; Alarcón Abellán, J.M. Programa de comunicación total habla signada, B. Consejería de Educación Y Cultura de la Región de Murcia; Murcia: Schaeffer, Spain, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  84. Marín, C.R. Estrategias de Intervención Comunicativa en Niños Con Trastorno Del Espectro Autista: Análisis Del Sistema de Comunicación Total de Benson Schaeffer; Punto Rojo Libros: Madrid, Spain, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  85. Tamarit, J. Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorder, ASD. Milestones and challenges (Intervención en el Trastorno del Espectro del Autismo TEA. Hitos y retos). J. Stud. Educ. Dev. 2020, 43, 872–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Biggs, E.E.; Carter, E.W.; Gilson, C.B. Systematic review of interventions involving aided AAC modeling for children with complex communication needs. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 123, 443–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Goldstein, H. Communication intervention for children with autism: A review of treatment efficacy. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2002, 32, 373–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Hadley, P.A.; Walsh, K.M. Toy talk: Simple strategies to create richer grammatical input. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2014, 45, 159–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Raver, S.A.; Childress, D.C. Collaboration and teamwork with families and professionals. In Family-Centered Early Intervention: Supporting Infants and Toddlers in Natural Environments; Raver, S.A., Childress, D.C., Eds.; Paul, H. Brookes Publishing: Baltimore, Spain, 2014; pp. 120–130. [Google Scholar]
  90. Stavropoulos, K.K.M.; Carver, L.J. Effect of familiarity on reward anticipation in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e106667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Sansosti, F.J.; Powell-Smith, K.A.; Kincaid, D. A research synthesis of social story interventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Focus. Autism Other Dev. Disabl. 2004, 19, 194–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Paúls, B.G.; Campos, V.M. Estrategias colaborativas de compensación de déficit lingüístico: La importancia del interlocutor-clave en el índice de participación conversacional. Pragmalingüística 2005, 13, 87–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Leung, Y.M. The Effectiveness of Caregiver-Implemented Enhanced Milieu Teaching Using Teach-Model-Coach-Review Model in Preschool Children with Language Impairments: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  94. Cuesta-Gómez, J.L.; Sánchez Fuentes, S.; Orozco Gómez, M.L.; Valenti, A.; Cottini, L. Trastorno del espectro del autismo: Intervención educativa y formación a lo largo de la vida. Psychol. Soc. Educ. 2017, 8, 157–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. González-Moreno, C.X. Intervención en un niño con autismo mediante el juego. Rev. Fac. Med. 2018, 66, 365–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Mebarak, M.; Martínez, M.; Serna, A. Revisión bibliográfico analítica acerca de las diversas teorías y programas de intervención del autismo infantil. Psicol. Caribe. 2009, 24, 120–146. [Google Scholar]
  97. Millá, M.G.; Mulas, F. Atención temprana y programas de intervención específica en el trastorno del espectro autista. Rev. Neurol. 2009, 48 (Suppl. S2), S47–S52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Yunta, J.A.M.; Baduell, M.P.; Salvadó, B.S.; Santasusana, A.V. Autismo: Identificación e intervención temprana. Acta Neurol. Colomb. 2006, 22, 97–105. [Google Scholar]
  99. Pérez-González, L.A.; Williams, G. Programa integral para la enseñanza de habilidades a niños con autismo. Psicothema 2005, 17, 233–244. [Google Scholar]
  100. Sanz-Cervera, P.; Fernández-Andrés, M.I.; Pastor-Cerezuela, G.; Tárraga-Mínguez, R. Efectividad de las intervenciones basadas en metodología TEACCH en el trastorno del espectro autista: Un estudio de revisión. Pap. Psicol. 2018, 39, 40–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Pérez, M.D.L.L.A.; Pérez, R.B. Alternativas de tratamiento en los trastornos del espectro autista: Una revisión bibliográfica entre 2000 y 2016. Rev. Psicol. Clín. Niños Adolesc. 2018, 5, 22–31. [Google Scholar]
  102. Fuentes-Biggi, J.; Ferrari Arroyo, M.J.; Boada Muñoz, L.; Touriño Aguilera, E.; Artigas-Pallarés, J.; Belinchón-Carmona, M.J.; Muñoz-Yunta, J.A.; Hervás-Zúñiga, A.; Canal-Bedia, R.; Hernández, J.M.; et al. Guía de buena práctica para el tratamiento de los trastornos del espectro autista. Rev. Neurol. 2006, 43, 425–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Martos, J.; Llorente, M. El Niño Al Que SE Le Olvidó Cómo Mirar: Comprender Y Afrontar El Autismo; La. esfera de los libros: Madrid, Spain, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  104. Mulas, F.; Ros-Cervera, G.; Millá, M.G.; Etchepareborda, M.C.; Abad, L.; Téllez de Meneses, M. Modelos de intervención en niños con autismo. Rev. Neurol. 2010, 50, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Rattazzi, A. La importancia de la detección precoz y de la intervención temprana en niños con condiciones del espectro autista. Rev. Argent. Psiquiatr. 2014, 25, 290–294. [Google Scholar]
  106. Karanth, P.; Chandhok, T.S. Impact of early intervention on children with autism spectrum disorders as measured by inclusion and retention in mainstream schools. Indian. J. Pediatr. 2013, 80, 911–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Koegel, L.K.; Koegel, R.L.; Ashbaugh, K.; Bradshaw, J. The importance of early identification and intervention for children with or at risk for autism spectrum disorders. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2014, 16, 50–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Hampton, L.H.; Kaiser, A. Intervention effects on spoken-language outcomes for children with autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2016, 60, 444–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Kaiser, A.P.; Roberts, M.Y. Advances in early communication and language intervention. J. Early Interv. 2011, 33, 298–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Kasari, C.; Siller, M.; Huynh, L.N.; Shih, W.; Swanson, M.; Hellemann, G.S.; Sugar, C.A. Randomized controlled trial of parental responsiveness intervention for toddlers at high risk for autism. Infant. Behav. Dev. 2014, 37, 711–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Pickles, A.; Anderson, D.K.; Lord, C. Heterogeneity and plasticity in the development of language: A 17-year follow-up of children referred early for possible autism. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatr. 2014, 55, 1354–1362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Weismer, S.E.; Kover, S.T. Preschool language variation, growth, and predictors in children on the autism spectrum. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatr. 2015, 56, 1327–1337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Koegel, R.L.; Camarata, S.; Koegel, L.K.; Ben-Tall, A.; Smith, A.E. Increasing speech intelligibility in children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1998, 28, 241–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Petinou, K. Promoting speech intelligibility in autism spectrum disorder through the implementation of phonologically similar stimuli. Folia Phoniatr. Logopaed. 2021, 73, 174–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Rahman, M.M.; Ferdous, S.M.; Ahmed, S.I. Increasing intelligibility in the speech of the autistic children by an interactive computer game. In Proceedings of the 2010 IEEE International Symposium on Multimedia, San Diego, CA, USA, 13–15 December 2020; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2010; pp. 383–387. [Google Scholar]
  116. Smith, A.E.; Camarata, S. Using teacher-implemented instruction to increase language intelligibility of children with autism. J. Posit. Behav. Interv. 1999, 1, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Wetherby, A.M.; Prizant, B.M. Enhancing language and communication development in autism spectrum disorders: Assessment and intervention guidelines. In Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3rd ed.; Stone-MacDonald, A., Cihak, D.F., Zager, D., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2004; pp. 334–372. [Google Scholar]
  118. Binns, A.V.; Oram Cardy, J. Developmental social pragmatic interventions for preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Autism Dev. Lang. Impair. 2019, 4, 2396941518824497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Ingersoll, B.; Dvortcsak, A.; Whalen, C.; Sikora, D. The effects of a developmental, social—Pragmatic language intervention on rate of expressive language production in young children with autistic spectrum disorders. Focus. Autism Other Dev. Disabl. 2005, 20, 213–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Papadopoulos, D. A general overview of the pragmatic language-social skills and interventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Open Access 2018, 8, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Parsons, L.; Cordier, R.; Munro, N.; Joosten, A.; Speyer, R. A systematic review of pragmatic language interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0172242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Pereira, T.; Ramalho, A.M.; Sá Couto, P.; Lousada, M. Exploring the effectiveness of the Pragmatic Intervention Programme (PICP) with children with autism spectrum disorder and developmental language disorder: A non-randomised controlled trial. Autism 2024, 13623613241287017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Pereira, T.; Ramalho, A.M.; Valente, A.R.S.; Couto, P.S.; Lousada, M. The effects of the Pragmatic Intervention Programme in children with autism spectrum disorder and developmental language disorder. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Keating, P.A. Phonetic representations in a generative grammar. J. Phonetics 1990, 18, 321–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Keydana, G. From phonetics to grammar. Indogermanische Forschungen 2014, 119, 269–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Abah, J.A.; Emodi, L.N. The distinctive and symbiotic relationship between phonology, phonetics and morphology. Ansu J. Lang. Lit. Stud. 2017, 1, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
  127. van de Vijver, R.; Tomaschek, F. Phonological and phonetic variation in spoken morphology. Morphology 2021, 31, 83–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Beaver, D.I.; Clark, B.; Flemming, E.S.; Jaeger, T.F.; Wolters, M. When semantics meets phonetics: Acoustical studies of second-occurrence focus. Lang 2007, 83, 245–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Nordgren, P.M. Phonological development in a child with autism spectrum condition: Case study of an intervention. J. Interact. Res. Commun. Disord. 2015, 6, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Chaldi, D. Evaluation of the effectiveness of video-modeling for teaching articulation in verbal children with autism spectrum disorder. Rev. Eur. Stud. 2023, 15, 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Kaiser, A.P.; Grim, J.C. Teaching functional communication skills. In Instruction of Students with Severe Disabilities; Snell, M., Brown, F., Eds.; Pearson: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 447–488. [Google Scholar]
  132. Schreibman, L.; Dawson, G.; Stahmer, A.C.; Landa, R.; Rogers, S.J.; McGee, G.G.; Kasari, C.; Ingersoll, B.; Kaiser, A.; Bruinsma, Y.; et al. Naturalistic developmental behavioral interventions: Empirically validated treatments for autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 2411–2428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Lane, J.D.; Lieberman-Betz, R.; Gast, D.L. An analysis of naturalistic interventions for increasing spontaneous expressive language in children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Spec. Educ. 2016, 50, 49–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Morrier, M.J.; McGee, G.G.; Daly, T. Effects of toy selection and arrangement on the social behaviors of an inclusive group of preschool-aged children. Early Child. Serv: An. Interdiscip. J. Effect. 2009, 3, 157–177. [Google Scholar]
  135. Schreibman, L.; Koegel, R.L. Training for parents of children with autism: Pivotal responses, generalization, and individualization of interventions. In Psychosocial Treatment for Child and Adolescent Disorders: Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice, 2nd ed.; Hibbs, E.D., Jensen, P.S., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2005; pp. 605–631. [Google Scholar]
  136. Chiang, H.M.; Wineman, I. Factors associated with quality of life in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: A review of literature. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2014, 8, 974–986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Ikeda, E.; Hinckson, E.; Krägeloh, C. Assessment of quality of life in children and youth with autism spectrum disorder: A critical review. Qual. Life Res. 2014, 23, 1069–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Tobin, M.C.; Drager, K.D.; Richardson, L.F. A systematic review of social participation for adults with autism spectrum disorders: Support, social functioning, and quality of life. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2014, 8, 214–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Lerna, A.; Esposito, D.; Conson, M.; Massagli, A. Long-term effects of PECS on social–communicative skills of children with autism spectrum disorders: A follow-up study. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2014, 49, 478–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Proposed intervention objectives.
Table 1. Proposed intervention objectives.
Objectives Related to Speech and Language Precursors
1. Auditory discrimination
- Improve the patient’s ability to audibly discriminate phonemes, syllables, and pairs of words.
2. Communicative elements
- Enhance eye contact so that the child can appreciate and interpret images and gestures.
- Incorporate joint attention patterns by working on alternating eye gaze between the child and an object.
- Encourage reciprocal gaze patterns to promote secondary intersubjectivity.
- Optimize the understanding of the cause–effect relationship and the means–end connection.
- Objectives related to speech.
Objectives Related to Speech
- Promote the use of tonal inflections during speech, emphasizing the melody of different types of sentences.
- Improve vocal dynamics during speech to facilitate the capture of the listener’s attention.
- Adjust speech speed and rhythm.
- Increase speech intelligibility.
Objectives Related to Language
1. Phonetic–phonological area
- Improve the voluntary control of articulatory movements to achieve automation.
- Achieve correct articulation points and modes for all the consonantal phonemes in Spanish.
- Promote the identification of phonemes, their grouping into syllables, and their combination into words.
- Foster the correct articulation of altered phonemes in syllables (both direct and inverse positions), words (monosyllabic, bisyllabic, trisyllabic, and polysyllabic), and sentences, reducing the processes of speech simplification detected:
  ○ Deaffrication of the consonant /ch/.
  ○ Occlusion of fricative sounds such as /x/ (replacing with /k/), /f/ (replacing with /p/), and /s/ (replacing with /t/).
  ○ Posteriorization of coronal sounds like /t/ (replacing with /k/) and /d/ (omitting or replacing with distorted /g/).
  ○ Voicing of sounds (/b/, /d/, and /g/ tend to be replaced by /p/, /t/, or /k/, or omitted).
  ○ Semi-consonantization of liquids (/kiase/ instead of /klase/).
  ○ Conversion of the approximant /ñ/ into a liquid (/nino/ instead of /niño/).
  ○ Absence of the trilled /r/.
  ○ Distortion of the trilled /r/.
  ○ Omission of final consonants or codas.
  ○ Omission of syllabic onsets at the beginning of words in polysyllabic words.
  ○ Simplification of diphthongs.
  ○ Simplification of complex syllabic onsets (/futa/ instead of /fruta/).
- Generalize correctly produced phonemes into spontaneous language.
2. Morphosyntactic area
- Promote an Age-Appropriate Mean Length of Utterance (MLU).
- Encourage the production of all the words in a sentence.
- Achieve the use and generalization of function words.
- Promote the production of more complex sentences, correctly joining sentences together.
- Learn morphological variations of verbs and nouns regarding tense, number, and person.
3. Lexical–semantic area
- Encourage the integration of vocabulary according to the phoneme being worked on at the time.
- Increase the understanding of word meanings and their integration into discourse.
4. Pragmatic area
- Promote the consistent use of communicative functions for behavioral regulation (requesting objects, actions, and rejecting) and social interaction (requests for social routines, greetings and farewells, showing, calling, requesting permission, and thanking).
- Learn to use oral language functionally and contextually, thereby acquiring new modes of communication.
- Increase the content to be communicated, enhancing the quantity of stimuli and conversation partners with whom communicative functions for behavioral regulation and social interaction can be used.
- Complexify the formal aspects of communication by using combinations of words.
- Incorporate the use of joint attention functions.
- Promote positioning in the adjacent pair to balance conversational participation.
- Foster conversational priority so the patient can provide semantically related responses.
- Incorporate the use of compensatory editing tasks to improve intelligibility.
Table 2. Comparison of speech simplification processes before and after the intervention.
Table 2. Comparison of speech simplification processes before and after the intervention.
Substitution ProcessesPre-InterventionPost-Intervention
Deaffrication/ʃ/xx
Occlusion/ʝ/x
/f/xx
/s/x
Posteriorization/t/x
/d/x
Voicing/b/xx
/d/x
/g/x
Semi-consonantization of liquidsx
Conversion /ɲ// to liquidx
Lateralization of vibrants x
Omission of trilled rxx
Syllabic Structure Processes
Omission of codasx
Omission of syllabic onsetx
Simplification of diphthongsx
Simplification of complex onsetxx
Table 3. Comparison between language skills before and after the intervention.
Table 3. Comparison between language skills before and after the intervention.
Pre-InterventionPost-Intervention
DSCSTSDSCSTS
Morphology113010
Syntax2111141942
Semantic61364121
Pragmatic41183118
Note: DS: Direct score; CS: Centil score; TS: Transformed score.
Table 4. Comparison of phonological awareness skills before and after the intervention.
Table 4. Comparison of phonological awareness skills before and after the intervention.
SkillPre-InterventionPost-Intervention
Identification6/108/10
Addition3/109/10
Omission0/103/10
Syllabic Awareness5/1511/15
Phonemic Awareness4/159/15
Table 5. Comparison between pragmatic skills before and after the intervention.
Table 5. Comparison between pragmatic skills before and after the intervention.
AchievedDominatedIn ProgressNot Used
Pre-intentional behavior pre100%
Pre-intentional behavior post100%
Intentional behavior pre0%25%0%75%
Intentional behavior post100%0%0%0%
Unconventional communication pre0%75%0%25%
Unconventional communication post0%87.5%12.5%0%
Conventional communication pre0%7%21%72%
Conventional communication post0%29%43%28%
Concrete symbols pre0%12%23.5%64.5%
Concrete symbols post0%23.5%57.2%19.3%
Abstract symbols pre0%23.5%23.5%53%
Abstract symbols post0%35.3%47%17.5%
Language pre0%0%35.5%64.5%
Language post0%0%41.2%68.8%
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Moraleda-Sepulveda, E.; Pulido-García, N.; Loro-Vicente, N.; Santos-Muriel, N. Effectiveness of Intensive Linguistic Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Case Study. Children 2025, 12, 182. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020182

AMA Style

Moraleda-Sepulveda E, Pulido-García N, Loro-Vicente N, Santos-Muriel N. Effectiveness of Intensive Linguistic Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Case Study. Children. 2025; 12(2):182. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020182

Chicago/Turabian Style

Moraleda-Sepulveda, Esther, Noelia Pulido-García, Nadia Loro-Vicente, and Noelia Santos-Muriel. 2025. "Effectiveness of Intensive Linguistic Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Case Study" Children 12, no. 2: 182. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020182

APA Style

Moraleda-Sepulveda, E., Pulido-García, N., Loro-Vicente, N., & Santos-Muriel, N. (2025). Effectiveness of Intensive Linguistic Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Case Study. Children, 12(2), 182. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020182

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop