Next Article in Journal
Reference Values of Absolute and Relative Handgrip Strength in Chilean Schoolchildren with Intellectual Disabilities
Previous Article in Journal
Study on Nutritional Knowledge, Attitude and Behavior of Chinese School Football Players
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Adaptive Behavior in Slovak Children with Intellectual Disability in Institutional Care

by
Margaréta Hapčová
1,
Hana Celušáková
2,
Daniela Turoňová
1,
Michaela Souček Vaňová
3,
Lenka Besedová
4,
Diana Demkaninová
1 and
Katarína Babinská
2,*
1
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, Comenius University, 811 02 Bratislava, Slovakia
2
Institute of Physiology, Faculty of Medicine, Comenius University, 813 72 Bratislava, Slovakia
3
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, University of Matej Bel, 974 11 Banská Bystrica, Slovakia
4
Center for Educational and Psychological Counseling and Prevention, 811 04 Bratislava, Slovakia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2022, 9(12), 1911; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121911
Submission received: 20 October 2022 / Revised: 25 November 2022 / Accepted: 2 December 2022 / Published: 6 December 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Mental Health)

Abstract

:
This study aimed to analyze the adaptive skills of children with intellectual disabilities in institutional care. We focused on communication, socialization, daily living skills and their relationship with risk factors, and institutional care. Our sample included 197 children aged 5–18 years (M = 12.8, SD = 2.97), 50% boys, with IQ < 85 placed in different types and lengths of stay in institutional care. There were 17% that presented with borderline intellectual functioning (IQ 84–87) and 83% that had intellect disabilities. Adaptive behavior (AB) was assessed by Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (VABS-3). The BIF and Mild ID groups did not differ in Socialization. The profile of adaptive behavior for BIF and Mild ID was Daily Living Skills > Communication > Socialization, and for Moderate and Severe ID, Socialization > Daily Living Skills > Communication. Longer institutional care was associated with lower competencies in AB. Gender differences were found, females overperformed males in Socialization, Daily Living Skills, and ABC score. Levels of ID, gender, length of stay in institutional care, and neonatal difficulties were significant predictors in the model which explain the 63% variance of AB. The practical implications of the results are discussed related to the assessment of ID, prevention, and care for institutionalized children.

1. Introduction

When reflecting on the adaptive skills of children with intellectual disabilities (ID) who are in institutional care, it is necessary to consider various factors. They include intellectual disabilities that are associated with the decline in adaptive behavior, their level depending on the ID severity [1,2,3]. The presence of co-occurring conditions is associated with changes in adaptive behavior [4,5,6]; furthermore, maltreatment before being placed in institutional care could have a negative impact on current adaptive behavior [7,8,9]. Finally, current institutional care is associated with specific conditions that may have a negative impact on adaptive behavior [4,10,11].
Adaptive behavior (AB) refers to abilities and skills that people learn throughout life and that allow them to function independently in society. AB is age-appropriate behavior, defined according to the expectations or standards of society [12]. The level of adaptive behavior reflects how much support a person needs in order to lead an independent life. The complexity of adaptive behavior increases with age [13,14,15], but the development of adaptive skills may not be uniform in all children. Gender differences have been described in several studies, but they explain only a small amount of variance. Moreover, it seems that they occur at different ages in different domains of adaptive behavior. On the other hand, girls with neurodevelopmental conditions (e.g., autism spectrum disorder—ASD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder—ADHD) appear to have higher adaptive scores than their male peers [16,17]. The educational and social environment provides a stimulus and an opportunity to acquire skills [18].
It has been shown that AB is tightly related to intellectual disability (ID). It is assumed that the level of AB declines with decreasing intellectual functioning [3]. In accordance with DSM-5 and ICD-11 criteria, intellectual disability is defined by a decrease in intellectual and adaptive functioning two or more standard deviations of the score below the mean [2,3]. The ID severity level is classified based on the degree of support needed [2]. According to the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine [19], individuals with a mild level of ID represent the largest subgroup of ID (about 85%). Populations with mild disabilities are generally slower in conceptual development, social skills, and daily functioning skills. They can learn practical skills that will allow them to live independently or with minimal support. This group can achieve literacy and partial economic and social independence in adulthood [20], but they are at risk of burnout, especially with full-time work and low job satisfaction [21]. The development of individuals with a moderate level of ID (making up 10% of ID) varies but it is generally limited to basic skills. Considerable and consistent support is needed for their independent functioning in adulthood. With support, they can travel to familiar places, and they can take care of themselves, their safety, and their health [3,19,22]. A person with a severe level of ID (making up 3.5% of ID) needs extensive support for daily activities. Language and capacity for the acquisition of academic skills are limited. Basic self-care skills may be acquired with intensive training, and motor impairments occur frequently. Individuals with a profound level of ID (making up 1.5% of ID) need intensive support for every aspect of daily routines. Their communication skills are limited, and motor or sensory impairments are frequent [3].
Intellectual functioning is more a continuum of abilities than a categorical variable, similar to AB. Borderline intellectual functioning (BIF) is at the border between normal intellectual functioning and intellectual disability. The intelligence quotient (IQ) is between 70 and 85 points. BIF is not a diagnosis in DSM or ICD, but it includes a heterogeneous group of specific neurodevelopmental syndromes, disorders, and extreme variations of normality [23]. These individuals do not meet the criteria for Disorders of Intellectual Development (DID). Their adaptive behavior varies, and some people with BIF can manage several areas of life without a need for support. Nevertheless, due to their impairments in cognitive and executive functions, AB, academic and social skills, and more mental health problems [24], their outcome is dependent on education, social connections, and personality [24]. People with BIF cannot gain access to support and services that are given to persons with DID, which makes their success in society difficult [25].
Institutionalization is the most common societal intervention for orphaned, abandoned, or maltreated children throughout the world [26]. In most cases, the reasons for placing children in institutional care are poverty and maltreatment, along with the hope to improve access to health care and education [27]. Children with disabilities are over-represented in institutions worldwide; however, the exact number of children in institutions, in general, but also specifically with disabilities or ID, is unknown [27,28]. Institutional care is also associated with increased rates of mental health problems. It is estimated that 50% of children have mental disorders [11]. Children with a history of institutional care/foster care show a higher rate of psychiatric symptoms in youth, including depression, anxiety, somatization, dissociation, and the symptom dimensions of posttraumatic stress disorder, while the impact of institutional care is more deleterious when compared to foster care [10,29,30]. Institutionalized children usually have a variety of genetic, pre-, peri- and postnatal risk factors. These risk factors are often associated with lower intellectual functioning [31]. More challenged children have a lower chance to be adopted and raised in foster families, and they stay longer in institutional care. All children in institutions are vulnerable [32] but children with disabilities are at higher risk of maltreatment [27,28,33], especially children with physical impairments are at risk of sexual abuse [34]. In recent years, several studies focused on AB in children or adolescents with maltreatment or placed in institutional care have been published. In the study of Viezel, Lowell and Davis [7], the authors have shown that neglected children had lower scores of AB in all domains (Communication, Daily Living Skills, Socialization) than abused children or children in the control group, and abused children had lower scores than the control group [7,35]. Deprivation is associated with lower cognitive abilities, especially verbal aspects [29,36,37], and poor social skills. Mediating, also moderating function between adversity and social problems is a disruption of reward processing [38,39].
A specific factor influencing AB of children in institutional care is a risk of cumulative lifetime traumatization, ongoing maltreatment in an institution, or institution-specific adverse effects, that may result in psychosocial deficits as a consequence of the institution’s care [30]. That includes frequent changing of caregivers, numbers of children in group care, presence of children with disability in the same group, not enough attention for an individual’s needs, separation from other social environments (e.g., allocated education group at living place), and lack of potential supporting systems (hobby, sports, peer groups, community life) [30,40]. Together with early life adverse circumstances, children in institutional care are at risk of worse outcomes in adulthood [40]. We assume that these factors also have a negative effect on adaptive behavior.
Intellectual disabilities are another significant predictor of mental health problems in children in institutional care [11,41,42]. One of the well-documented differences compared to children from families in the literature is the lower opportunity to create a secure attachment with a consistent caregiver [43,44]. Secure attachment is an important factor for individual resilience and positive adaptation [45]. Children with reactive attachment disorder and/or disinhibited social engagement disorder have significantly different abilities in the socialization and motor domain [46]. Becker-Weidman [47] reports a significant difference in the chronological and developmental age of children in foster care with a reactive relationship disorder. The most delayed area was communication (according to VABS-II). In addition to predominantly insecure attachment and difficulty recognizing emotions, these children are generally more lagging in cognitive development [43,44]. Furthermore, we can assume that cognitive abilities mediate the relationship between neglect, deprivation, and AB.
The aim of this study is to analyze the adaptive skills of a sample of children with intellectual disabilities in institutional care in Slovakia. The analysis reflects differences in adaptive domains related to intellectual functioning, gender, length of stay in institutional care, and type of institutional care. In the prediction model, we analyze the relationship of specific factors related to institutional care and pre-, peri- and postnatal risk factors in the development of AB. We included the level of intellectual functioning in the model also due to the assumed associations with other predictors that are specific for the children in institutional care. The age at institutionalization and length of stay may be indirectly linked with the severity of the ID and associated medical (somatic) conditions that have an impact on the AB [48]. In addition, some of the risk factors that we analyzed are also associated with the level of ID [31].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

Our sample consisted of 197 children aged 5.3–18.8 years (mean = 12.8, SD = 2.97), of that 50% (n = 98) were boys. The inclusive criterion was intellect lower than IQ 85. In our sample, 16.8% of children had borderline IQ (84–70), and the remaining children had a diagnosed DID by ICD-10 criteria: of that, 56.8% were mild, 19.8% moderate, and 6.6% severe. Children with a profound level were excluded from further analysis due to their low count (2). All children were in institutional care in various forms (14.2% in professional foster care, 62.4% in group-based care, 18.3% in group-based care for children with disabilities, 5.1% in group-based for children with mental disorders). Children were placed in institutional care most often in preschool and younger school age (range 0–17 years old, mean 6.7 years), with a severe level of ID 3–4 years earlier than others. The mean length of stay in institutional care was 6.1 years (SD = 4.4), and the duration was the longest in children with a severe level of ID (M = 10.23 years, SD = 3.32 years). The ethnic composition of the sample was 83% Slovak, 14% Roma, and 3% Hungarian.
IQ of the children was examined for estimation of the child’s educational needs. IQ testing was performed by trained psychologists—diagnosticians from educational and psychological counseling institutions for children with special needs. Intellect was examined using standardized methods available in Slovakia (WISC-III, SON-R, S-B IV, K-ABC, Raven progressive matrices).
In the sample, children were diagnosed with the following co-occurring conditions: 4.6% (9) autism spectrum disorder (ASD), 3.6% (6) Down syndrome, 19.1% (18) cerebral palsy, 26.4% (52) communication disorders, 39.1% (77) conduct disorder, 16.2% (32) attention deficit and hyperactivity disorders (ADHD), 1.5% (3) SLD (specific learning disorders), 7.1% (14) anxiety disorder, 8.6% (17) depressive disorder, 4.1% (8) hearing impairment (different levels), 8.6% (17) visual impairment (different levels), and 10.2% (13) epilepsy. Characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 1.

2.2. Methods

Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition (VABS-3) is a standardized measure of adaptive behavior [12]. We used the Slovak research version of the Comprehensive Form for parents/caregivers that was filled in by staff (caregivers) at children’s homes. VABS-3 assesses three domains of AB—Communication, Daily Living Skills, Socialization, and global AB Composite score ABC. Each domain includes 3 subdomains.
Communication domain: Receptive—attending and understanding and responding appropriately; Expressive—using words and sentences to express oneself verbally to others; Written—using reading and writing skills.
Daily Living Skills domain: Personal—self-sufficiency in such areas as eating, dressing, washing, hygiene, and health care; Domestic—performing household tasks such as cleaning up after oneself, chores, and food preparation; Community—functioning in the world outside the home, including safety, using money, travel, rights, and responsibilities, practically using numeric concepts—time, dates, and money.
Socialization domain: Interpersonal relationship—responding and relating to others, including friendship, caring, social appropriateness, and conversation; Play/Leisure—engaging in play and fun activities with others; Coping skills—demonstrating behavior and emotional control in different situations involving others.
Higher scores in domains and subdomains indicate better adaptive behaviors. Slovak normative data are not available; therefore, American normative data were used for the transformation of raw data to a standard score.
Anamnestic questionnaire. Caregivers (staff) in institutional care or professional foster parents filled in an anamnestic questionnaire regarding demographic information, risk factors in development, and information about institutional care—length, and type of institutional care (professional foster care vs. group-based care). Information on risk factors was obtained from children’s documentation. Due to the large heterogeneity of data in the quality of information, we decided to merge some variables into umbrella variables. An explanation of individual risk factors is summarized below.
Substance use during pregnancy—alcohol and opioid use during pregnancy. It is not necessary for the child to have symptoms of neonatal abstinence syndrome (NAS) or fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS).
Neonatal infection—in utero infection such as syphilis, toxoplasmosis, CMV, hepatitis B, etc.
Prematurity—we did not differentiate the degree of prematurity. We considered all children born before the 38th week of gestation to be premature.
Labor complications—information in health documentation about any complication during labor, such as fetal distress, perinatal asphyxia, fetal injury, umbilical cord problems, etc.
Neonatal difficulties—every serious somatic complication after birth in the first 28 days, e.g., necrotizing enterocolitis, neonatal pneumonia, sepsis, hemorrhagic disease, and congenital heart defects.
Neglect of early health care (Neglect of EHC)—omitting regular preventive examinations and professional examinations in the first three years of a child’s life, that would lead to early identification of somatic and/or developmental problems. Omission of recommended treatment procedures—e.g., rehabilitation.
Maltreatment—includes all types of physical and/or emotional maltreatment, sexual abuse, and neglect in the child’s past.

2.3. Data Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS Version 22 Statistics for Windows, and Jamovi [49]. Normality tests were performed using histogram, Q-Q plot, Skewness, and Kurtosis. A one-way ANOVA and chi-square analysis were used to confirm that there were no differences between the four groups with respect to age and gender. Basic data analysis included Pearson correlations (VABS scores and age, VABS scores and Length of stay), and Independent Samples t-test (VABS scores and gender). One-way ANOVA (Fisher’s statistics, Welch statistics, Games–Howell post hoc test) was conducted to examine differences in adaptive behavior between groups. A paired t-test was used for the analysis of differences between AB domains within groups. Hierarchical regression models in 4 steps were used to examine if independent variables (Level of ID, Gender, Length of stay, Type of institutional care, Substance use during pregnancy, Neonatal Difficulties, Neglect EHC, Prematurity, Labor complications, Neonatal infection, and Maltreatment) could be relevant predictors for AB (standardized ACB composite score of VABS).
In the analyses, we used V-scores (M = 15, SD = 3) for the subdomains and standard scores (M = 100, SD = 15) for the AB domains and the ABC composite score. Data were expressed as means (M) ± standard deviation (SD). A value of p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

3. Results

A one-way ANOVA (Fisher’s statistics) has shown no significant differences in age in the four groups F(3,193) = 0.71, p = 0.548, η2 = 0.011: BIF (M = 13.4, SD = 2.91), Mild ID (M = 12.6, SD = 2.87), Moderate ID (12.8, SD = 3.3), Severe ID (M = 13.1, SD = 2.96). A chi-square test of independence showed that there was no significant association between Gender and Level of ID, X2 = (3,197) = 2.74, p = 0.434, Cramer’s V = 0.118.

3.1. Comparison of Adaptive Behavior between Levels of ID

One-way ANOVA (Welch statistics, Games–Howell post hoc test) showed that there was a statistically significant difference in ABC score and all AB domains between groups (Level of ID) except for BIF and Mild ID groups in the Socialization domain (p = 0.265). With the increasing severity of ID, a progressive decline in AB standard scores was observed. Overall, the highest scores were achieved in all dimensions by children with BIF, and within the dimension, all children achieved the highest scores in Daily Living Skills (Table 2). Mean differences and post hoc analysis are reported in Appendix A Table A1.
We found very similar results when evaluating the subdomains (Table 3). Based on the results of the post hoc analysis (Games–Howell post hoc test), the groups differed significantly in all subdomains except for children with BIF and Mild ID. They differed significantly only in the subdomains Daily Living Skills—Personal (p = 0.007), Daily Living Skills—Community (p < 0.001), and Communication—Written (p < 0.001). In the other subdomains, children from these two groups achieved very similar results: Socialization—Interpersonal (p = 0.558), Socialization—Play/Leisure (p = 0.292), Socialization—Coping Skills (p = 0.760), Daily Living Skills—Domestic (p = 0.319), Communication—Receptive (p = 0.162), and Communication—Expressive (p = 0.200). Descriptive statistics of each subdomain, mean differences, and post hoc analysis are reported in Appendix A Table A2 and Table A3.

3.2. Profile of Adaptive Behavior for Each Level of ID

A profile of adaptive domains was analyzed for each level of ID (Figure 1). The paired-sample t-test was used for pairwise comparison of standard scores in domains of AB on each level of ID (in Appendix A Table A5).
The profile of AB in the BIF group was characterized by Daily Living Skills > Communication > Socialization. Children scored better in the domain Daily Living Skills (M = 85.4, SD = 9.67), as compared to Communication (M = 80.27, SD = 10.1). This difference was statistically significant, t(32) = −3.157, p = 0.003. The poorest adaptive abilities were observed in the Socialization domain (M = 77, SD = 9.43), in comparison to Daily Living Skills, and this difference was significant t(32) = 5.530, p < 0.001.
A mild level of ID displayed similar results as BIF with the best abilities in Daily Living Skills domain (M = 77.7, SD = 13.31), which was significantly different from the Communication (M = 74.48, SD = 11.76), t(111) = −3.60, p < 0.001. The score was lowest in the Socialization domain (M = 73.16, SD = 13.75), and this difference was statistically significant when compared to the Daily Living Skills t(111) = 4.75, p < 0.001.
The profile of AB in the group of children with a moderate level of ID was Socialization > Daily Living Skills > Communication. Differences between domains were statistically significant in: Communication (M = 52.41, SD = 20.61) and Daily Living Skills (M = 58.85, SD = 16.1), t(38) = −3.313, p = 0.002, and Communication vs. Socialization (M = 59, SD = 17.27) t(38) = −2.87, p = 0.007. Children with a severe level of ID had similar profiles as the group with Moderate ID: Socialization (M = 31.23, SD = 18.68) > Daily Living Skills (M = 27.92, SD = 8.4) > Communication (26.85, SD = 15.74). Differences between Communication and Socialization were statistically significant (t(12) = −3.156, p = 0.008). Other domains displayed no significant differences.
To provide more detailed information about the AB of children in institutional care, Figure 2 represents profiles for each level of ID in VABS-3 subdomains. Complete information about pairwise t-tests is shown in Appendix A (Table A4).
All groups had the same profile of competencies in the Communication domain: Expressive > Receptive > Written. However, the significance differs in individual levels of ID. In BIF and Moderate ID groups, no significant difference between receptive and expressive communication was found. None of the communication skills in the Severe ID group differed significantly from each other. The differences between other communication subdomains in each level of ID were significant.
In the Daily Living Skills domain, the profile of competencies was the same in all groups: Domestic > Personal > Community. Differences between skills were significant in all groups, except in BIF, where the difference between Domestic and Personal was not significant.
The Socialization domain had a uniform profile in each level of ID. In group BIF, Mild ID and Moderate ID was profile same: Play and Leisure > Interpersonal > Coping skills. Differences between each skill did not differ significantly except in the Moderate ID group, where Play and Leisure scored higher than Coping skills. Children with Severe ID had a different profile: Interpersonal = Coping skills > Play and Leisure. They had developed these skills at a very low level.

3.3. Gender Differences in Adaptive Behavior

Gender was related to several AB domain scores (Table 4). The independent t-test showed that girls overperformed boys in Socialization and Daily Living Skills domains and ABC score, with the biggest difference in Daily Living Skills—mean difference of 9.86, CI95% [4.53, 15.2]. Results indicate non-significant differences between girls and boys in Communication. Girls overperformed boys also in all subdomains except for Expressive. The effect was biggest in the Domestic subdomain with a mean difference of 2.31 CI95% [1.21, 3.40].

3.4. Hierarchical Multiple Regression Model

A hierarchical multiple regression model was performed to examine the relationship between 11 independent variables (Level of ID, Gender, Type of institutional care, Length of stay in institutional care, Substance use during pregnancy, Neonatal Difficulties, Neglect EHC, Prematurity, Labor complications, Neonatal infection, Maltreatment) and AB (standardized ACB composite score of VABS-3). Preliminary analyses were conducted to ensure no violation of the assumptions of normality, linearity, and homoscedasticity.
Figure 3 represents four hierarchical models.
In the first step, the level of ID accounted for 54.8% of the variance. The model was statistically significant F(1,142) = 172.44, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.548.
In the second step, Gender accounted for an additional 2% of variance. The model was statistically significant F(2,141) = 91.53, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.565, R2Adjusted = 0.559). Both Level of ID (p < 0.001) and Gender (p = 0.022) were significantly associated with AB.
After entry of Length of stay in institutional care and Type of institutional care at Step 3, the total variance explained by the model was 59.9%, F(4,139) = 51.98, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.599, R2Adjusted = 0.588. Only the Level of ID (p < 0.001), Gender (p = 0.025), and Length of stay (p = 0.007) significantly contributed to the model.
After the entry of risk factors in the development (Neonatal Difficulties, Neglect of early health care, Prematurity, Labor complications, Neonatal infection, Substance use during pregnancy, Maltreatment), the total variance explained by the model was 63.6%, F(11,132) = 20.935, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.636, R2Adjusted = 0.605.
In the final model, 4 out of 11 predictor variables were statistically significant, including the Level of ID (p < 0.001), Gender (p = 0.011), Neonatal Difficulties (p = 0.029), and Length of stay in institutional care (p = 0.009).

4. Discussion

The aim of the study was to analyze AB in children with ID and BIF in institutional care. We focused on a specific vulnerable group of children who depend on the support of the social system much more than children who have ID and grow up in families.

4.1. Differences of AB in Severity Levels of ID and BIF Groups

We analyzed AB in the different levels of ID and BIF groups. In our sample, a significant difference was present in the domains of AB and in ABC scores, which supports previous findings of decreasing AB with decreasing levels of ID [1,24,25,50,51]. BIF and Mild ID groups differed in Communication and Daily Living Skills but not in Socialization. Compared to normative data of ABC composite score and AB domains, the BIF and Mild ID group scored in the range of 1 to 2 SD below the mean [12]. Significant limitations in AB are one of the criteria for a diagnosis of ID, which means an AB score that is “approximately two standard deviations below the mean in one of the three adaptive skills areas” [2].
Children in our sample were diagnosed according to ICD-10 criteria (in that time being the official classification system), where the assessment of adaptive behavior was not one of the essential criteria of ID. The severity level of ID was determined mainly by the result of the IQ test. If the ICD-11 criteria were applied, it is possible that some of the children who are in the Mild ID group would no longer meet these criteria [52]. Another explanation of the small differences between BIF and Mild ID groups may be the relationship between AB and IQ itself. This relationship, however, tended to decrease as IQ increased [51]. Not everyone with ID must also present significant deficits in AB, and conversely, those with significant deficits in AB may not also have ID (e.g., children with ASD and average intellect) [53]. Next, stronger correlations between intelligence and adaptive behavior are expected in younger children, especially children in the preschool period [51]. The mean age of our sample was 12.8 years.
In a more detailed analysis of the results in the BIF and Mild ID groups, a significant difference was observed only in some subdomains. The Written subdomain reflects the quality of reading, writing, and working with information. These skills are closely linked to cognitive processes and require more executive control than receptive and expressive communication. We expected the Mild ID group to have a weaker cognitive capacity to solve more complex tasks than the BIF group. However, the quality of reading and writing also depends on the method of education and stimulation. In Slovakia, most children with Mild ID are educated in schools for children with special needs, separated from other children (without ID) [54]. These schools are not a tool for temporary support but represent a permanent separate educational path, that has an extremely negative impact on some groups of children and limits their opportunities to achieve a complete primary education [54,55].
Despite the similar profile of AB in the BIF and Mild ID groups, we found significant differences in the Personal subdomain that reflects self-care, such as feeding, dressing, hygiene habits, and health care. Children with BIF scored in the range up to 1 SD below the mean and overperformed the Mild ID group. Similarly, the BIF group scored better in the subdomain Community, which reflects skills to function outside the home, e.g., using money, traveling, respecting others’ rights, etc.
Not all children with BIF have problems with adaptive behavior. However, both BIF and Mild ID groups are at greater risk of social isolation. Problems in contact with peers are very common, and social challenges are similar for both groups [24]. Although the cognitive potential of children with BIF is better, they seem to be exposed to greater psychosocial issues than children with Mild ID. Children with BIF have more mental health problems and more complicated family backgrounds [56]. These factors may contribute to the explanation of the similarity of the quality of Socialization in both groups, which ranged from 1 to 2 SD below the average.
Another possible explanation for the small differences between BIF and Mild ID is the specificity of children in institutional care. The care for these children was neglected during their sensitive developmental period. The conflicting relationship between the caregivers and the children is in negative relation to children’s adaptive skills [9]. Maltreatment has long-term consequences for the children’s development, also for AB [7,35]. Children with BIF and Mild ID were placed in institutional care at the time (M = 7.5 years) when their primary education begins (after reaching 6 years). It has been shown that neglect and a low-stimulating environment can lead to problems in the assessment of intellect, also in making decisions about special needs education. Children with BIF also have poorer access to the support system than children with ID, so sometimes conclusions of assessment can be related to access to this help [25].

4.2. Profiles of AB in Severity Levels of ID and BIF Groups

BIF and Mild ID groups had similar profiles of adaptive domains with the best performance in Daily Living Skills, followed by Communication, and weakest in the Socialization domain. Daily Living Skills were approximately 1 SD below the mean in the BIF group and 1.5 SD below the mean in the Mild ID group. Scores in the Domestic subdomain were highest in both the Mild ID and BIF groups. In our sample, 15% of children with BIF and 18% with Mild ID were placed in professional foster care, which is closest to family life. There were 79% of children with BIF and 70% with Mild ID placed in group-based care. Children in group-based care live in a household with a maximum of 8 children and are cared for by 3–4 alternating caregivers. Children in group-based care are involved in shopping, cleaning up after themselves, chores, food preparation, and cooking, helping younger children. The weakest area in the Daily Living Skills is Community in all groups in our sample. The mean score in BIF was −1 SD, and in the Mild ID group, it was below −2 SD. Problems include difficulties searching for information online, using communication technologies, time and date orientation, money, travel, and traffic orientation, safety behavior, etc. We assume that the poorer score in this subdomain is due to institutional care, which limits children’s ability to function independently in the world outside the home. Overall, we can consider Daily Living Skills as a strength of children in institutional care [57].
On the contrary, Socialization is manifested in the BIF and Mild ID groups as the weakest area; their average values were between 1 and 2 SD below the mean. Socialization may have been impaired due to maltreatment or early adverse experience. Maltreatment can impact children’s coping skills, attachment, social functioning, and psychopathology. The quality of socialization is closely connected with challenging behavior and mental health problems [38,39,58,59]. Intellectual disabilities are a significant predictor of mental health problems in children in institutional care [11,42]. A stay in institutional care can also have a negative effect on the quality of relationships [43,44].
In the Communication domains, the Written subdomain was the weakest for both groups and in all severity levels of ID. Reading and writing are very complex cognitive processes sensitive to a decline in cognitive abilities. For children with ID, however, reading can be an area of relative weakness but there is evidence that it can also be a strength [60].
A different pattern was observed in the Moderate and Severe ID groups. They reached the best scores in the Socialization domain, followed by the Daily Living Skills, and the poorest performance was in the Communication domain. Moderate and Severe ID are often associated with genetic factors and somatic, motor, and sensory impairments that negatively impact AB. In the Moderate ID group, Socialization was at a similar level as Daily Living Skills, i.e., 3 SD below the average. This group had a significant decline in the Written subdomain in the Communication domain, similar to all the other groups. In the Severe ID group, the decrease in AB in all domains is 5 SD below the mean. Persons with Severe ID typically display impairment in motor domains and significant deficits in communication [2], which we can also see in our sample.
When the profile of AB is analyzed, the co-occurring disorders and neonatal risk factors should be considered, as they may have a different impact on cognitive functions and may lead to varying levels of intellectual disability [61,62,63,64,65,66]. Furthermore, the stability of the AB profile for individuals is different. For example, with the Fragile X chromosome, there may be a decrease in AB over time. However, females and males have different trajectories [61]. On the contrary, at Prader Willi syndrome, the AB remains stable over time [67]. Our sample included a relatively large age range (5–18 years) and is heterogeneous in terms of the presence of co-occurring disorders (Down syndrome, ASD, SLD, ADHD) or syndromes (cerebral palsy). Therefore, the AB profiles for a single ID level represent a certain simplification of our result.

4.3. Gender Differences in AB

We observed gender differences in Daily Living Skills, Socialization and ABC score by 1 SD. In all these areas, the girls overperformed the boys. Some studies that include typically developing children and children with neurodevelopmental disorders also point to gender differences in adaptive behavior [17,57,68,69,70]. Differences are often in Communication domains, where girls, both in the general population and with disorders, have better results [17]. In our group, girls scored higher in Communication, but the difference was not significant. Significant differences were observed only in the subdomains Receptive and Written.
Our sample’s most significant gender differences were in Daily Living Skills and its subdomain Domestic. It may also be related to the cultural context, different expectations, and attitudes toward raising boys and girls. We assume that girls are more involved in the household, such as cooking, cleaning, etc. [18]. We presume that institutional care is likely to copy social trends.
In a longitudinal study of Japanese children from the general population, gender differences were observed in the Daily Living Skills and Socialization domains, especially in children with lower intelligence; on the contrary, gender differences were not observed in children with average intelligence. These differences persisted over time [17].
The VABS-3 instrument does not consider gender differences [15]. The different trajectories for girls and boys suggest that assessing adaptive behavior requires consideration of the child’s gender. It may play an essential role in the early identification of children with deficits (e.g., ASD) [17].

4.4. Predictors of AB

The level of ID accounted for 54.8% of the variance in AB. Correspondingly with our assumptions, it was the most significant predictor. Gender accounted for an additional 2% of variance. In the next model, we analyzed specific predictors of AB related to institutional care. The type of care, i.e., comparison of professional foster care (close to life in a family with the primary caregiver) with group-based care (in which the child grows up in a group of several children without a primary caregiver) was not a significant predictor. Only the Length of stay in institutional care was a significant predictor for AB. This factor accounted for an additional 3.1% of variance of the AB. The Length of stay in institutional care was weakly negatively correlated with the quality of AB, similar to the results of a previous study with infants and toddlers in institutional care [57]. The longest stay was observed in children with Severe ID (mean ten years,) and at the same time, they were placed in institutional care at the youngest age (before three years of their age) compared to other groups (range of 6–7 years old). Often, these are children who also have other somatic complications and diseases. Therefore, there is only a small probability that these children will return to their biological families, or that they will be adopted. In a meta-analysis by Goemans and van Geel and Vedder [71], it was pointed out that children do not improve their AB while in foster care. Some studies have shown that a long-term stay in foster care had a negative impact on AB [71]. Institutional care can only partially “catchup” with lagging in development. The specifics of institutional care also negatively affect the development of a child [72]. Difficulties in developing a secure attachment, frequent changes of caregivers, and a large group of children with whom they grow up, can negatively affect development and bring specific adverse experiences to the child [30,40,57]. The type of institutional care was not a significant factor in predicting AB in our sample. Therefore, the assumption that staying in professional foster care compared to group-based care could have a better effect on AB, was not confirmed. Our study did not monitor how long the child was in this type of care. During the stay, children often alternate the types of care, and professional parents can also alternate. The stability of the environment in foster care is one of the most critical factors that can affect a child’s development and well-being [42].
Risks in development (Neonatal Difficulties, Neglect of early health care, Prematurity, Labor complications, Neonatal infection, Substance use during pregnancy, Maltreatment) accounted for an additional 3.4% variance of AB in our sample. Risk factors better predicted the level of intellect than AB alone [30]. Circumstances during childbirth or just after birth are often associated with an increased risk for neurodevelopmental difficulties or cognitive deficits [61,65,73,74,75]. However, the factors we followed are not very specific for AB outcome and we assume only their indirect effect on AB.
In conclusion, the quality of AB results from several factors that influence the child’s development. We were able to explain 63% of the variance by combining various factors related to cognitive abilities, the quality of institutional care, and risk factors in the child’s development.

4.5. Implications and Limits of the Study

Our results highlight the difficulties of children with BIF in AB. BIF and Mild ID groups are close in terms of AB and both groups have similar challenges. This result emphasizes the need to provide systematic help even to children who do not meet the diagnostic criteria for ID. These are the groups of children who have the best perspective on becoming independent after leaving institutionalized care. The transition to independence in adulthood is often difficult, many children do not have sufficient skills [76]. The area that requires special attention is the functioning outside the home, where the Mild ID and BIF groups had the greatest deficits in their abilities.
Another important result of our study is the gender differences in AB in children with ID and BIF. This supports the need to have adequate gender-specific AB assessment tools [17].
The results of this study can be applied in social prevention, gynecology, and primary pediatrics. Early identification of at-risk mothers and their children, and intervention might reduce the negative impact on children’s development. Prevention should also focus on families of children with ID and BIF, that are exposed to complex problems [77]. Problems in the family context increase the likelihood of adverse childhood experiences and this in turn increases the likelihood of the child being placed in institutional care [78]. Therefore, it is important to focus on the high-risk children with pre-, peri- and postnatal difficulties and provide them with early health and social intervention, which has the potential to reduce the risk of adverse circumstances in the child’s development. In addition to social support, for example, support for parenting skills can lead to better health care for the child.
Identifying the at-risk mothers could help to reduce the harm for the mother, but also to her child [79]. Even though there are no consistent findings regarding the impact of a parent’s addiction on a child’s outcome, an intervention program focused on the relationship between a child and a parent has the potential to improve parents’ sensitivity to the child, the ability to apply appropriate parenting strategies that have a long-term effect on the child’s development [80,81]. By supporting the parents and the at-risk mothers, we can prevent the placement of children in institutional care.
Our study has several limitations that reduce the possibility of generalizing our results. The splitting of the sample into groups according to the level of ID was only based on anamnestic data. As we have seen, the results of the BIF and Mild ID groups were very close, which could be due to the inaccurate diagnosis of intellectual disorders. We were missing several pieces of information about the development, health, and social background of the children that would have helped to refine our analyses. For example, prematurity has different degrees that we could not distinguish due to the lack of detailed data and thus there might have been interfering health complications with an impact on AB. Many children in institutional care lack comprehensive information about their health status, also about their psychosocial development. The unavailability of this information is linked to the reasons why children end up in institutional care.
We did not consider the presence of comorbidity, which can affect the outcome of AB. For example, it has been shown that in ASD or ADHD, the presence of these disorders reduces the quality of AB. In the future, it would be appropriate to analyze AB not only according to the level of ID but also to work with specific groups, e.g., children with Down syndrome, ASD, ADHD, etc. Analysis of such profiles could provide more specific information about the needs of these children. Our results have a cross-sectional design, which limits making specific predictions. Prospective monitoring of children in institutional care would make it possible to better identify the factors that have an impact on AB and thus apply appropriate interventions.
In the analyses, we worked with a standard score that is derived from American normative data. There is no information about the Slovak population against which it would be possible to compare these children. Distinct environments have different expectations for age-related adaptive abilities [12,82,83]; therefore, we need adequate normative data to assess AB. Research findings on Slovak children or adolescents are still marginal [52].
We also needed to consider the Slovak context, such as the specifics of Slovak institutional care (e.g., organization of care), the poor quality of assessment tools (availability and out of date) used in the diagnostics process of ID, specifics in the education of children with disabilities (e.g., limited inclusiveness).

5. Conclusions

This study provided cross-sectional information on the adaptive behavior of children with ID and BIF in institutional care. Profiles for the level of ID and BIF were described. We have identified gender differences, which are important for assessment and practices. The strongest predictor of AB was the level of ID. Risk factors in development and factor associated with institutional care were not very specific for AB outcome and we assume only their indirect effect on AB.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.H., H.C. and D.T.; data curation, M.H., H.C. and D.T.; writing—original draft preparation, M.H., M.S.V. and L.B.; writing—review and editing, K.B. and H.C.; visualization, D.D.; formal analysis, D.T.; project administration, M.H. and L.B.; supervision, K.B.; funding acquisition, K.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by The Slovak Research and Development Agency grant scheme (APVV), grant number APVV-20-0139.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived. The study was conducted in collaboration with the Slovak Central Office of Labor, Social Affairs and Family (COLSAF), which monitored the mental health of children in institutional care. COLSAF is an organization created and coordinated by the Slovak Ministry of Labor, Social Affairs and Family of the Slovak Republic According to Slovak legislation this governmental organization has the authority to collect, store and analyze these types of data. Our study processed only a completely anonymized data set.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from caregivers of all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

This research project was supported by Comenius University, Bratislava, Slovakia.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Post hoc comparisons of AB domain in different levels of ID.
Table A1. Post hoc comparisons of AB domain in different levels of ID.
ComparisonMean Differencedftp(Games-Howwel)
CommunicationBIF–Mild ID5.7960.12.790.035
BIF–Moderate ID27.957.17.46<0.001
BIF–Severe ID53.416.011.35<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID22.146.96.34<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID47.613.610.57<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID25.626.94.67<0.001
Daily Living SkillsBIF–Mild ID7.7771.33.700.002
BIF–Moderate ID26.663.78.67<0.001
BIF–Severe ID57.625.220.3<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID18.957.26.59<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID49.819.818.81<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID30.940.28.91<0.001
SocializationBIF–Mild ID3.8476.01.830.265
BIF–Moderate ID18.060.65.60<0.001
BIF–Severe ID45.814.58.42<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID14.255.74.64<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID41.913.67.85<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID27.819.34.73<0.001
ABCBIF–Mild ID4.4373.32.930.023
BIF–Moderate ID19.657.07.67<0.001
BIF–Severe ID48.514.311.76<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID15.251.16.23<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID44.113.310.88<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID28.920.56.35<0.001
Table A2. Descriptive of subdomains VABS-3 in each level of ID.
Table A2. Descriptive of subdomains VABS-3 in each level of ID.
Levels of ID NMeanSDMinimumMaximum
BIFReceptive3312.852.41618
Expressive3313.32.8817
Written339.732.58615
Personal3313.271.89918
Domestic3313.672.17917
Community3310.702.36715
Interpersonal3310.941.95715
Play/Leisure3311.61.87614
Coping Skills3310.792.50516
Mild IDReceptive11211.763.1117
Expressive11212.212.47317
Written1127.712.71116
Personal11211.942.39419
Domestic11212.593.25119
Community1128.542.93218
Interpersonal11210.132.92117
Play/Leisure11210.212.63116
Coping Skills11210.103.10117
Moderate IDReceptive397.494.54117
Expressive398.184.39116
Written393.873.16111
Personal398.233.62114
Domestic399.383.45416
Community394.413.2113
Interpersonal397.673.26215
Play/Leisure397.853.00113
Coping Skills397.133.74215
Severe IDReceptive132.383.40111
Expressive132.923.97112
Written131.230.6013
Personal131.541.4516
Domestic133.001.8717
Community131.541.12714
Interpersonal132.923.50111
Play/Leisure132.623.1519
Coping Skills132.922.84110
Note. V-score for subdomains (M = 15, SD = 3) according to normative data [1].
Table A3. Post hoc comparisons of AB subdomain in different levels of ID.
Table A3. Post hoc comparisons of AB subdomain in different levels of ID.
Comparison (Level of ID)Mean Differencedftp(Games-Howwel)
Communication—ReceptiveBIF–Mild ID1.964.22.150.149
BIF–Moderate ID5.3659.76.39<0.001
BIF–Severe ID10.4617.010.13<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID4.2750.25.48<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID9.3714.39.51<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID5.1027.44.28<0.001
Communication—ExpressiveBIF–Mild ID0.81660.91.890.242
BIF–Moderate ID4.8556.26.13<0.001
BIF–Severe ID10.1114.78.72<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID4.346.65.44<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID9.2913.18.26<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID5.2622.64.20.003
Communication—WrittenBIF–Mild ID2.0154.53.90<0.001
BIF–Moderate ID5.8669.98.65<0.001
BIF–Severe ID8.5039.417.76<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID3.8458.56.77<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID6.4884.721.26<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID2.6445.04.95<0.001
Daily Living Skills—PersonalBIF–Mild ID1.3464.93.340.007
BIF–Moderate ID5.0459.17.56<0.001
BIF–Severe ID11.7328.722.57<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID3.7150.05.95<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID10.4020.522.55<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID6.6948.19.48<0.001
Daily Living Skills—DomesticBIF–Mild ID1.0878.22.210.130
BIF–Moderate ID4.2865.16.40<0.001
BIF–Severe ID10.6725.516.61<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID3.2063.15.7<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID9.5921.615.90<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID6.3838.88.43<0.001
Daily Living Skills—CommunityBIF–Mild ID2.1663.64.36<0.001
BIF–Moderate ID6.2969.69.89<0.001
BIF–Severe ID9.1642.217.72<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID4.1364.57.40<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID7.0035.816.77<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID2.8749.24.98<0.001
Socialization—InterpersonalBIF–Mild ID0.80578.21.840.262
BIF–Moderate ID3.2763.55.26<0.001
BIF–Severe ID8.0215.07.80<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID2.4760.64.18<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID7.2114.07.15<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID4.7419.44.310.002
Socialization—Play/LeisureBIF–Mild ID0.85572.92.90.166
BIF–Moderate ID3.2164.75.53<0.001
BIF–Severe ID8.4515.59.6<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID2.3659.54.36<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID7.5914.08.36<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID5.2319.85.23<0.001
Socialization—Coping SkillsBIF–Mild ID0.69063.81.320.556
BIF–Moderate ID3.6666.74.95<0.001
BIF–Severe ID7.8619.78.74<0.001
Mild ID–Moderate ID2.9757.24.46<0.001
Mild ID–Severe ID7.1815.58.53<0.001
Moderate ID–Severe ID4.2127.04.25<0.001
Table A4. Paired-samples t-test for subdomains in domains of VABS-3 for each level of ID.
Table A4. Paired-samples t-test for subdomains in domains of VABS-3 for each level of ID.
Levels of ID StatisticdfpMean Differenced
BIFReceptive–Expressive−0.62320.540−0.182−0.11
Receptive–Written7.2532<0.0013.1211.27
Expressive–Written7.7332<0.0013.3031.51
Personal–Domestic−1.47320.151−0.394−0.26
Personal–Community6.8332<0.0012.5761.19
Domestic–Community6.8432<0.0012.9701.10
Interpersonal–Play/Leisure−0.42320.680−0.121−0.07
Interpersonal–Coping Skills0.47320.6390.1520.08
Play/Leisure–Coping Skills1.00320.3250.2730.17
Mild IDReceptive–Expressive−2.631110.010−0.4554−0.25
Receptive–Written15.1111<0.0014.04461.82
Expressive–Written20.94111<0.0014.001.89
Personal–Domestic−3.181110.002−0.6518−0.30
Personal–Community17.82111<0.0013.181.41
Domestic–Community16.51111<0.0014.05361.98
Interpersonal–Play/Leisure−0.351110.725−0.0714−0.03
Interpersonal–Coping Skills0.171110.8700.03570.02
Play/Leisure–Coping Skills0.541110.5940.10710.05
Moderate IDReceptive–Expressive−1.96380.057−0.692−0.31
Receptive–Written6.2438<0.0013.6150.99
Expressive–Written8.1138<0.0014.3081.83
Personal–Domestic−3.52380.001−1.154−0.56
Personal–Community10.4138<0.0013.8211.64
Domestic–Community17.5638<0.0014.9742.13
Interpersonal–Play/Leisure−0.59380.557−0.179−0.1
Interpersonal–Coping Skills1.52380.1360.5380.24
Play/Leisure–Coping Skills2.5380.0470.7180.33
Severe IDReceptive–Expressive−1.62120.131−0.538−0.45
Receptive–Written1.47120.1661.1540.41
Expressive–Written1.78120.1001.6920.49
Personal–Domestic−3.08120.010−1.462−0.85
Personal–Community0.00121.0000.0000.00
Domestic–Community3.96120.0021.4621.1
Interpersonal–Play/Leisure1.8120.3030.3080.3
Interpersonal–Coping Skills0.00121.0000.0000.00
Play/Leisure–Coping Skills−0.69120.502−0.308−0.19
Table A5. Mean differences of AB domains in each level of ID.
Table A5. Mean differences of AB domains in each level of ID.
AB DomainMean Differencet-testdfpCohen’s d
BIFCommunication–Daily Living Skills−5.21−3.157320.003−0.550
Communication–Socialization3.271.47320.1530.255
Daily Living Skills–Socialization8.485.5332<0.0010.963
Mild IDCommunication–Daily Living Skills−3.23−3.60111<0.001−0.340
Communication–Socialization1.321.091110.2780.103
Daily Living Skills–Socialization4.554.75111<0.0010.449
Moderate IDCommunication–Daily Living Skills−6.436−3.313380.002−0.531
Communication–Socialization−6.590−2.87380.007−0.460
Daily Living Skills–Socialization−0.154−0.115380.909−0.019
Severe IDCommunication–Daily Living Skills−1.08−0.313120.760−0.087
Communication–Socialization−4.38−3.156120.008−0.876
Daily Living Skills–Socialization−3.31−0.799120.440−0.222

References

  1. Tassé, M.J.; Balboni, G.; Navas, P.; Luckasson, R.; Nygren, M.A.; Belacchi, C.; Bonichini, S.; Reed, G.M.; Kogan, C.S. Developing Behavioural Indicators for Intellectual Functioning and Adaptive Behaviour for ICD-11 Disorders of Intellectual Development. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2019, 63, 386–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. World Health Organization International Classification of Diseases, Eleventh Revision (ICD-11). Available online: https://icd.who.int/en (accessed on 10 October 2022).
  4. Balboni, G.; Rebecchini, G.; Elisei, S.; Tassé, M.J. Factors Affecting the Relationship between Adaptive Behavior and Challenging Behaviors in Individuals with Intellectual Disability and Co-Occurring Disorders. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2020, 104, 103718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kanne, S.M.; Gerber, A.J.; Quirmbach, L.M.; Sparrow, S.S.; Cicchetti, D.V.; Saulnier, C.A. The Role of Adaptive Behavior in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Implications for Functional Outcome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2011, 41, 1007–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Weber, P.; Bolli, P.; Heimgartner, N.; Merlo, P.; Zehnder, T.; Kätterer, C. Behavioral and Emotional Problems in Children and Adults with Cerebral Palsy. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2016, 20, 270–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Viezel, K.; Lowell, A.; Davis, A.; Castillo, J. Differential Profiles of Adaptive Behavior of Maltreated Children. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 2014, 6, 574–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Herruzo, C.; Raya Trenas, A.; Pino, M.J.; Herruzo, J. Study of the Differential Consequences of Neglect and Poverty on Adaptive and Maladaptive Behavior in Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Crossnine, C.B. Examining Associations Between Trauma, The Parent-Child Relationship, and Children’s Functioning during Middle Childhood. Master’s Thesis, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN, USA, August 2021. [Google Scholar]
  10. Zeanah, C.H.; Egger, H.L.; Smyke, A.T.; Nelson, C.A.; Fox, N.A.; Marshall, P.J.; Guthrie, D. Institutional Rearing and Psychiatric Disorders in Romanian Preschool Children. Am. J. Psychiatry 2009, 166, 777–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bronsard, G.; Alessandrini, M.; Fond, G.; Loundou, A.; Auquier, P.; Tordjman, S.; Boyer, L. The Prevalence of Mental Disorders Among Children and Adolescents in the Child Welfare System: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Medicine 2016, 95, e2622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Sparrow, S.S.; Cicchetti, D.V.; Saulnier, C.A. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition (Vineland-3); Pearson: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  13. Tassé, M.J.; Schalock, R.L.; Balboni, G.; Bersani, H.; Borthwick-Duffy, S.A.; Spreat, S.; Thissen, D.; Widaman, K.F.; Zhang, D. The Construct of Adaptive Behavior: Its Conceptualization, Measurement, and Use in the Field of Intellectual Disability. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 117, 291–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Harrison, P.L.; Oakland, T. ABAS II [Kit]: Adaptive Behavior Assessment System; Psychological Corporation; Pearson: London, UK, 2003; ISBN 0-15-800451-5. [Google Scholar]
  15. Sparrow, S.S.; Balla, D.A.; Cicchetti, D.V. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition: Survey Forms Manual; AGS Publications: Crowley, TX, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  16. Oakland, T.; Harrison, P.L. Adaptive Behavior Assessment System-II: Clinical Use and Interpretation; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2011; ISBN 978-0-08-055978-0. [Google Scholar]
  17. Nishimura, T.; Kato, T.; Okumura, A.; Harada, T.; Iwabuchi, T.; Rahman, M.S.; Hirota, T.; Takahashi, M.; Adachi, M.; Kuwabara, H.; et al. Trajectories of Adaptive Behaviors During Childhood in Females and Males in the General Population. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 13, 817383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Selvam, S.; Thomas, T.; Shetty, P.; Zhu, J.; Raman, V.; Khanna, D.; Mehra, R.; Kurpad, A.V.; Srinivasan, K. Norms for Developmental Milestones Using VABS-II and Association with Anthropometric Measures among Apparently Healthy Urban Indian Preschool Children. Psychol. Assess. 2016, 28, 1634–1645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Boat, T.; Wu, J.T. Mental Disorders and Disabilities Among Low-Income Children; National Academies Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2015; ISBN 978-0-309-37688-4. [Google Scholar]
  20. Shapiro, B.K.; Batshaw, M. Intellectual Disability. In Children with Disabilities; Paul, H., Ed.; Brookes Publishing Company: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2019; pp. 249–265. ISBN 978-1-68125-320-6. [Google Scholar]
  21. Gutierrez-Martínez, I.; González-Santos, J.; Rodríguez-Fernández, P.; Jiménez-Eguizábal, A.; del Barrio-del Campo, J.A.; González-Bernal, J.J. Explanatory Factors of Burnout in a Sample of Workers with Disabilities from the Special Employment Centres (SEC) of the Amica Association, Spain. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Patel, M.; Bailey, R.K.; Jabeen, S.; Ali, S.; Barker, N.C.; Osiezagha, K. Postpartum Depression: A Review. J. Health Care Poor Underserved 2012, 23, 534–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Salvador-Carulla, L.; García-Gutiérrez, J.C.; Ruiz Gutiérrez-Colosía, M.; Artigas-Pallarès, J.; García Ibáñez, J.; González Pérez, J.; Nadal Pla, M.; Aguilera Inés, F.; Isus, S.; Cereza, J.M.; et al. Borderline Intellectual Functioning: Consensus and Good Practice Guidelines. Rev. Psiquiatr. Salud Ment. 2013, 6, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Peltopuro, M.; Ahonen, T.; Kaartinen, J.; Seppälä, H.; Närhi, V. Borderline Intellectual Functioning: A Systematic Literature Review. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2014, 52, 419–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Hassiotis, A.; Emerson, E.; Wieland, J.; Bertelli, M.O. Borderline Intellectual Functioning. In Textbook of Psychiatry for Intellectual Disability and Autism Spectrum Disorder; Bertelli, M.O., Deb, S.S., Munir, K., Hassiotis, A., Salvador-Carulla, L., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2022; pp. 95–106. ISBN 978-3-319-95720-3. [Google Scholar]
  26. Zeanah, C.H.; Humphreys, K.L. Global Prevalence of Institutional Care for Children: A Call for Change. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2020, 4, 343–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Duschinsky, R.; Fox, N.A.; Goldman, P.S.; Gunnar, M.R.; Johnson, D.E.; Nelson, C.A.; Reijman, S.; Skinner, G.C.M.; et al. Institutionalisation and Deinstitutionalisation of Children 1: A Systematic and Integrative Review of Evidence Regarding Effects on Development. Lancet Psychiatry 2020, 7, 703–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Barriga, S.R.; Buchanan, J.; Ćerimović, E.; Sharma, K. Children with Disabilities: Deprivation of Liberty in the Name of Care and Treatment. In Protecting Children Against Torture in Detention: Global Solutions; Human Rights Watch: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 285–302. [Google Scholar]
  29. Wade, M.; Wright, L.; Finegold, K.E. The Effects of Early Life Adversity on Children’s Mental Health and Cognitive Functioning. Transl. Psychiatry 2022, 12, 244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Lueger-Schuster, B.; Knefel, M.; Glück, T.M.; Jagsch, R.; Kantor, V.; Weindl, D. Child Abuse and Neglect in Institutional Settings, Cumulative Lifetime Traumatization, and Psychopathological Long-Term Correlates in Adult Survivors: The Vienna Institutional Abuse Study. Child Abus. Negl. 2018, 76, 488–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hapčová, M.; Turoňová, D.; Celušáková, H.; Demkaninová, D. Risk Factors during Development in Children with Intellectual Disability in Institutional Care. In Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the European Health Psychology Society, Bratislava, Slovakia, 23–27 August 2022; Comenius University: Bratislava, Slovakia, 2022; p. 693. [Google Scholar]
  32. World Health Organization. European Declaration on the Health of Children and Young People with Intellectual Disabilities and Their Families: Better Health, Better Lives: Children and Young People with Intellectual Disabilities and Their Families: Bucharest, Romania, 26–27 November 2010; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  33. Maclean, M.J.; Sims, S.; Bower, C.; Leonard, H.; Stanley, F.J.; O’Donnell, M. Maltreatment Risk Among Children with Disabilities. Pediatrics 2017, 139, e20161817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Mueller-Johnson, K.; Eisner, M.P.; Obsuth, I. Sexual Victimization of Youth with a Physical Disability: An Examination of Prevalence Rates, and Risk and Protective Factors. J. Interpers Violence 2014, 29, 3180–3206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Endrikat, E.G. Adaptive Behavior and Posttraumatic Symptomatology of Physically Neglected Children. PhD. Thesis, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, NJ, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  36. Bos, K.J.; Fox, N.; Zeanah, C.H.; Nelson Iii, C.A. Effects of Early Psychosocial Deprivation on the Development of Memory and Executive Function. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2009, 3, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Sheridan, M.A.; Peverill, M.; Finn, A.S.; McLaughlin, K.A. Dimensions of Childhood Adversity Have Distinct Associations with Neural Systems Underlying Executive Functioning. Dev. Psychopathol. 2017, 29, 1777–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Sheridan, M.A.; McLaughlin, K.A.; Winter, W.; Fox, N.; Zeanah, C.; Nelson, C.A. Early Deprivation Disruption of Associative Learning Is a Developmental Pathway to Depression and Social Problems. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 2216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Kasparek, S.W.; Jenness, J.L.; McLaughlin, K.A. Reward Processing Modulates the Association Between Trauma Exposure and Externalizing Psychopathology. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 8, 989–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Van Ijzendoorn, M.H.; Palacios, J.; Sonuga-Barke, E.J.S.; Gunnar, M.R.; Vorria, P.; McCall, R.B.; Le Mare, L.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Dobrova-Krol, N.A.; Juffer, F.I. Children in Institutional Care: Delayed Development and Resilience. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2011, 76, 8–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Tarren-Sweeney, M. Retrospective and concurrent predictors of the mental health of children in care. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2008, 30, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Dubois-Comtois, K.; Bussières, E.-L.; Cyr, C.; St-Onge, J.; Baudry, C.; Milot, T.; Labbé, A.-P. Are children and adolescents in foster care at greater risk of mental health problems than their counterparts? A meta-analysis. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2021, 127, 106100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Johnson, R.; Browne, K.; Hamilton-Giachritsis, C. Young Children in Institutional Care at Risk of Harm. Trauma Violence Abus. 2006, 7, 34–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Vorria, P.; Papaligoura, Z.; Sarafidou, J.; Kopakaki, M.; Dunn, J.; van Ijzendoorn, M.; Kontopoulou, A. The development of adopted children after institutional care: A follow-up study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2006, 47, 1246–1253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Rasmussen, P.D.; Storebø, O.J.; Løkkeholt, T.; Voss, L.G.; Shmueli-Goetz, Y.; Bojesen, A.B.; Simonsen, E.; Bilenberg, N. Attachment as a Core Feature of Resilience: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Rep. 2019, 122, 1259–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Giltaij, H.P.; Sterkenburg, P.S.; Schuengel, C. Adaptive behaviour, comorbid psychiatric symptoms, and attachment disorders. Adv. Ment. Health Intellect. Disabil. 2016, 10, 82–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Becker-Weidman, A. Effects of Early Maltreatment on Development: A Descriptive Study Using the Vineland Adaptive Be-havior Scales-II. Child Welf. 2009, 88, 137–161. [Google Scholar]
  48. Central Office of Labour, Social Affairs and Family. Annual Report about Providing Care and Education to Children in the Centers for Children and Families per Year 2021; Internal Report (Unpublished manuscript); COLSAF: Bratislava, Slovakia, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  49. Jamovi. The Jamovi Project, Version 1.6; Computer Software: Sydney, Australia, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  50. Tassé, M.J.; Luckasson, R.; Schalock, R.L. The Relation Between Intellectual Functioning and Adaptive Behavior in the Diagnosis of Intellectual Disability. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2016, 54, 381–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Alexander, R.M.; Reynolds, M.R. Intelligence and Adaptive Behavior: A Meta-Analysis. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 49, 85–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hapčová, M.; Demkaninová, D.; Besedová, L. Adaptive Behavior of Children with Disorders of Intellectual Development and His Correspondence with the Measured Level of IQ. Česká A Slov. Psychiatr. 2021, 117, 9–10. [Google Scholar]
  53. Tassé, M.J.; Balboni, G. Theories and Measurement of Adaptive Behavior. In APA Handbook of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities: Foundations; APA Handbooks in Psychology®; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2021; Volume 1, pp. 425–450. ISBN 978-1-4338-3195-9. [Google Scholar]
  54. Hall, R.; Dráľ, P.; Fridrichová, P.; Hapalová, M.; Lukáč, S.; Miškolci, J.; Vančíková, K. Analýza Zistení o Stave Školstva Na Slovensku: To Dá Rozum; MESA10: Bratislava, Slovakia, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  55. Ramberg, J.; Watkins, A. Exploring inclusive education across Europe: Some insights from the European Agency Statistics on Inclusive Education. FIRE: Forum Int. Res. Educ. 2020, 6, 85–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Nouwens, P.J.G.; Lucas, R.; Embregts, P.J.C.M.; van Nieuwenhuizen, C. In plain sight but still invisible: A structured case analysis of people with mild intellectual disability or borderline intellectual functioning. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2016, 42, 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Naumova, O.Y.; Rychkov, S.Y.; Kornilov, S.A.; Odintsova, V.V.; Anikina, V.; Solodunova, M.Y.; Arintcina, I.A.; Zhukova, M.A.; Ovchinnikova, I.V.; Burenkova, O.V.; et al. Effects of early social deprivation on epigenetic statuses and adaptive behavior of young children: A study based on a cohort of institutionalized infants and toddlers. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0214285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. McLaughlin, K.A.; DeCross, S.N.; Jovanovic, T.; Tottenham, N. Mechanisms linking childhood adversity with psychopathology: Learning as an intervention target. Behav. Res. Ther. 2019, 118, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. I Gerin, M.; Hanson, E.; Viding, E.; McCrory, E.J. A review of childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the brain: Implications for clinical practice and prevention. Adopt. Foster. 2019, 43, 310–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Di Blasi, F.D.; Buono, S.; Città, S.; Costanzo, A.A.; Zoccolotti, P. Reading Deficits in Intellectual Disability Are still an Open Question: A Narrative Review. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Klaiman, C.; Quintin, E.-M.; Jo, B.; Lightbody, A.A.; Hazlett, H.C.; Piven, J.; Hall, S.S.; Reiss, A.L. Longitudinal Profiles of Adaptive Behavior in Fragile X Syndrome. Pediatrics 2014, 134, 315–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Vermeulen, K.; De Boer, A.; Janzing, J.G.E.; Koolen, D.A.; Ockeloen, C.W.; Willemsen, M.H.; Verhoef, F.M.; Van Deurzen, P.A.M.; Van Dongen, L.; Van Bokhoven, H.; et al. Adaptive and maladaptive functioning in Kleefstra syndrome compared to other rare genetic disorders with intellectual disabilities. Am. J. Med Genet. Part A 2017, 173, 1821–1830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Dykens, E.M.; Hodapp, R.M.; Evans, D.W. Profiles and development of adaptive behavior in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndr. Res. Pr. 2006, 9, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Needham, M.; Packman, W.; Rappoport, M.; Quinn, N.; Cordova, M.; Macias, S.; Morgan, C.; Packman, S. MPS II: Adaptive Behavior of Patients and Impact on the Family System. J. Genet. Couns. 2013, 23, 330–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Åse, F.; Ilona, A.-R.; Mirjam, K.; Pekka, S.; Eugene, H.H.; Sarah, M.N.; Marit, K. Adaptive behaviour in children and adolescents with foetal alcohol spectrum disorders: A comparison with specific learning disability and typical development. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2012, 21, 221–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Pathak, M.; Bennett, A.; Shui, A.M. Correlates of adaptive behavior profiles in a large cohort of children with autism: The autism speaks Autism Treatment Network registry data. Autism 2019, 23, 87–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Dykens, E.M.; Roof, E.; Hunt-Hawkins, H. Cognitive and adaptive advantages of growth hormone treatment in children with Prader-Willi syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2016, 58, 64–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Mandic-Maravic, V.; Milovancevic, M.P.; Mitkovic-Voncina, M.; Kostic, M.; Aleksic-Hil, O.; Radosavljev-Kircanski, J.; Mincic, T.; Lecic-Tosevski, D. Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Does Sex Moderate the Pathway from Clinical Symptoms to Adaptive Behavior? Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 10418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Alfieri, P.; Scibelli, F.; Montanaro, F.A.M.; Digilio, M.C.; Ravà, L.; Valeri, G.; Vicari, S. Differences and Similarities in Adaptive Functioning between Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Williams–Beuren Syndrome: A Longitudinal Study. Genes 2022, 13, 1266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Zwaigenbaum, L.; Bryson, S.E.; Szatmari, P.; Brian, J.; Smith, I.M.; Roberts, W.; Vaillancourt, T.; Roncadin, C. Sex Differences in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Identified Within a High-Risk Infant Cohort. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 2585–2596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Goemans, A.; van Geel, M.; Vedder, P. Over three decades of longitudinal research on the development of foster children: A meta-analysis. Child Abus. Negl. 2015, 42, 121–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Åkerman, A.E.; Holmqvist, R.; Falkenström, F. What changes during specialized foster care? A study on adaptive functioning and emotional and social problems. Child Fam. Soc. Work 2022, 1–12, (early view verion). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Eves, R.; Mendonça, M.; Baumann, N.; Ni, Y.; Darlow, B.A.; Horwood, J.; Woodward, L.J.; Doyle, L.W.; Cheong, J.; Anderson, P.J.; et al. Association of Very Preterm Birth or Very Low Birth Weight with Intelligence in Adulthood: An Individual Partic-ipant Data Meta-Analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2021, 175, e211058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Huang, J.; Zhu, T.; Qu, Y.; Mu, D. Prenatal, Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors for Intellectual Disability: A Systemic Review and Meta-Analysis. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0153655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Schieve, L.A.; Clayton, H.B.; Durkin, M.S.; Wingate, M.S.; Drews-Botsch, C. Comparison of Perinatal Risk Factors Associated with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), Intellectual Disability (ID), and Co-occurring ASD and ID. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 2361–2372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Häggman-Laitila, A.; Salokekkilä, P.; Karki, S. Transition to adult life of young people leaving foster care: A qualitative systematic review. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2018, 95, 134–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Knot-Dickscheit, J.; Knorth, E.J. Gezinnen Met Meervoudige En Complexe Problemen: Theorie En Praktijk; Lemniscaat: Rotterdam, The Netherlands, 2019; ISBN 90-477-1168-8. [Google Scholar]
  78. Vervoort-Schel, J.; Mercera, G.; Wissink, I.; Van der Helm, P.; Lindauer, R.; Moonen, X. Prevalence of and relationship between adverse childhood experiences and family context risk factors among children with intellectual disabilities and borderline intellectual functioning. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2021, 113, 103935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Stephenson, L.A.; Beck, K.; Busuulwa, P.; Rosan, C.; Pariante, C.M.; Pawlby, S.; Sethna, V. Perinatal interventions for mothers and fathers who are survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Child Abus. Negl. 2018, 80, 9–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Kaminski, J.W.; Claussen, A.H. Evidence Base Update for Psychosocial Treatments for Disruptive Behaviors in Children. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2017, 46, 477–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Andrade, B.F.; Aitken, M.; Brodkin, S.; Sawrikar, V. Multiple Needs and Multiple Treatments. What’s a Clinician to Do? Update on the Psychosocial Treatment of Disruptive Behaviours in Childhood. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2022, 35, 409–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Kumar, R.; Shankar, K.; Kush, V.; Kumar, C.; Bhave, A.; Agarwal, V. Adaptation: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale for 3–9 year-old Indian children. Int. J. Disabil. Hum. Dev. 2016, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Nievar, M.A.; Luster, T. Developmental Processes in African American Families: An Application of McLoyd’s Theoretical Model. J. Marriage Fam. 2006, 68, 320–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Profiles of AB domains (VABS-3) in different levels of ID. Standard scores: M = 100, SD = 15.
Figure 1. Profiles of AB domains (VABS-3) in different levels of ID. Standard scores: M = 100, SD = 15.
Children 09 01911 g001
Figure 2. Profiles of AB (subdomains VABS-3) in different levels of ID. Communication domain includes the Receptive, Expressive, and Written subdomains. Daily Living Skills include the Personal, Domestic, and Community subdomains. Socialization domain includes the Interpersonal, Play/Leisure, and Coping Skills subdomains. All subdomains are in V-score (M = 15, SD = 3).
Figure 2. Profiles of AB (subdomains VABS-3) in different levels of ID. Communication domain includes the Receptive, Expressive, and Written subdomains. Daily Living Skills include the Personal, Domestic, and Community subdomains. Socialization domain includes the Interpersonal, Play/Leisure, and Coping Skills subdomains. All subdomains are in V-score (M = 15, SD = 3).
Children 09 01911 g002
Figure 3. Hierarchical regression models for predicting ABC scores. Significant predictors were in four steps of regression: (a) Level of ID, (b) Level of ID and Gender of children, (c) Level of ID, Gender, and factors related to institutional care—Length of stay and Type of institutional care, (d) Level of ID, Gender, factors related to institutional care—Length of stay, Type of institutional care, and risk factors in development—Neonatal difficulties. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Figure 3. Hierarchical regression models for predicting ABC scores. Significant predictors were in four steps of regression: (a) Level of ID, (b) Level of ID and Gender of children, (c) Level of ID, Gender, and factors related to institutional care—Length of stay and Type of institutional care, (d) Level of ID, Gender, factors related to institutional care—Length of stay, Type of institutional care, and risk factors in development—Neonatal difficulties. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Children 09 01911 g003aChildren 09 01911 g003b
Table 1. Profile of the study sample.
Table 1. Profile of the study sample.
BIF
N = 33
(16.8%)
Mild
N = 112
(56.8%)
Moderate
N = 39
(19.8%)
Severe
N = 13
(6.6%)
Sample Total
N = 197
(100.0%)
Gender
  Boys19 (57.6)55 (49.1)20 (51.3)4 (30.8)98 (49.7)
  Girls14 (42.4)57 (50.9)19 (48.7)9 (69.2)99 (50.3)
Age (in years)
  Mean (SD)13.4 (2.9)12.6 (2.9)12.8 (3.3)13.1 (3.0)12.8 (3.0)
  Range6.8–17.95.3–18.86.1–17.77.7–18.75.3–18.8
Type of institutional care
  Professional foster parents5 (15.2)20 (17.9)3 (7.7)-28 (14.2)
  GBC26 (78.8)78 (69.6)19 (48.7)-123 (62.4)
  GBC for children with disability2 (6.1)11 (9.8)11 (28.2)12 (92.3)36 (18.3)
  GBC for children with mental disorders-3 (2.7)6 (15.4)1 (7.7)10 (5.1)
Age of institutionalization
  Mean (SD)7.3 (3.7)7.1 (4.2)6.4 (4.9)2.8 (3.4)6.7 (4.3)
  Range1.1–17.60.8–15.70.1–16.10–110.8–17.6
Length of stay in institutional care (in years)
  Mean (SD)6.1 (3.9)5.5 (4.1)6.3 (5.2)10.2 (3.3)6.1 (4.4)
  Range0.0–12.00.4–16.20.3–17.04.0–15.00.0–17.0
Risk factors in the development *
  Substance use during pregnancy7 (21)28 (25)6 (15)1 (7.7)42 (22.6)
  Neonatal infection1 (3.0)5 (4.5)8 (20.5)3 (23.1)17 (8.6)
  Prematurity5 (15.2)14 (12.5)10 (25.6)7 (53.8)36 (18.3)
  Labor complications3 (9.1)6 (5.4)9 (23.1)2 (15.4)20 (10.2)
  Neonatal difficulties3 (9.1)11 (9.8)12 (30.8)5 (38.5)31 (15.7)
  Neglect EHC9 (27.3)35 (31.2)16 (41.0)-60 (30.5)
  Maltreatment5 (15.2)13 (11.6)7 (17.9)-25 (12.7)
  Not available11 (33.3)34 (30.3)5 (12.8)1 (7.6)51 (25.8)
Note. BIF—borderline intellectual functioning; GBC—group-based care; Neglect EHC—neglect early health care. * 25.8% (51) of children did not have available information about risk factors in development.
Table 2. Comparison of Standard scores for AB domain in each level of ID.
Table 2. Comparison of Standard scores for AB domain in each level of ID.
One-Way ANOVA
Levels of IDNMSDMinMaxFWelchDf1Df2p
Communication aBIF3380.310.075310554.7342.4<0.001
Mild ID11274.511.7630107
Moderate ID3952.420.612085
Severe ID1326.815.752066
Daily Living Skills aBIF3385.59.6769104151.4349.2<0.001
Mild ID11277.713.3148116
Moderate ID3958.816.062695
Severe ID1327.98.402050
Socialization aBIF33779.43569830.3343.6<0.001
Mild ID11273.213.7529103
Moderate ID395917.272889
Severe ID1331.218.682072
ABC aBIF3378.56.89689958.2343.0<0.001
Mild ID11274.19.7345107
Moderate ID3958.914.133084
Severe ID133014.232062
Note. BIF—borderline intellectual functioning; ID—intellectual disability; ABC—composite score. a Standard score (M = 100, SD = 15).
Table 3. Group (Level of ID) differences between subdomains of AB.
Table 3. Group (Level of ID) differences between subdomains of AB.
FWelchDf1Df2p
Communication
Receptive42.9343.6<0.001
Expressive34.2341.8<0.001
Written212.1373.8<0.001
Daily Living Skills
Personal201.7349.6<0.001
Domestic104.8350.6<0.001
Community139.1359.0<0.001
Socialization
Interpersonal25.8344.3<0.001
Play/Leisure32.9344.1<0.001
Coping Skills31.7345.2<0.001
Table 4. Descriptive data for domains and subdomains of VABS-3 and Gender.
Table 4. Descriptive data for domains and subdomains of VABS-3 and Gender.
t-test
GenderNMSDMinMaxtdfpd
DomainsCommunication aGirls9871.818.87201072.731950.0770.39
Boys9964.120.572096
Daily Living Skills aGirls9876.918.42201163.65195<0.0010.52
Boys9967.119.5020106
Socialization aGirls9873.015.77201003.77195<0.0010.54
Boys9963.519.4620103
Composite scoreABC aGirls9872.914.99201073.54195<0.0010.51
Boys9964.916.522190
CommunicationReceptive bGirls9811.294.031182.641950.0090.38
Boys999.684.49117
Expressive bGirls9811.443.741171.741950.0840.25
Boys9910.444.27117
Written bGirls987.573.551162.811950.0060.40
Boys996.163.51114
Daily Living SkillsPersonal bGirls9811.603.641193.151950.0020.45
Boys999.893.99116
Domestic bGirls9812.663.641194.16195<0.0010.59
Boys9910.324.13119
Community bGirls988.433.611183.061950.0020.44
Boys996.823.76114
SocializationInterpersonal bGirls9810.293.041174.05195<0.0010.58
Boys998.333.68117
Play/Leisure bGirls9810.082.991162.981950.0030.43
Boys998.693.54115
Coping Skills bGirls9810.053.331173.44195<0.0010.49
Boys998.263.93116
a Standard score (M = 100, SD = 15). b V-score (M = 15, SD = 3).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hapčová, M.; Celušáková, H.; Turoňová, D.; Souček Vaňová, M.; Besedová, L.; Demkaninová, D.; Babinská, K. Adaptive Behavior in Slovak Children with Intellectual Disability in Institutional Care. Children 2022, 9, 1911. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121911

AMA Style

Hapčová M, Celušáková H, Turoňová D, Souček Vaňová M, Besedová L, Demkaninová D, Babinská K. Adaptive Behavior in Slovak Children with Intellectual Disability in Institutional Care. Children. 2022; 9(12):1911. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121911

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hapčová, Margaréta, Hana Celušáková, Daniela Turoňová, Michaela Souček Vaňová, Lenka Besedová, Diana Demkaninová, and Katarína Babinská. 2022. "Adaptive Behavior in Slovak Children with Intellectual Disability in Institutional Care" Children 9, no. 12: 1911. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121911

APA Style

Hapčová, M., Celušáková, H., Turoňová, D., Souček Vaňová, M., Besedová, L., Demkaninová, D., & Babinská, K. (2022). Adaptive Behavior in Slovak Children with Intellectual Disability in Institutional Care. Children, 9(12), 1911. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121911

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop