Next Article in Journal
Limb Length Discrepancy and Corticospinal Tract Disruption in Hemiplegic Cerebral Palsy
Next Article in Special Issue
Psychomotor Skills Activities in the Classroom from an Early Childhood Education Teachers’ Perspective
Previous Article in Journal
New and Old Challenges in Pediatric Health Policies
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Novel Data-driven Approach to Examine Children’s Movements and Social Behaviour in Schoolyard Environments
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Assessment of the Psychomotor Profile in Children: Preliminary Psychometric Analysis of the Portuguese Version of the Batterie d’Evaluation des Fonctions Neuropsychomotrices de L’enfant (NPmot.pt)

1
POLO UIDEFMH, Faculdade de Motricidade Humana, Universidade de Lisboa, 1499-002 Lisboa, Portugal
2
Centro de Reabilitação de Paralisia Cerebral de Coimbra, 3030-188 Coimbra, Portugal
3
UIDEFMH, Faculdade de Motricidade Humana, Universidade de Lisboa, 1499-002 Lisboa, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2022, 9(8), 1195; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081195
Submission received: 16 June 2022 / Revised: 1 August 2022 / Accepted: 4 August 2022 / Published: 9 August 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Child Psychomotricity: Development, Assessment, and Intervention)

Abstract

:
Psychomotor assessment is an expanding research field. A deep knowledge of the typical development will allow for better child-centered planning. Due to the scarcity of psychomotor assessment instruments validated in Portugal, our research aims to perform a preliminary analysis of the psychometric properties of the Portuguese version of the Battery for Neuropsychomotor functions evaluation (NPmot.pt). The NPmot.pt was translated and adapted to the Portuguese language/culture and applied to 200 children, 4–12 years old (6 y 10 m ± 0 y 4 m), with (n = 150) and without developmental disorders, attending regular school. For content validity, nine experts classified all items according to their relevance, clarity, simplicity and ambiguity. All indexes (IVC > 0.78) pointed out the representativeness of indicators, corroborated by experts proportion agreement (>0.42), and Cohen’s kappa scores (0.02 > k < 0.95). Reliability was confirmed through internal consistency, with Cronbach alphas/split-half (α > 0.45) and temporal reliability (test-retest technique, 0.45 > r < 0.99). Construct validity was analyzed through domains and domains-total correlations, tending to moderate to strong (0.31 > r < 0.92); exploratory factor analysis pointed out an eight-factor solution, explaining 88.5% of the total variance. For discriminant validity, we conducted a comparative study between children with and without developmental disorders that revealed significant differences (p < 0.05). The NPmot.pt seems to confirm validity and reliability for Portugal; however, more studies are needed.

1. Introduction

Psychomotricity is an area of knowledge that is seeing an increased interest in education and health care in Europe [1], including Portugal [2]. Fundamentally, it involves the interaction between psychomotor development and psychological-emotional functions [3]. Psychomotor Therapy (PMT) is being provided as one of the main supports to children in diverse contexts (schools, hospitals, institutions) and from a three-dimensional perspective: educational, re-educational and therapeutic [2,4], through an evolutionary, developmental and competencial understanding of the process of human growth [5]. PMT, integrating cognition, body movement and emotions, is based on learning-by-movement experiences, on a holistic view of the child, through the unity of body and mind [6]. The goal is to enable the person to act accordingly and autonomously within their own psychosocial context. “The moving body in all its aspects is the cornerstone of the psychomotor approach [distinguishing…] psychomotor therapy from other approaches” [7] (p. 28). PMT is performed by a psychomotor therapist [8].
Recent evidence supports the relevance of the quality of the psychomotor profile in child development [3]. Evidence pointed out the universality (all children go through the same stages) of the specific developmental sequence of different psychomotor skills [9]. The acquisition of motor milestones is an important indicator of developmental trajectory, affecting the overall development of children and their interaction with environmental demands. Further, even though not a formal diagnosis criterion for most developmental disorders, psychomotor limitations are present in several disorder manifestations, such as intellectual and developmental disability (IDD) [10] or autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [11,12], among others. The screening and detection of psychomotor developmental limitations and clinical concerns at various stages of childhood development is essential for the diagnosis, informed planning, progress assessment and monitoring of interventions [13]. The collection of data should be both quantitative (product-oriented) and qualitative (process-oriented) for a more holistic understanding of the movement performance of children. The appreciation of these individual variations (quantitative and qualitative) can provide significant, semiological, and diagnostic indications [14,15,16,17,18,19,20], establishing symptomatic pictures and enabling comparison with peers, aiming to describe in detail the “real value” of the limitation expressed by the child [20]. Current research in the field of PMT warns of the fact that assessment and understanding of clinical pictures of developmental disorders [14,16,17] neglect psychomotor and perceptual-motor limitations.
The lower quality of psychomotor performance of children with developmental disorders results from the complexity of the interaction of the cognitive-motor and sensory systems [21,22], with a less positive impact on the relationship with involvement. Changes in psychomotor skills that involve tonus and balance [23,24], with repercussions on gait [25], strength and coordination of the upper and lower limbs are reported in children with developmental disorders [10], interfering with performance in other tasks involving, among other skills, eye-hand coordination [26]. Moreover, the greater the cognitive impairment, the greater the difficulties in terms of activities with the ball, balance and manual dexterity [27]. The balance and integration of sensory stimuli’s limitations are also observable in children from six to ten years. In their comparative studies between children with and without developmental disorders, Shum and Pang [28] and Geuze [29] noticed significant difficulties in balance and integration of sensory stimuli. Children with T21 tend to use their left hand, which increases with age vs. typical peers that tend to use their right hand [30]. Manual proficiency was clearly slower in the T21 group, regardless of age, although it improved with increasing age. The right hand was the most proficient in both groups. Children with developmental disorders present significant limitations on coordination skills: children with ASD demonstrate a more generalized commitment concerning gestural performance and tend to present a higher number of errors/distortion in imitated gestures than the other groups [31]. Children with ADHD tend to fail tests that assess body image, with a high level of impulsivity index [20]. In terms of auditive attention, Vaivre-Douret [20] evaluated 20 children with ADHD (four from each age group) and found few correct choices with a high level of impulsivity index. Miyahara and collaborators [32] compared the fine motor skills of children with Asperger’s Syndrome (SA) and Specific Learning Difficulties (SLD), noting that the former demonstrates better manual dexterity and lesser skills in handling balls.
Despite the evidence, research does not enable us to understand the type and nature of such disturbances. Few instruments rely on a developmental approach. The relation between neurodevelopmental processes and central nervous system maturation needs to be more explored for a clear understanding of the origin of such limitations [12] and to act accordingly. Despite this, there are some motor instruments (Lincoln–Oseretsky test [33], Motor Coordination Scale [34], and the Child Movement Assessment Battery M-ABC [35]) that are not developmental [12] and show higher levels of complexity in association with increasing age [20]. Most are dependent on learning and practice and do not allow drawing a minor deficits symptoms profile (neurological soft signs) during an overall motor skills evaluation [12]. In addition, these assessments measure mostly competencies related to the practice/training of a task [20]. Some traditional psychomotor tests involve the assessment of right–left discrimination and spatial orientation as an implicit coordinate system [9]. Body awareness/image (recognition of body parts, reproduction of movements, drawing figures) is another targeted dimension of other psychomotor tests, particularly in adolescence [36], but lacking in childhood. There are a few tests that do not fully satisfy the metric requirement; when analyzed with little psychometric data on the validity and reliability of models and tests, most do not have cultural adaptation, which limits their use and does not allow comparisons. Some findings in reliability are not acceptable [37], others present low correlations between domains in children [38,39,40], and the confirmatory factorial analysis does not validate the original factor model [41].
Psychomotor development is characterized by periods of growth and abilities acquisition (2 to 5 years) followed by stabilization moments for the specialization of those skills (>6 years [20]). The typical development respects the natural sequence of the central nervous system maturation and all the subsystems associated that contributes to a harmonious development of psychomotor skills [6]. The normative evolution allows placing the child in its physiological (genetic) maturation and detecting a functional disorder with a symptomatic value of a neurological, psychomotor or psychological origin. There is a need to establish and analyze the psychomotor profile of children [42]. This assessment should integrate all dimensions of childhood and not be focused solely on behavior analysis [17,43]. Psychometric assessment provides data about performance in several activities on a norm-referenced comparison [44]. However, for the psychomotor therapist, this is a very restricted vision of the child [6,17,43,45] because the performance should not be reduced to a single number to be compared with a pre-established norm. That performance should be contextualized, and qualitative observation (precision, quality) should, therefore, be considered an important key factor in the assessment [20]. One of the major contributions of PMT assessment is not just to be focused on the final motor performance (product), but also on how the child integrates, programs, executes and plans their thoughts vs. observable behavior [6]. However, there are no updated norms about children’s psychomotor development, and most of the tests used are more focused on cognitive and verbal competencies rather than psychomotor ones [46].
The most consensual model of the human psychomotor system [6,20,47] comprises a set of psychomotor factors, interdependent and in constant interaction, that differs in number and naming, with weak to strong correlations between them. The Psychomotor Battery developed by Vítor da Fonseca [42] was based on an extensive literature review (theoretical approach) but without empirical data. Correlations between factors ranged from 0 (lateralization and fine motor skills) to 1 (fine motor skills and space-time, body awareness and global motor skills). No exploratory or confirmatory analyses were performed. In France, the Vaivre-Douret [20] model comprises nine functions: tonus, gross motor skills, laterality (tonic, spontaneous/gestural, psychosocial and usual), manual praxis, tactile gnosis, hand-eye coordination, spatial orientation, rhythm and auditive attention.
Based on a selection of (psychomotor) items from other tests to evaluate psychomotor development, in 2006, Vaivre-Douret proposed the creation of the Neuropsychomotor Function Evaluation Battery-NP-MOT [20], updating the application rules in order to respond to the psychomotor therapist’s difficulty in assessing psychomotor development [17,19,20,43]. The NP-MOT covers the motor components and enables the assessment of neuromotor functions: extrapyramidal (related to general and daily activity involving both involuntary and voluntary movements, mastery/specialization, and speed [6,42], at rest, passive, static, automated and involuntary and voluntary movements), cerebellar (e.g., the ability to remain static and dynamic balance with the collaboration between involuntary and voluntary movements) and pyramidal (reflected at the voluntary movements). The NP-MOT assesses from two different, but essential, points of view—the mastery and specialization (quality) of the movement and the speed (quantitative aspect) of the involuntary and voluntary movements produced. The NP-MOT may be applied for diagnosis, identification and description of strengths and areas to be promoted, contributing to a better semiology and a more personalized and effective intervention approach [18,19,43,47]. This new battery was developed to monitor physiological evolution, identify significant deviations from typical development and detect minor functional disorders that might have a pathologic impact on learning processes [12,20]. The establishment of the psychomotor profile is an emergent need for psychomotor therapists to perform a comprehensive, accurate and reliable measurement considering all these factors [38].
There is a call for appropriate testing to identify psychomotor skills across countries [39,40], not only for cross-cultural comparisons but also to develop a common language among psychomotor therapists [38,40]. In France, as well as other countries (including Portugal), there are no up-to-date standards for child neurological development due to the absence of a valid and reliable instrument to assess children’s psychomotor functions with quantitative evidence to establish the psychomotor profile and the respective cut-off scores. Further, it also appears that the tests used to assess evolutionary development and maturation are mostly oriented toward the assessment of cognitive and verbal, excluding, directly or indirectly, the assessment of psychomotor skills [46,48]. Moreover, the quantitative assessment of the child is not enough for the psychomotor therapist, who needs to also gather data about behavior analysis (during assessment). According to Fonseca [42], the interpretation of data from a psychomotor assessment should complement the performance (product) with data about how the child integrates, programs, executes and regulates their behavior (process).
It is an emergent standard instrument to assess psychomotor functions and analyze the integration of sensory and neuromotor systems linked to the central nervous system’s maturation [20]. Most of the psychomotor assessment instruments are outdated, some are based on classical and old statistical analyses and others do not meet the quality metrics that a diagnosis instrument should have [13]. In the national scenario, despite the availability of some psychomotor tests for adults and elderly [38,49,50], little research has been focused on the analysis of metric qualities, such as validity and reliability, especially with children. The use of instruments with robust psychometric properties will allow the gathering of data-based evidence, fundament to the PMT conceptual model, for a more valid child-centered plan and to assess the psychomotor interventions’ effectiveness [9,13,40].
Therefore, and because of sparse psychometric research, our goal aims to present studies in the field of validation of the Portuguese version of NP-MOT for children between 4 and 12 years, with and without developmental disorders, to contribute to a valid measurement instrument for establishing the psychomotor profile, through a preliminary analysis of the psychometric properties: content validity, reliability, discriminant and construct validity.
The French battery was chosen for several reasons. This instrument is the result of ten years of research in the field of French neurology and is an innovative psychomotor assessment tool that promotes a neuro-functional understanding of the child’s brain organization. This feature allows us to detect and analyze the nature of various developmental changes [51]. The NP-MOT is a clinical, standardized, valid and reliable instrument [20] that aims “to capture maturational aspects in the ability to perform and quality of performance, reflecting the development of neurological mechanisms contributing to movement timing, motor control, motor coordination and motor execution” ([48], p. 4). It is also a developmental and age/maturation-related assessment, with identical subtests (qualitative and quantitative developmental norms) across ages and without a global score from the battery. Further, it comprises a quantitative and qualitative approach to the assessment of neuromotor functions, which allows detecting neurological soft signs often never systematically addressed [48]. The mere translation of tests is not enough to be applied within different age or sociocultural subgroups, and all the procedures suggested in the literature [52] were followed. There is still a lack of knowledge and absence of standardized instruments of body structures and functions in Portugal. “It provides information on maturation levels for each of the functions explored, independently of other functions [entailing] one or more tests, some of which consist of various items” ([48], p. 2).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Our convenience sample involved 200 children between 4 years and 12 years and 6 months old (6 y 10 m ± 0 y 4 m), with (nfemale = 19 and nmale = 31) and without developmental disorders (nfemale = 86 and nmale = 64), attending regular and public schools, enrolled from preschool education (n = 71) to the second cycle of basic education (n = 129), in the district of Beja. As inclusion criteria were: the ages of participants (4 > x < 12 years), meeting the original standardization sample, but extending to 12 years because the PMT is one of the supports provided at these ages in the school community; attendance at a regular school; and, in case of children with a developmental disorder, existence of prior clinical identification of developmental disorder (ASD, IDD) in their educational process. Within the scope of the exclusion criteria were defined: age (x < 4 e x > 12) and the existence of an associated sensory and/or motor disability that made the application of tasks impossible.
The test–retest was analyzed through the application of the test twice, with an interval of 2–3 weeks, to 50 children (males n = 25) and five children at each age stage (6 y 1 m ± 0 y 4 m) who respected all inclusion criteria of the study.
The sample was divided into six age groups, according the following distribution: group 1–4 years to 4 years and 9 months (nfemale = 25 and nmale = 4); group 2–4 years and 10 months to 5 years and 8 months (nfemale = 13 and nmale = 28); group 3–5 years and 9 months to 6 years and 6 months (nfemale = 7 and nmale = 0); group 4–6 years and 7 months to 7 years and 5 months (nfemale = 20 and nmale = 8); group 5–7 years and 6 months to 8 years and 5 months (nfemale = 30 and nmale = 22); and group 6–8 years and 6 months to 12 years (nfemale = 10 and nmale = 13).

2.2. Instrument

The Portuguese version of the NP-MOT (NPmot.pt) was translated and adapted from the standard French version [20]. The NPmot.pt aims to assess the neuropsychomotor skills of children between 4 and 12 years, to understand the normative development of neuromotor, neurosensory and perceptual integration functions, as well as to establish and understand the neuropsychomotor profile of children (strengths and weaknesses) with and without typical development. The data obtained will contribute to the planning and implementation of effective interventions.
NPmot.pt, in its Portuguese version, maintained the same original structure, with all 52 items distributed over 9 psychomotor domains [20]: Tonus (residual, action, support, attitude tonus and rotellian reflexes); Gross Motor Skills (dynamic and static balance, gait); Laterality (spontaneous, daily and psychosocial gestures), Manual Praxis (repetitive and alternating movements), Tactile Gnosis, Hand-Eye Coordination, Spatial Orientation (in themself, in the other, about an object and through a map), Rhythm (time, auditive-kinesthetic, auditive-perceptual-motor) and Auditive Attention (selected and sustained). The domains can be applied isolated (if there is a need to understand only one specific function), but the global assessment of all domains will allow a better definition of a global neuropsychomotor development profile, contributing to the definition and analysis of differential diagnoses [20].
The items are rated by assigning two grades (one quantitative and another qualitative). The quantitative refers to the child’s performance and varies from 0 (does not perform) to 2 or 5 (corresponding to the best performance, depending on the items). This assessment is conjugated by a qualitative observation of the components of the gesture (quality, precision, speed). The qualitative observation is characterized by the performance of a harmonious movement or with visible changes (resistance, tremors, anxiety). The test should be norm-referenced, and data should be interpreted according to a comparison with the standard values [20]. It will be possible to define a global neuropsychomotor profile, allowing the identification of strong areas and areas to promote a more adjusted and personalized neuropsychomotor intervention [20].
The content validity of the original version was obtained by expert agreement (76% to and 98%). Reliability was analyzed by temporal stability (test-retest technique), in which scores ranged from 0.70 (Tonus) to 0.96 (Laterality), and internal consistency through Cronbach’s alpha showed scores above 0.70 [20,53,54]. The sensitivity of the test was assessed through statistical analysis, showing a significant increase in values from age to age. For the criterion’s validity, the author analyzed the correlations between the NP-MOT and the Lincoln–Bruininsky–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency Psychomotor Development Battery [33]. Both scales were applied to 50 children with and without developmental coordination disorder (DCD), and IQ values above average. Correlations were strong (0.72 > r < 0.84), and the comparative study indicated that children with developmental coordination disorder had lower results (1 or 2 standard deviations) in almost all domains.

2.3. Procedures

2.3.1. Translation and Adaptation

The present investigation respected all ethical procedures inherent to the validation process of an evaluation instrument [52]. Firstly, it was obtained the original author’s permission to carry out the adaptation. After translating and adapting the procedures to guarantee that the construct was understood in the same way across language and cultural groups, content validity was performed to confirm equivalence between both versions. Particular attention was given to context–cultural–linguistic adaptation (experts agreement), trying to use natural, simple and clear, not ambiguous language. The initial translation was focused on functional rather than literal equivalence. Therefore, two Portuguese and French-native speakers, both experts in PMT, respectively, performed a forward and back translation. The two versions were compared by two practitioners to clarify instructions and response options and to sustain a similar structure through all tasks. All disagreements were solved by the third investigator. A pre-final of the test was established. Then, the Portuguese version, with a clarification of the goal and target group of the test, was sent to 9 experts, selected based on their academic, methodological and professional experience in PMT and children development, to classify and rate each item according to its relevance, clarity, simplicity and ambiguity. However, because a theoretical-qualitative approach is not enough to guarantee the suitability of adaptation and the scales’ equivalence, a field test was performed.

2.3.2. Administration and Statistical Analysis

After obtaining Ethical Approval for the study, contacts were carried out with several schools to request authorization to apply the NPmot.pt to students between 4 and 12 years, with and without developmental disorders. For this matter, informed consent with the explanation of research (goal, procedures, ethical topics, confidentiality, anonymity, analysis and contributions) was sent to all School Board Directors. In case of accordance, a similar document was sent to primary caregivers. All participants (with and without developmental disorders) signed the informed consent/gave oral consent. The application of the NPmot.pt was conducted according to its protocol, in scholar gyms, according to children’s schedules and availability, to minimize the interference with other curricular and extracurricular activities. Each application took about 90 minutes on average, depending on the child’s characteristics. There were no missing data, and all data collected were analyzed.
We investigated three types of validity. For the content validity analysis, two distinct phases were carried out [55,56,57]: a developmental descriptive based on an extensive literature review about the construct and assessment tools, and an empirical one that required the participation of 9 experts (parents, practitioners and academics). Of those experts, four have a Ph.D. in Special Education and two in Human Kinetics (PMT), three are experts in validation methodology, two have a master’s degree in PMT, and three are psychomotor therapists that work with children with and without developmental disorders in schools, for the last ten years, in Portugal. Each expert rated all items based on relevance, clarity, simplicity and ambiguity through a four-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (much irrelevant) to 4 (much relevant). Then, these four options were transformed into a dichotomous scale with items rated previously with 1 and 2, considered not having content validity, and 3 and 4 as valid [55,56,58]. All content validity indexes (CVI) were determined: for items, scale average (CVI_A), and universal agreement (CVI_UA). However, this analysis is insufficient because these indexes do not provide data about the adjustment of agreement expected by chance [55,58]. Therefore, the proportion between experts and Cohen’s kappa was also performed. The cut-off scores assumed for each index were [55]: CVI > 0.78, CVI_UA > 0.80, CVI_A > 0.90. Cohen’s kappa interpretation was based on the following values: poor (k < 0.40), moderate (0.41 > k < 0.60), substantial (0.61 > k < 0.80) and excellent agreement if k > 0.81 [58,59].
For reliability, we employed Cronbach’s alpha and split-half to analyze internal consistency, assuming that scores below 0.70 are inadequate, between 0.71 and 0.80 are acceptable, exceptional above 0.81 [60] and preferably above 0.90 for diagnosis purposes [60]; and the correlations coefficients of Pearson (technique test-retest: two applications of the test within a 2–3 week interval) to examine temporal stability: weak if r < 0.40, moderate when 0.41 > k < 0.69, high between 0.70 and 0.89 and very high if superior to 0.90 [61]. The construct analysis considered domains-domains and domains-total, and an exploratory factorial analysis (EFA) to explore the underlying structure of the construct was measured. Finally, the discriminant validity to evaluate differences between children with and without developmental disorders was analyzed through the parametric techniques of t-student and One-way ANOVA, using a 95% significance level. The choice of tests for the statistical analysis of the data collected was based on the fulfillment of the assumptions necessary for parametric tests. In this sense, the dependent variables presented a normal distribution in both groups, most of the cases being close to the mean value. The existence of homogeneity of variances (homoscedasticity) was also verified; that is, the variances of the dependent variables were homogeneous in all groups. The test used to verify Normality was the Kolmogorov–Smirnov, and the Levene test was used to test the homogeneity of variances. The sample size (n = 200) also consolidated the decision to use parametric tests.
The application of the battery is simple, individual and uses little material; the original protocol was followed by all examiners, starting with physical observations, as suggested [20]. All examiners had access to a suitcase with the materials.
Data processing was performed using Microsoft Excel version 14 (14.0.7268.5000), 64-bit, developed by Microsoft, Lisbon, Portugal and Statistical Package of Social Sciences (SPSS) software, version 25, developed by IBM, Lisbon, Portugal.

3. Results

Content validity measures the degree to which an instrument covers the construct that we want to measure [56]. After the translation, a reconciliation procedure aimed to guarantee the equivalence of the Portuguese version, and some clarifications were made. Due to the limitations of such a qualitative analysis, an empirical approach was also performed through expertise agreement [55,56,58]. This method required multiple levels of the agreement by a nine-expert committee. All items (52) were considered relevant, and only the items that do not account for at least 0.78 [55] in any of the other categories are presented (Table 1).
Because some scores in clarity, simplicity and ambiguity were lower than 0.75, particular attention was given to the wording and readability of items. Reformulations were performed in nine items (e.g., a simple explanation was introduced about a specific item because only the name was not understandable to the experts) as well as a new order in presenting the items for a more effective application (e.g., applying the Tonus exam last, applying the items of Bi-manual and Digital Praxis next to the syncinesies exam or starting the evaluation beginning in the Registration Book 2). Item names were simplified and clarified (e.g., item 14: Lateralization was replaced by Tonic-Manual Lateralization and Manual Daily Lateralization), and a small description was added for faster identification of what was measured. Despite the lower scores in clarity, simplicity and ambiguity, the relevance category presented indexes above 0.78. The CVI_AU is related to the proportion of items considered relevant by all experts, ranges from 0 (total disagreement) to 1 (total agreement) and should be superior to 0.80. The CVI_A is the ratio of all relevant options (3 and 4), in each of the categories, by the total number of experts and should be above 0.90 [55]. Both indexes were significant for the relevance level.The decision was to keep all items. Due to CVI limitations, the agreement between experts was also calculated (Table 2), and there was an excellent agreement among all experts concerning the relevance of items.
Only in clarity (0.41 to 0.67) and simplicity (0.58 to 0.77), the indexes were below 0.80, but the agreement was still considered moderate to substantial. The number of experts may influence these results. Many of the items were reformulated, and these initial analyses played a critical role in the adaptation-formulation, understanding and readability of questions. Finally, Cohen’s kappa (Table 3), to analyze the agreement level between each pair of experts [62], was calculated. Considering the number of experts (n = 9) the agreement tended to be positive, ranging from moderate (>0.40) to excellent (0.96), although some lower scores related to experts 5 and 7 (k = 0.05), 7 and 9 (k = 0.02) and 9 and 3 and 4 (k = 0) [58,59]. The lowest scores may be explained by the absence of disagreement between the pair of experts, which may lead to a decrease in these experts’ global agreement [55].
After confirming the content validity, the final version of NPmot.pt was established, and a test pilot was conducted. The battery was then applied twice within 2–4 weeks to 50 children to analyze temporal stability [54,61]. Further, reliability was also analyzed in terms of internal consistency through the calculation of Cronbach’s alpha, Split-Half and intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) (Table 4). The reliability indexes ranged from 0.45 to 1, indicating NPmot.pt as a reliable instrument, although there was a lower score on digital gnosis (0.45).
The construct validity was analyzed through domain inter-correlations (Table 5) using Pearson’s correlation coefficient [53,63] to measure the degree of independence of the domains among themselves [54]. Domains correlated with each other moderately (0.30 > x < 0.60) [57] and at strong levels, ranging from 0.31 (Laterality and Tonus) to 0.92 (Spatial Orientation and Gross Motor Skills domains). Only the score of 0.27 between the Tactile Gnosis and Tonus stood out as weak correlation domains.
EFA (Table 6) was also performed to explore the organization of items by domains [20] using the maximum likelihood estimation method, the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin index (KMO) and the Bartlett sphericity test [54]. The factor loadings pointed out the multidimensionality of the construct on an eight-domain structure (and not nine as in the original version). The first two components explained about 69.2% of the overall variance. Eigenvalues greater than 1, a minimum of 5%, explained variance per component of the scree plot; factor loadings of 0.40 and above were the criteria assumed for the extraction [54].
One of the important characteristics of instruments of this nature is the discriminant validity for distinguishing profiles between groups [20]. We analyzed several variables: gender, diagnosis (with and without developmental disorders) (Table 7) and age (Table 8 and Table 9). Parametric Student’s t-tests were used for dichotomous variables (i.e., gender and diagnosis) and One-way ANOVA with post-hoc Scheffé, given the uneven number of participants per step age.
Statistically significant differences were observed using the Student’s t-test at the level of neuropsychomotor skills of participants with and without developmental disorders in all domains (p < 0.01), as expected. In terms of gender, there were only differences found in Laterality and Rhythm.
The results show that the mean scores in all domains were similar, despite the increase in age. This corroborates the conceptual model behind the construction of this instrument since the author mentions that the same items are applied to all children, with standardized classes that allow framing the child in its age group and not with all children aged between 4 and 12 years. Lower values were also observed in the older age group, which may be contradictory, but the fact that in this age group, there is a high percentage of children with developmental disorders may justify these results.
Overall, there were statistically significant differences between the children between 4 and 5, 8 years old and the older ones. Further, group 6, which included the oldest children, also tends to present significant differences from the others, maybe because children with developmental disorders are included in this group. These findings seem to be aligned with the developmental approach of the instrument NPmot.pt. The critical period of acquisition of neuropsychomotor skills seems to be the range of 4–6 years, showing a stabilization and maturation from 8 years.

4. Discussion

This research analyzes the metric properties of a neuropsychomotor assessment tool for children aged 4 to 12 years. Validity and reliability are the prerequisites to guarantee the quality of any instrument, and because they are not “transferable” from the cultural/age group, they should always be analyzed with a new distinctive group [39]. According to the authors: “reporting of reliability and validity properties of results from motor competence assessments should be a common occurrence in peer-reviewed journal articles” (p. 1778). The starting point was the cross-cultural adaptation because the mere translation of a test is not enough and should be avoided due to the lack of rigorousness in data interpretation [38,50,52,64]. Special attention was paid to the translation and adaptation of the battery [52] to the Portuguese language and cultural values. This adaptation required an exhaustive literature review of the construct and its evidence-based indicators. The translation process considered the inclusion of the expert’s native speaker of both the original (French) and Portuguese versions for an initial content and semantic equivalence. After, it was asked to three psychomotor therapists to analyze the battery in order to adequate the wording and instructions of items. Finally, the empirical procedure involved nine experts, whose judgment agreement allowed establishing a final evidence-based version of the correspondence between items and respective domains. In the case of having more than six experts, CVI should be superior to 0.78 [52,56].
In our findings in the Relevance category, only four items (items 13—Rotellian Reflexes, 32—Psychosocial Laterality, 33—Symmetrical bi-manual Pronation-Supination, and 34—Simultaneous Asymmetrical Bi-manual Pronation-Supination) presented a 0.78 score, maybe due to the existence solely of the name and not what was asked in each task. Based on experts’ judgments and qualitative comments, language was clarified and simplified. Further, some of these items seemed redundant for some experts (qualitative comment), and therefore, special attention was given to them. All the rest of the items scored higher (0.89) in their representativeness as an indicator of each domain. The decision was to keep all items, although the lower scores in other categories (clarity, simplicity and ambiguity) led to some reformulations and clarifications, to make the items more understandable, avoiding the use of ambiguous terms. One of the main changes was to make clearer the naming of some items (e.g., item 13—rotellian reflexes) as well as to simplify/add explanatory instructions of its tasks. Some other items (5, 7, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 28, 29, 32, 49, 51 and 52) needed a reformulation on semantic, grammar and sentence construction (e.g., item 5, in the French version: Extensibilité des angles poplités, to the Portuguese version: Extensibilidade do poplíteo (popliteal extensibility)). According to the perspective of the original author, the tonus domain (items 5 to 13) needed more specific items rather than using more comprehensive items as other scales (M-ABC or BOPMT-2). Vaivre-Douret [20] considered this domain as neglected, despite its important role in gathering information about child development. The CVI of each item was corroborated by the AU/IVC-E (0.83) and the M/IVC-E (0.97). Experts assumed the relevance of all items of NPmot.pt. There are no data about the psychomotor battery used in our country.
This accordance was corroborated in the proportion of agreement among experts with scores higher than 0.73 (in the ambiguity category). Again, when it comes to relevance, the experts tend to present an excellent agreement (>0.96), very similar to the original version [20]. The poor agreement scores tend to be associated with experts 5, 7 and 9 that presented, however, an exceptional agreement between experts in relevance (0.98 to 1), and moderate to strong on clarity (>0.42), simplicity (>0.80) and ambiguity (>0.58). The lowest scores may be explained by the reduced disagreement between the pair of experts, which may lead to a decrease in these experts’ global agreement [55]. Further, Cohen’s kappa is sensitive to the types of disagreement [59], which may influence the final scores. Although our findings seem to be in line with the original study [20]—which also presented a very good to excellent agreement proportion (0.76 to 0.98), there are no further details about the selection of items and empirical description of the content validity indexes. Therefore, we were unable to compare our findings with the original ones. However, both scales are based on a similar multidimensional model. Then, the reliability of the NPmot.pt was analyzed. Our findings suggest that NPmot.pt has overall good to excellent reliability, although further studies should be conducted with larger samples. Internal consistency presented excellent scores (α > 0.85) in all domains, except in hand-eye coordination (0.79), which was acceptable, and digital gnosis (α = 0.45). The internal consistency of the total scale was 0.86. These findings, corroborated by other reliability indexes calculated, seem to be aligned with Nunnaly’s [60] reminder of the need to have scores above 0.90 for diagnosis purposes. All indices point out the battery as a reliable instrument, corroborating the original scores [20], although it needs closer attention, especially in the Tactile Gnosis domain, which in France reported an acceptable 0.83 [17,20]. Still, more studies are recommended.
There were consistently moderate to high correlations between NPmot.pt domains, varying from 0.31 and 0.92, except tonus and digital gnosis (0.27), with a weak correlation. This may be explained by the need for a certain level of attention and proprioceptive integration that is not dependent on the tonus muscle. Another hypothesis is that these scores may reflect the non-maturation of these skills, particularly in children with developmental disorders [20]. This score should be further analyzed since it is the domain that is consistently presenting lower scores. The domain-to-domain correlations show moderate to strong correlation, which is aligned with the original version [20] and higher than those reported by Fonseca [6], and seems to support construct validity. Each domain of NPmot.pt has different indicators, and domains are related to each other. The EFA produced consistent findings with previous studies [20] and conceptualizations [6] of a multidimensional construct, although in our sample, the indicators seem to be organized into eight domains, explaining 88.5% of the total variance.
Finally, in an attempt to understand the existence (or not) of differences in terms of psychomotor skills in a set of variables (age, gender and diagnosis, i.e., with and without the developmental disorder), a comparative study of the psychomotor skills of the participants was carried out. There were significant differences between males and females, which were not found in the original study [20] in the domains of Laterality and Rhythm, with a tendency for slightly higher mean values in females. This idea was already reported by Eckert [64], who mentions the precocity of female motor development, except in the domains of Tonus and Gross Motor Skills, where higher values are expected for males. However, it should be noted that the majority of the participants with a developmental disorder were male.
Analyzing mean scores confirms group differentiation between children with and without developmental disorders in the neuropsychomotor profile, demonstrating potential clinical significance, i.e., NPmot.pt appears to be able to differentiate children with typical from atypical development. Children with typical development score higher than children with developmental disorders. This finding seems to be aligned with others [11,12,13,21,23,24,25,26,32,33,38], and in direct association with cognitive, executive and motor limitations [12,65,66,67], being of particular interest the muscle tone alterations [24], balance and gait [23,25], eye-hand coordination [26,68], laterality (preference and proficiency) and space-time [30]. As expected, significant differences were found between all groups, especially group 6, which despite having older participants, also included children with developmental disorders. Findings seem to corroborate the developmental trajectory until approximately 8 years old [20,64], and by that time, there is a visible tendency for a period of stabilization and consolidation of acquired skills [20,64].
However, findings should be interpreted with caution due to some limitations. The experts’ committee should have involved parents and teachers of children with and without developmental disorders for adding other perspectives on the importance of each item in daily life. The sample was reduced and geographically circumscribed, and there was no control for the level of severity of developmental disorders. The recommendation is to continue to apply the NPmot.pt to a representative, significant and stratified sample to deepen the hierarchical structure of the test and establish the cut-off scores for a more precise and rigorous diagnosis of developmental delays. The establishment of these cut-offs assumes an essential role in order to avoid incorrect/false diagnoses with adverse consequences in children’s lives. The criterion validity is another step that should be performed. The comparison between groups (ages, diagnosis, level of severity, etc.) is also a suggestion for future research with practical implications. The analysis of how psychomotor functions correlate with other variables (e.g., intellectual functioning, adaptive behavior, scholar performance) is also another topic to be explored.
In this particular research, our results will allow an evidence-based use of the psychomotor battery for establishing the psychomotor profile of children between 4 and 12 years. This is a pioneer study due to the scarcity of standardized neuropsychomotor assessment instruments in the context of PMT and the urgent need for a valid test to be used with children. The development of a theory and the design of a sound psychometric measure to gather valid data is essential for several purposes, both in clinical practice and in research [69]. If instruments are not valid or reliable, results obtained cannot be used with confidence, compromising the effectiveness of neuropsychomotor interventions. This data will allow gathering data-based evidence to be used to test the theory/conceptual model of PMT, which is it being supported by the data provided from that measure, and may be used in decision making [13], such as a more valid children-centered plan [2]. It will also allow establishing the neuropsychomotor profile and setting the cut-off scores for determining significant limitations (above average) in neuropsychomotor skills, which will positively impact intervention [6]. Moreover, it will also contribute to the assessment of the neuropsychomotor interventions [9,13,40]. The existence of such an instrument will also contribute to strengthening the psychomotor therapist’s (national and international) identity and the relevance of PMT application to several vulnerable groups [2,13].

5. Conclusions

This research aimed to present the cross-cultural adaptation of one of the most recent instruments in the PMT field, as well as the preliminary analysis of reliability and validity qualities. The NPmot.pt seems to be a reliable and valid instrument and could be used for assessing and screening the neuropsychomotor profile of children with and without developmental disorders, appearing to be useful in educational and therapeutic contexts, providing semiological and diagnostic indications, and giving guidance for the development of more precise children-based planning, within the PMT field. Our study goes beyond the mere translation or the proportion of experts’ agreement. The literature review allowed a major understanding of the construct that is intended to be measured that should be understood in the same way across language and cultural groups [52], being the foundation of valid cross-cultural comparisons [40,52]. Judgments of the construct-item match and suitability for the language groups involved were one of the main concerns, and the inclusion of an expert committee was essential for content validity analysis, also introducing empirical data. The equivalence of the structure of the test was guaranteed, and all items were considered representative. The battery is also reliable. However, particular attention should be given to Tactile Gnosis. Moderate to strong correlations and the potential organization of all indicators by eight domains seems to corroborate the multidimensional conceptual model.
The NPmot.pt distinguishes children with typical developmental from those with developmental disorders and points out the developmental trajectory during childhood. The Portuguese version comprises a quantitative record of the child’s performance and incorporates a qualitative holistic approach, aiming to screen minor cerebral disorders that might not be captured through a global performance assessment [70], but with an impact on learning functions (talking, writing, etc.). In the field of PMT, there is a need to have a gold standard that is consistently used across countries and age groups. It is equally important, to examine neuropsychomotor functions in a developmental approach, which promotes cross-cultural studies and group comparisons. Besides the individual profile, the use of valid data collected by the NPmot.pt will assist the assessment of the interventions’ effectiveness—through a comparison of scores over time and a more common language among PMT therapists across the world.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, N.D.A. and S.S.; methodology, N.D.A. and S.S.; software, N.D.A. and S.S.; validation: N.D.A. and S.S.; formal analysis, N.D.A., J.P. and S.S.; investigation, N.D.A. and S.S.; writing—original draft preparation: N.D.A.; writing—review and editing, S.S.; supervision, S.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study required ethical approval and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Associação Portuguesa de Psicomotricidade (protocol code: 2021/01 and approved 19 October 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent has been obtained from the patient(s) to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and analyzed in this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bellemans, T.; Didden, R.; Van Busschbach, J.T.; Hoek, P.T.A.P.; Scheffer, M.W.J.; Lang, R.B.; Lindsay, W.R. Psychomotor Therapy Targeting Anger and Aggressive Behaviour in Individuals with Mild or Borderline Intellectual Disabilities: A Systematic Review. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2019, 44, 121–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Santos, S. Psychomotor Therapy & Intelectual Disability: From 0 to 100. Int. J. Psychol. Neurosci. 2017, 3, 22–37. [Google Scholar]
  3. Maite, M.; Asunción, A.; Mònica, A.; Àngels, L.; Anna, R.; Mercè, X. Different Perceptive Qualities for the Psychomotricity. 2015. Available online: http://article.scieducationalresearch.com/pdf/education-3-12-19.pdf (accessed on 22 December 2021).
  4. Berdila, A.; Talaghir, L.G.; Iconomescu, T.M.; Rus, C.M.; Dunarea de Jos University of Galati, Romania. Values and Interferences of Psychomotricity in Education a Study of the Domain-Specific Literature. Rev. Rom. Pentru Educ. Multidimens. 2019, 11, 22–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. European Forum of Psychomotricity. Organisation Internationale de Psychomotricité et de Relaxation, & Red Fortaleza de Psicomotricidad; Declaración de París: Paris, France, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  6. Fonseca, V. Neuropsicomotricidade: Uma Abordagem à Luz Das Neurociências; Âncora Editores: Lisbon, Portugal, 2021; Volume 4. [Google Scholar]
  7. Probst, M. Psychomotor Therapy for Patients with Severe Mental Health Disorders; IntechOpen: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Emck, C.; de Lange, J.; Scheewe, T.; Busschbach, J.; Van Damme, T. Psychomotor Interventions for Mental Health. In Children & Adolescents—A Movement and Body; Boom: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  9. Romero Martínez, S.J.; Ordóñez Camacho, X.G.; Gil Madrona, P. Development of the Checklist of Psychomotor Activities for 5- to 6-Year-Old Children. Percept. Mot. Skills 2018, 125, 1070–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wuang, Y.-P.; Lin, Y.-H.; Su, C.-Y. Rasch Analysis of the Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency-Second Edition in Intellectual Disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2009, 30, 1132–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Frazão, A.; Santos, S.; Lebre, P. Psychomotor Intervention Practices for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Scoping Review. Rev. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2021, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Paquet, A.; Olliac, B.; Bouvard, M.-P.; Golse, B.; Vaivre-Douret, L. The Semiology of Motor Disorders in Autism Spectrum Disorders as Highlighted from a Standardized Neuro-Psychomotor Assessment. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Serrão, C.; Domingos, D.; Bagarrão, J.; Carrilho, J.; Santos, S. A Validação de Instrumentos e a Sua Importância Para a Área Da Reabilitação Psicomotora. Psicomotricidade, 2021; in press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Albaret, J.-M. Intérêt de La Mesure Dans l’examen Psychomoteur. Evol. Psychomot. 2003, 15, 65–74. [Google Scholar]
  15. Albaret, J. Évolution des terminologies et de la demarche diagnostique. Dossier—Bilan Psychomoteur-pour une nouvelle sémiologie et classification des troubles psychomoteurs. Évol. Psychomot. 2005, 17, 193–200. [Google Scholar]
  16. Albaret, J.-M. L’examen Psychomoteur Chez Les Jeunes Enfants Présentant Un Trouble Du Langage Oral. Rééduc. Orthophonique 2007, 45, 99–112. [Google Scholar]
  17. Albaret, J.-M. Place de l’examen psychomoteur dans l’évaluation neuropsychologique de l’enfant. Evol. Psychomot. 2009, 21, 10. [Google Scholar]
  18. Boscaini, F. The Specifity of Psychomotor Semiology for an Adequate Diagnosis. Psicomotricidade 2004, 3, 50–60. [Google Scholar]
  19. Boscaini, F. Pourquoi Une Sémiologie Spécifique Aux Psychomotriciens, Simplicité et Complexité. Dossier-Bilan Psychomoteur-pour une nouvelle sémiologie et classification des troubles psychomoteurs. Évol. Psychomot. 2005, 17, 88–100. [Google Scholar]
  20. Vaivre-Douret, L. NP-MOT—Batterie D´Evaluations des Fonctions Neuro-Psychomotrices D´Enfant, 4 Ans à 8 Ans 6 Mois—Manuel, 1 Éme; ECPA par Pearson: Paris, France, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  21. Smits-Engelsman, B.; Hill, E.L. The Relationship Between Motor Coordination and Intelligence Across the IQ Range. Pediatrics 2012, 130, e950–e956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Wassenberg, R.; Feron, F.J.M.; Kessels, A.G.H.; Hendriksen, J.G.M.; Kalff, A.C.; Kroes, M.; Hurks, P.P.M.; Beeren, M.; Jolles, J.; Vles, J.S.H. Relation Between Cognitive and Motor Performance in 5- to 6-Year-Old Children: Results From a Large-Scale Cross-Sectional Study. Child Dev. 2005, 76, 1092–1103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Rigoldi, C.; Galli, M.; Mainardi, L.; Crivellini, M.; Albertini, G. Postural Control in Children, Teenagers and Adults with Down Syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 170–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Lewis, C.L.; Fragala-Pinkham, M.A. Effects of Aerobic Conditioning and Strength Training on a Child with Down Syndrome: A Case Study. Pediatr. Phys. Ther. 2005, 17, 30–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Cleaver, S.; Hunter, D.; Ouellette-Kuntz, H. Physical Mobility Limitations in Adults with Intellectual Disabilities: A Systematic Review. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2009, 53, 93–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Carmeli, E.; Bar-Yossef, T.; Ariav, C.; Levy, R.; Liebermann, D.G. Perceptual-Motor Coordination in Persons with Mild Intellectual Disability. Disabil. Rehabil. 2008, 30, 323–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Vuijk, P.J.; Hartman, E.; Scherder, E.; Visscher, C. Motor Performance of Children with Mild Intellectual Disability and Borderline Intellectual Functioning: Motor Performance of Children with Intellectual Disabilities. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2010, 54, 955–965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Shum, S.B.M.; Pang, M.Y.C. Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Have Impaired Balance Function: Involvement of Somatosensory, Visual, and Vestibular Systems. J. Pediatr. 2009, 155, 245–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Geuze, R.H. Static Balance and Developmental Coordination Disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2003, 22, 527–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Oliveira, A.; Silva, M.A.; Vasconcelos, O. Preferência Manual e Proficiência Manual em Crianças com Síndrome de Down e Crianças Ditas Normais, em Idade Pré-Escolar. In Estudos em Desenvolvimento Motor da Criança II; Instituto Politécnico da Guarda: Guarda, Portugal, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  31. Dewey, D.; Cantell, M.; Crawford, S.G. Motor and Gestural Performance in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders, Developmental Coordination Disorder, and/or Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2007, 13, 246–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Miyahara, M.; Tsujii, M.; Hori, M.; Nakanishi, K.; Kageyama, H.; Sugiyama, T. Brief Report: Motor Incoordination in Children with Asperger Syndrome and Learning Disabilities. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1997, 27, 595–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Rogé, B. Manuel de l’Echelle de Développement Moteur de Lincoln-Oseretsky; Editios du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée: Paris, France, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  34. Albaret, J.; Noack, N. L´échelle de Coordenations Motrices de Charlop-Atwell: Étalonnage Sur Une Population D'enfants de 3 Ans 6 Mois à 6 Ans. Entret. Psychomot. 1993, 18–24. [Google Scholar]
  35. Henderson, E.; Sugden, A. Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC); Psicological Corporation: London, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  36. Amaya-Hernández, A.; Alvarez-Rayón, G.; Ortega-Luyando, M.; Mancilla-Díaz, J. Peer Influence in Preadolescents and Adolescents: A Predictor of Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating Behaviors. Available online: https://reader.elsevier.com/reader/sd/pii/S2007152316300301?token=FFF96A5EFD3BFE083060680FCF5EE44B51C10BFABB779A8AB60951C07A324E06D0D355931FB0B12B97ABB108CE4D178D&originRegion=eu-west-1&originCreation=20220616083958 (accessed on 16 June 2022).
  37. Rodrigues, A.; Duarte, C.; Correia, E.; Rosa, F.; Carvalho, J.; Morato, P. Teste de Proficiência Motora de Bruininks-Oseretsky, Segunda Edição. In Avaliação da Proficiência Motora nas Perturbações do Desenvolvimento; Edições FMH: Cruz-Quebrada, Portugal, 2005; pp. 5–24. [Google Scholar]
  38. Morais, A.; Santos, S.; Lebre, P.; Simões, C. Assessing Motor and Cognitive Areas in Older People: Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Portuguese Version of Éxamen Géronto-Psychomoteur. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 2020, 90, 50–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Hulteen, R.M.; Barnett, L.M.; True, L.; Lander, N.J.; del Pozo Cruz, B.; Lonsdale, C. Validity and Reliability Evidence for Motor Competence Assessments in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. J. Sports Sci. 2020, 38, 1717–1798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Wassel, J.I. A Review of Morais, Santos, and Lebre’s (2016) “Psychometric Properties of the Portuguese Version of the Éxamen Geronto-Psychomoteur” and the Critique of the Influence of Scholar Literature: A Call for Greater Responsibility. Educ. Gerontol. 2016, 42, 513–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Danişman, Ş.; Dereli İman, E.; Akın Demircan, Z.; Yaya, D. Examining the Psychometric Properties of the Emotional Regulation Checklist in 4- and 5-Year-Old Preschoolers. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2016, 14, 534–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Fonseca, V. Manual de Observação Psicomotora—Significação Psiconeurológica Dos Factores Psicomotores, 2nd ed.; Âncora Editores: Lisbon, Portugal, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  43. Pitteri, F. Psychomotor Evaluation. Psicomotricidade 2004, 3, 47–52. [Google Scholar]
  44. Pinto, M. Vigilância do desenvolvimento psicomotor e sinais de alarme. Rev. Port. Med. Geral E Fam. 2009, 25, 677–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Saint-Cast, A. Modalidades de Avaliação Do Perfil Psicomotor Da Criança. Psicomotricidade 2004, 4, 7–21. [Google Scholar]
  46. Vaivre-Douret, L. Contribution Au Diagnostic Par l’évaluation Standardisée Des Fonctions Neuro-Psychomotrices Chez l’enfant. Evol. Psychomot. 2009, 21, 10–17. [Google Scholar]
  47. Berruezo, P.; Adelantado, J. El Contenido de La Psicomotricidad: Reflexiones Para La Delimitación de Su Âmbito Teórico y Práctico. The Content of Psychomotricity: Reflections for the Delimitation of Its Theoretical and Practical Scope. Rev. Interuniv. Form. Profr. 2008, 62, 19–34. [Google Scholar]
  48. Vaivre-Douret, L.; Lalanne, C. Specific Impairments and Predictive Markers for Developmental Coordination Disorder Subtypes in Children: The Importance of Multidimensional Developmental Assessments in Cluster Analysis. J. Transl. Sci. 2020, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Guimarães, S.C.M.; dos Santos, A.S.P.G.; Lebre, P. Validade e Confiabilidade da Versão Portuguesa da Brief Motor Scale em Pessoas com Esquizofrenia: Um Estudo Preliminar. Rev. Port. Psiquiatr. E Saúde Ment. 2020, 6, 109–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Morais, A.; Santos, S.; Lebre, P. Psychometric Properties of the Portuguese Version of the Éxamen Geronto-Psychomoteur (P-EGP). Educ. Gerontol. 2016, 42, 516–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Vaivre-Douret, L. Batterie d’évaluations des fonctions neuro-psychomotrices de l’enfant. Carnet PSY 2007, 115, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. The ITC Guidelines for Translating and Adapting Tests. Available online: https://www.intestcom.org/files/guideline_test_adaptation_2ed.pdf (accessed on 22 December 2021).
  53. Noonan, V.K.; Miller, W.C.; Noreau, L.; The SCIRE Research Team. A Review of Instruments Assessing Participation in Persons with Spinal Cord Injury. Spinal Cord 2009, 47, 435–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Pestana, M.; Gageiro, J. Análise de Dados Para Ciências Sociais: A Complementaridade Do SPSS; Edições Sílabo: Lisbon, Portugal, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  55. Polit, D.F.; Beck, C.T. The Content Validity Index: Are You Sure You Know What’s Being Reported? Critique and Recommendations. Res. Nurs. Health 2006, 29, 489–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Yaghmale, F. Content Validity and Its Estimation. J. Med. Educ. 2003, 3. [Google Scholar]
  57. Marôco, J. Análise Estatística Com Utilização Do SPSS; Report Number Editores: Lisbon, Portugal, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  58. Wynd, C.A.; Schmidt, B.; Schaefer, M.A. Two Quantitative Approaches for Estimating Content Validity. West. J. Nurs. Res. 2003, 25, 508–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Viera, A.J.; Garrett, J.M. Understanding Interobserver Agreement: The Kappa Statistic. Fam. Med. 2005, 37, 360–363. [Google Scholar]
  60. Nunnally, J. Psychometric Theory, 2nd ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  61. Marôco, J. Análise Estatística Com o SPSS Statistics; Report Number Editores: Lisbon, Portugal, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  62. Watkins, M.W. Interobserver Agreement in Behavioral Research: Importance and Calculation. J. Behav. Educ. 2001, 10, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. DeVon, H.A.; Block, M.E.; Moyle-Wright, P.; Ernst, D.M.; Hayden, S.J.; Lazzara, D.J.; Savoy, S.M.; Kostas-Polston, E. A Psychometric Toolbox for Testing Validity and Reliability. J. Nurs. Scholarsh. 2007, 39, 155–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Eckert, H. Comportamento Motor Na Fase Precoce Da Infância. In Desenvolvimento Motor; Editora Manole LTDA: São Paulo, Brazil, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  65. Odeh, C.E.; Gladfelter, A.L.; Stoesser, C.; Roth, S. Comprehensive Motor Skills Assessment in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Yields Global Deficits. Int. J. Dev. Disabil. 2022, 68, 290–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Paquet, A.; Olliac, B.; Vaivre-Douret, L. Évaluation des fonctions neuro-psychomotrices chez des enfants avec un trouble du spectre de l’autisme: Entre possibilité et nécessité. Neuropsychiatr. Enfance Adolesc. 2019, 67, 10–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Paquet, A.; Olliac, B.; Golse, B.; Vaivre-Douret, L. Evaluation of Neuromuscular Tone Phenotypes in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Exploratory Study. Neurophysiol. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 47, 261–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Brown, T. Structural Validity of the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency—Second Edition Brief Form (BOT-2-BF). Res. Dev. Disabil. 2019, 85, 92–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Fenn, J.; Tan, C.-S.; George, S. Development, Validation and Translation of Psychological Tests. In BJPsych Advances; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2020; Volume 26. [Google Scholar]
  70. Vaivre-Douret, L.; Paquet, A. L’évaluation standardisée développementale des fonctions neuro-psychomotrices: Présentation de la batterie normée française NP-MOT. Neurophysiol. Clin. 2019, 49, 421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Content Validity Indexes (CVI) of NPmot.pt (items and scale).
Table 1. Content Validity Indexes (CVI) of NPmot.pt (items and scale).
RelevanceClaritySimplicityAmbiguity
Item 10.890.671.000.56
Item 50.890.440.670.56
Item 60.890.670.890.67
Item 70.890.220.560.33
Item 90.890.780.890.89
Item 130.780.670.560.44
Item 320.780.440.220.44
Item 330.780.670.780.78
Item 340.780.780.780.78
AU/IVC-E0.830.100.370.29
M/IVC-E0.970.780.870.82
Table 2. Proportion of agreement among experts.
Table 2. Proportion of agreement among experts.
RelevanceClaritySimplicityAmbiguity
E110.8110.80
E20.9610.981
E30.960.940.940.77
E410.920.980.98
E50.980.420.800.58
E60.980.800.860.80
E70.980.670.880.73
E810.630.810.81
E91111
Table 3. Content validity—Cohen’s kappa.
Table 3. Content validity—Cohen’s kappa.
Experts123456789
110.210.560.250.770.620.630.670.73
2 10.920.790.760.940.950.950.96
3 10.300.770.670.690.620
4 10.930.870.900.900
5 110.050.210.21
6 10.210.540.57
7 10.100.02
8 10.45
9 1
Table 4. NPmot.pt reliability indexes.
Table 4. NPmot.pt reliability indexes.
DomainsTest-
Retest (r)
(n = 50)
Internal Consistency α (n = 200)Split-Half
(n = 200)
Correlation Coefficient Spearman-Brown (n = 200)Correlation Coefficient Guttman (n = 200)
Tonus0.850.8700.880.990.92
Gross Motor Skills0.900.900.900.950.86
Laterality11111
Manual l Praxis0.930.930.930.860.79
Hand-eye coordination0.790.790.790.810.79
Tactile Gnosis0.450.450.450.460.45
Spatial Orientation0.910.910.910.910.89
Rhythm0.920.920.920.920.92
Auditive Attention11111
Total 0.860.860.860.880.85
Table 5. NPmot.pt domains’s intercorrelations (n = 200).
Table 5. NPmot.pt domains’s intercorrelations (n = 200).
DomainsTGPLMPTGHECSORIAA
Pearson’s Coefficients Correlations
Tonus1
Gross Motor Skills0.481
Laterality0.310.541
Manual Praxis0.430.890.411
Tactile Gnosis0.270.630.530.541
Hand-eye coordination 0.310.680.380.630.471
Spatial Orientation0.390.920.440.890.600.601
Rhythm0.570.910.570.800.660.690.801
Auditive Attention0.480.770.470.650.560.590.700.801
Total0.470.760.520.690.580.590.700.760.67
p < 0.05.
Table 6. NPmot.pt exploratory factorial analysis (structure matrix; n = 200).
Table 6. NPmot.pt exploratory factorial analysis (structure matrix; n = 200).
Component MatrixComponent Factor Loadings
12345678
Spatial_orientation_own0.695
Spatial_orientation_other0.751
2_Objects_spatial_orientation0.721
Objects_spatial_orientation0.721
Map_spatial_orientation0.715
Spatial_orientation_total0.845
Spontaneous _time0.863
Auditory visual kinesthetic0.783
Audio_perceptual_ motors0.877
Rhythm_gait0.877
Rhythm _total0.926
Test beat_quality0.797
Test beat_duration0.797
Auditive_attention_total0.797
Passivity pulse resistance0.502 0.555
Passivity on resistance footing0.482 0.456
Shoulder strength extensibility0.406 0.562
Pulse extensibility endurance0.544 0.571
Popliteal extensibility0.6680.706
Adductor extensibility0.6800.705
Heel-ear resistance0.6800.705
ankle contraction0.6850.687
Extensibility foot with bent leg0.6800.705
Extensibility foot with stretched leg0.6800.705
Trunk extensibility0.6800.705
Rotulien_reflexes_right_leg0.623.597
Rotulien_reflexes_left_leg0.6230.597
Passive_mobilization0.6850.687
Residual_tonus _total0.7520.627
Support_ tonus_ total0.6850.687
Attitude_tonus _total0.4690.053 0.383
diadochokinesis pronation_supination0.6850.687
Open_close_hands0.4860.428
Open_close_mouth0.4860.428
Syncinese_3tests0.6850.687
Syncinese0.6850.687
Action_Tonus _total0.6060.563
Tonus_total0.7610.635
Dynamic balance0.861
Spontaneous gait0.279 0.597
Walking to the front in a line0.768 0.124
Walking to back in a line0.531 0.097
Tiptoe gait0.815 0.068
Heels gait0.861 0.067
8 inches/20 cm jump0.720 0.083
Upper and lower limbs coordination0.720 0.083
Static balance0.942 0.145
Immobility0.886 0.066
Single-leg support0.729 0.332
Tiptoe immobility0.817 0.291
Global_Motricity_total0.915 0.078
Symmetrical bimanual supination pronation0.843 0.048
Asymmetrical bimanual supination pronation0.842 0.061
Bimanual supination pronation0.690 0.131
Forefinger_thumb0.717 0.107
Thumb opposition0.694 0.111
Manual and digital Praxis_total0.856 0.081
Tactile_Gnosis_right_hand0.822 0.437
Tactile_Gnosis_left_hand 0.5290.500
Tactile_Gnosis_total0.609 0.419
Hand_eye performance right hand0.743 0.190
Hand_eye performance left hand0.511 0.409
Hand-eye Coordination_total0.666 0.343
Gestual_Laterality0.543 0.429
Laterality_total0.543 0.429
Table 7. Means, standard deviation and Student’s t-test of NPmot.pt domains by gender and diagnosis (n = 200).
Table 7. Means, standard deviation and Student’s t-test of NPmot.pt domains by gender and diagnosis (n = 200).
NPmot.ptGender Female
(n = 105)
Gender Male
(n = 95)
Group with DD
(n = 50)
Group without DD
(n = 150)
M ± sdM ± sdpM ± sdM ± sdp
Tonus34.29 ± 5.0233.78 ± 5.140.4828.92 ± 7.8035.75 ± 1.63<0.01
Gross Motor Skills12.47 ± 3.8912.0 ± 4.50.436.24 ± 1.7114.25 ± 2.51<0.01
Laterality4.85 ± 0.044.9 ± 0.250.044.56 ± 0.55.00 ± 0.00<0.01
Manual Praxis6.06 ± 2.86.06 ± 2.330.883.02 ± 1.007.03 ± 2.09<0.01
Hand-eye coordination2.91 ± 1.42.64 ± 0.820.121.34 ± 0.713.27 ± 0.9<0.01
Tactile Gnosis2.80 ± 1.542.3 ± 1.550.060.82 ± 0.923.18 ± 1.24<0.01
Spatial Orientation2.82 ± 1.632.66 ± 1.720.510.74 ± 0.753.41 ± 1.33<0.01
Rhythm3.74 ± 1.603.19 ± 1.910.030.54 ± 0.54.46 ± 0.50<0.01
Auditive Attention3.14 ± 1.462.76 ± 2.030.220.52 ± 0.893.77 ± 1.10<0.01
Total8.12 ± 2.157.81 ± 2.060.315.19 ± 1.648.90 ± 1.26<0.01
p < 0.05; DD = developmental disorders.
Table 8. Mean and standard deviation of NPmot.pt domains by age.
Table 8. Mean and standard deviation of NPmot.pt domains by age.
NPmot.pt4.0–4.9
(nTP = 29)
4.10–5.8
(n = 41; nTP = 36; nDD = 5
5.9–6.6
(nTP = 7)
6.7–7.5
(n = 48 nTP = 37 e nDD = 11)
7.6–8.5
(n = 52 nTP = 41 e nDD = 11)
>8.5
(n = 23 nTP = 0 e nDD = 23)
M ± sdM ± sdM ± sdM ± sdM ± sdM ± sd
Tonus35.72 ± 0.8234.10 ± 5.1736.00 ± 0.0034.10 ± 4.9935.13 ± 2.7628.65 ± 8.62
Gross Motor Skills11.48 ± 0.6311.51 ± 1.2711.71 ± 0.4914.08 ± 3.9514.60 ± 4.705.52 ± 1.12
Laterality5.00 ± 0.005.00 ± 0.005.00 ± 0.005.00 ± 0.004.87 ± 0.354.35 ± 0.49
Manual and Digital Praxis4.93 ± 0.254.66 ± 0.945.43 ± 0.797.31 ± 2.627.67 ± 3.013.65 ± 0.78
Hand-eye coordination3.07 ± 0.262.93 ± 0.413.29 ± 0.492.94 ± 1.663.06 ± 1.041.09 ± 0.79
Tactile Gnosis2.97 ± 0.262.93 ± 0.413.29 ± 0.492.94 ± 1.663.06 ± 1.041.09 ± 0.79
Spatial Orientation2.00 ± 0.001.95 ± 0.223.43 ± 0.983.46 ± 1.464.04 ± 1.800.48 ± 0.67
Rhythm4.00 ± 0.003.54 ± 1.274.00 ± 0.004.00 ± 1.823.83 ± 1.940.70 ± 0.47
Auditive Attention3.79 ± 0.622.68 ± 1.463.71 ± 0.763.25 ± 1.963.35 ± 1.760.70 ± 0.97
Total8.11 ± 0.327.7 ± 1.118.43 ± 0.448.56 ± 2.248.85 ± 2.045.14 ± 1.63
TP = typical development; DD = developmental disorders.
Table 9. Post-hoc Scheffe test scores for the age ranges.
Table 9. Post-hoc Scheffe test scores for the age ranges.
NPmot.ptG1 vs. 2G1 vs. 3G1 vs. 4G.1 vs. 5G1 vs. 6G2 vs. 3G2 vs. 4G2 vs. 5G2 vs. 6G3 vs. 4G 3 vs. 5G3 vs. 6G 4 vs.5G4 vs. 6G5 vs. 6
T0.8410.831<0.0010.9610.95<0.0010.9610.030.94<0.001<0.001
GM110.04<0.001<0.00110.02<0.001<0.0010.640.41<0.0010.99<0.001<0.001
L1110.33<0.001110.22<0.00110.86<0.0010.17<0.001<0.001
MDP11<0.001<0.0010.430.98<0.001<0.0010.630.420.210.561<0.001<0.001
TG1111<0.0010.9811<0.0010.981<0.0011<0.001<0.001
HEC0.580.7611<0.00110.240.450.040.620.750.751<0.001<0.001
SO10.17<0.001<0.001<0.0010.12<0.001<0.001<0.00110.90<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
R0.89111<0.0010.990.8200.97<0.00111<0.0011<0.001<0.001
AA0.1210.810.90<0.0010.750.700.51<0.00111<0.0011<0.001<0.001
Total0.830.880.630.580.0490.860.530.460.080.850.790.150.79<0.001<0.001
p < 0.05; Subtitle: Group 1 = 4.0–4.9 years; Group 2 = 4.10–5.8 years; Group 3 = 5.9–6.6 years; Group 4 = 6.7–7.5 years; Group 5 = 7.6–8.4 years; Group 6 ≥ 8.5 years; T = Tonus; GM = Gross Motor Skills; L = Laterality; MDP = Manual and Digital Praxis; TG = Tactile Gnosis; HEC = Hand-eye coordination; SO = Spatial Orientation; R = Rhythm; AA = Auditive Attention.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Amorim, N.D.; Parreiral, J.; Santos, S. The Assessment of the Psychomotor Profile in Children: Preliminary Psychometric Analysis of the Portuguese Version of the Batterie d’Evaluation des Fonctions Neuropsychomotrices de L’enfant (NPmot.pt). Children 2022, 9, 1195. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081195

AMA Style

Amorim ND, Parreiral J, Santos S. The Assessment of the Psychomotor Profile in Children: Preliminary Psychometric Analysis of the Portuguese Version of the Batterie d’Evaluation des Fonctions Neuropsychomotrices de L’enfant (NPmot.pt). Children. 2022; 9(8):1195. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081195

Chicago/Turabian Style

Amorim, Nídia De, José Parreiral, and Sofia Santos. 2022. "The Assessment of the Psychomotor Profile in Children: Preliminary Psychometric Analysis of the Portuguese Version of the Batterie d’Evaluation des Fonctions Neuropsychomotrices de L’enfant (NPmot.pt)" Children 9, no. 8: 1195. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081195

APA Style

Amorim, N. D., Parreiral, J., & Santos, S. (2022). The Assessment of the Psychomotor Profile in Children: Preliminary Psychometric Analysis of the Portuguese Version of the Batterie d’Evaluation des Fonctions Neuropsychomotrices de L’enfant (NPmot.pt). Children, 9(8), 1195. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9081195

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop