Next Article in Journal
Self-Declared Physical Activity Levels and Self-Reported Physical Fitness in a Sample of Italian Adolescents during the COVID-19 Pandemic
Next Article in Special Issue
Analysis of Spanish Parents’ Knowledge about ASD and Their Attitudes towards Inclusive Education
Previous Article in Journal
Body Composition and Nutrients Dietary Intake Changes during COVID-19 Lockdown in Spanish Healthy Postmenopausal Women
Previous Article in Special Issue
Do Faculty Members Apply the Standards for Developing Gifted Students at Universities? An Exploratory Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Impact of Using Facemasks on Literacy Learning: The Perception of Early Childhood Education Teachers

by
Diego Vergara
1,*,
Álvaro Antón-Sancho
2,
Juan-José Maldonado
3 and
María Nieto-Sobrino
3
1
Department of Mechanical Engineering, Catholic University of Ávila, C/Canteros, s/n, 05005 Avila, Spain
2
Department of Mathematics and Experimental Science, Catholic University of Ávila, C/Canteros, s/n, 05005 Avila, Spain
3
Department of Education, Catholic University of Ávila, C/Canteros, s/n, 05005 Avila, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2022, 12(6), 639-654; https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060048
Submission received: 7 April 2022 / Revised: 11 June 2022 / Accepted: 15 June 2022 / Published: 17 June 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Factors Related to School Coexistence at Different Educational Stages)

Abstract

:
In this work, quantitative research is carried out on the importance that educators give to literacy work in early childhood education classrooms and the impact that the COVID-19 pandemic and the use of facemasks have had on it. To this end, a survey designed for this purpose has been used, which has been passed on to a set of 112 Spanish early childhood educators. The teachers surveyed occupy different positions in the classroom (tutors, support technicians, specialists in bilingualism, therapeutic pedagogy and speech and hearing), and, in addition, they themselves learned to read from different methods of literacy learning (synthetic or analytical). The results found in this study indicate that educators express intermediate evaluations of the importance of literacy work in the classroom, higher if it is done through digital resources, and higher for the synthetic method than for the analytical method. In addition, the impact of the use of masks on literacy learning was rated as very negative. On the other hand, gaps have been identified in the above perceptions by the position occupied in the classroom and by the method used to learn to read. Finally, some actions are suggested to homogenize the perceptions of the different professionals, and some lines of research are proposed.

1. Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic situation has been a turning point in daily life, resulting in major changes in daily, social, and work habits [1,2,3]. Among these transformations, the imposition of the mandatory use of facemasks in many countries as a sanitary measure stands out [4]. This fact has not left the educational field indifferent, which has been affected by the alteration of word and emotion recognition processes [5,6].
Language acquisition is a natural process that occurs in early childhood, which is directly related to the child’s socioemotional development [7]. It should be noted that, from about 2 years old, children undergo a language learning process, most of which takes place in the classroom [8]. However, after the pandemic situation, this learning process may have been truncated by the use of facemasks by teachers [5].
After a period based on online teaching, especially in the early stages of the pandemic, the return to schools has meant the adoption of safety and hygiene measures to prevent the spread of the virus [9], highlighting the mandatory use of facemasks in enclosed spaces. These precautions have been treated to different extents depending on the educational level or specialty, with the exclusion of teachers, who have not been able to do without them [10]. This fact has led to a new challenge for the teaching–learning process due to the sound attenuation caused by the use of the facemask by the early childhood education teachers, but also due to the lack of visibility of a large part of the interlocutor’s face [11].
This visual–gestural component is part of the basis for optimal communication between transmitter and receiver. In this sense, the sound affection caused using the facemask fluctuates according to its typology, oscillating for surgical facemasks around 4 dB, while for FFP2 around 12 dB [12]. In the same way, the loss of vision of part of the sender’s face and facial expressions affects the correct understanding of the message since nonverbal language is also affected [13].
This attenuation of sound in the transmission of the message causes various effects in the sender, e.g., vocal and respiratory efforts that cause a state of fatigue in the subject [11,14]. This fact translates in the educational environment into a communication whose fundamental component is that of a state of anxiety, as well as a lack of understanding due to the use of shorter messages, and lack of information [14]. This, in turn, has repercussions on the correct development of literacy, understood as the process of learning to read and write, since knowledge of oral vocabulary directly improves students’ reading skills [15,16]. Literacy is an educational area that is usually worked on in early childhood education (educational stage from 3 to 6 years of age), through two main methods: synthetic and analytical (Figure 1). Synthetic is referred to the teaching that starts from the simplest to the most complex, i.e., begins with the study of phonemes (sounds) associated with letters and syllables, which allows for greater articulation of speech during the reading process and better decoding of words, especially improving the recognition of new words, which is a more effective method in the teaching of literacy. Conversely, analytical refers to the process by which children learn from more complex structures, that is, from the global (words and phrases) to the simplest structures (letters and syllables), improving the child’s autonomy in this process. This can be a challenge for children since, during the literacy learning process, they must distinguish the parts from the whole, which makes the process more complicated. Both approaches to literacy are affected by the visual and phonological impact of the use of facemasks [16,17].
In this regard, the use of transparent facemasks in the teaching-learning process seems to be a resource that solves part of the limitations in communication generated by opaque facemasks, since the visual signals of the communication process are exposed [18]. This favors the relationships established in the classroom, but also the learning of language from imitation and phoneme recognition at initial levels [5,19]. However, the sound affection is reduced in this type of facemasks, being up to 13.3 dB in those that have a window, and around 21 dB in those that are completely transparent [19].
Despite this, in noisy situations, as may be the case in a kindergarten classroom, the use of transparent facemasks is strongly recommended, since the sound attenuation is compensated by a better view of the facial field of the transmitter. This type of facemask in turn leads to access to components that allow the message to be deciphered, e.g., lip reading or the visualization of facial expressions that directly expose the emotional state of the subject [20]. This means breaking down the physical barrier that impedes the intelligibility of the message, providing the receiver with a state of confidence and peace of mind in view of the possibility of having more resources for greater understanding in the communication process [20]. On the other hand, although this type of transparent facemask has the disadvantage of being susceptible to generate a “fog” effect due to the respiratory process, this can be solved with both homemade and commercially available correctors for this purpose [5,20,21].
Even so, to counteract the sound affectation caused by facemasks, the educational world has often resorted to the use of microphones to improve communication [21], which has given better results—in general—with transparent facemasks, as they present a higher level of perception of the orofacial system [20]. However, it should be noted that for a correct understanding and cognitive processing of speech to occur, it is necessary to see the entire face of the sender without any physical barrier [22].
In addition to this circumstance, the use of the facemask by early childhood education students, who, despite not being obliged to use it—unlike teachers—have shown great frustration due to communication and socialization problems between student–student and student–teacher [23]. In this regard, another aggravating factor is the alteration of the correct pronunciation of phonemes, which will hinder the teaching work both at a general and specific level for the improvement of speech and language disorders [24], which occurs in speech and hearing (SH) or therapeutic pedagogy (TP) classrooms.
Since the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, there have been publications warning of the difficulties that the introduction of facemasks in schools posed for education, at the level of communication, interpretation of language and expression of emotions [25]. Some of these studies have focused on demonstrating the negative impact that the use of facemasks has at the academic level on the learning of some specific areas among adolescent students, such as physical education [26,27], although no adverse medical effects on children’s or adults’ breathing have been described, although respiratory discomfort has been reported [28]. However, several studies have analyzed the impact of using facemasks on the management and expression of emotions and affect in preschool and elementary school children and young people in secondary school.
In particular, it has been shown that facemasks have made it difficult for children to express feelings of disgust and have made them more prone to anxiety and fear [29], an effect that is particularly intense in children in early childhood education [30]. In adolescents, an increase in anxiety derived from the use of masks has been observed [31]. As for preschool, there are studies that explore the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on early childhood education, but they are usually focused on describing the effects of the migration of education to virtual environments [32,33], or in exposing the experience teachers have had and the impact of the pandemic at the occupational level [34]. Other papers explore teachers’ and parents’ views on the overall impact of the pandemic, concluding that it has had a negative effect on the overall development of children in early childhood education, particularly in science, mathematics, and art [35]. However, as far as it has been possible to explore, there are no studies that analyze the impact that the use of masks has had, within the global phenomenon of the COVID-19 pandemic, on the concrete learning of reading and writing in early childhood education. Moreover, the perspective of early childhood teachers in this regard has also not been the subject of intense analysis in the preceding literature.
This way, this research is focused on the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic, particularly the use of facemasks, on literacy learning in early childhood education (3 to 6 years of age). Specifically, a statistical analysis is made of the opinion of early childhood education teachers in Spain regarding the use of facemasks in the classroom and the problems this generates in relation to the teaching–learning process of literacy at this educational stage. The study distinguishes the opinions of the different generalist and specialist teachers involved in the teaching of literacy and also differentiates by the method (synthetic or analytical) with which the participating teachers learned to read and write.
Specifically, the general objective of this research is to describe the opinion of early childhood education professionals on (i) literacy work in the classroom, (ii) the different methods for teaching it, and (iii) the influence of the COVID-19 pandemic and the use of facemasks on the development of literacy in students. In particular, the following specific objectives are pursued in this work: (i) to analyze the assessments of Spanish early childhood education teachers about the importance of working on literacy in the classroom and of doing so through digital resources; (ii) to study the assessments made by these professionals of the effectiveness of each of the possible methods of working on literacy (synthetic and analytical); (iii) to examine the influence of the use of facemasks on the achievement of literacy objectives in early childhood education during the COVID-19 pandemic; (iv) to identify differences in the above assessments by the teacher’s position in the classroom and, within each possible position, by the method with which the teacher learned to read; and (v) to analyze whether the COVID-19 pandemic has altered participants’ perception of the need for classroom reinforcement with a speech-language pathologist.
Two independent variables are considered in the research (Figure 2): (i) position occupied in the classroom; and (ii) literacy learning method with which the participant learned to read. The position occupied in the classroom is the main independent variable. It is a polytomous variable whose values reach all possible early childhood education professionals working in the field of literacy: (i) teachers specializing in bilingualism; (ii) SH specialists; (iii) TP specialists; (iv) early childhood education teachers acting as group tutors; and (v) early childhood education technicians acting as support staff. The variable measuring the method by which participants learned to read is a dichotomous variable whose possible values are the analytical method or the synthetic method.
The following dependent variables are defined as quantitative variables measured on a scale of 1 to 5:
  • Importance given by the participants to the teaching of literacy in early childhood education;
  • Importance of the use of digital resources and information and communication technologies (ICT) in the process of teaching literacy in early childhood education;
  • Assessment of the effectiveness of the analytical method in learning literacy;
  • Assessment of the effectiveness of the synthetic method in learning literacy;
  • Assessment of the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the development of literacy in early childhood education students;
  • Degree of negative impact of the use of facemasks on the development of literacy in early childhood education students;
  • Perception of the need for speech-language intervention in the classroom before and after the pandemic.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study involves 112 active Spanish early childhood education professionals, including generalist teachers and technicians and also specialists in different educational fields related to literacy. Specifically, among the participants there are group tutors and support technicians, teachers specializing in bilingualism, and teachers specializing in SH (Teacher of the Speech and Hearing Handicapped, TSHH) or TP. Participants were chosen through a non-probability convenience sampling process.
The distribution of the proportions of each of the above positions in the sample is shown in Figure 3. As can be noted, there is a notable majority of teachers acting as group tutors. The distribution of participants according to the method by which they learned to read is also shown in Figure 3. Most of the participating teachers learned through the synthetic method. To conclude the description of the sociological profile of the participants, it is noteworthy that there is a large majority of females (91.07%) versus males (8.93%), the latter being only classroom tutors. Almost half of the participants are between 41 and 50 years old (46.43%), 33.04% are 40 years old or younger and 20.53% are over 50 years old. Only 6.25% are under 30 years old. Regarding teaching experience, 75.00% of the participants have more than 10 years of teaching experience, and 87.50% have more than 5 years of experience. Therefore, they have, in general, extensive teaching experience.

2.2. Instrument

In this research, a survey designed for this purpose has been used to delimit the sociological and professional profile of the participants and to obtain their opinions on the variables studied. In the survey, the following aspects of the sociological and educative profile of the participants are assessed: (i) gender; (ii) age; (iii) position held in the early childhood education classroom; (iv) teaching experience; and (v) method by which the participant learned to read (synthetic or analytical). Participants are then asked a set of 15 Likert-type questions valued from 1 to 5 to measure the teachers’ perception of the dependent variables analyzed (Table 1). In the answers to these questions, 1 means the lowest valuation and 5 is the highest valuation.
The validation of the instrument was carried out by means of a factor analysis, the computation of internal consistency parameters and an assessment of convergent validation by means of Pearson’s correlation coefficients. The answers of the first 13 questions of the survey (Table 1) were subjected to an EFA with Varimax rotation, which has determined the existence of six latent factors that explain this part of the survey (Table 2). The factor weights of questions 1, 4, 5, 10, and 13 are greater than 0.90, so the answers to these questions can be considered redundant and, consequently, have been removed from the analyzed results. The six-factor theoretical model derived from the EFA allows explaining the survey results with statistical significance (chi-square = 5.59, p-value = 0.78) and explain 64.3% of the variance (Table 3). The factors identified in this theoretical model correspond exactly to the first six dependent variables defined in the study. The last dependent variable (which measures the perception of the need for speech-language intervention in early childhood education before and after the pandemic) has been assessed by comparing the mean answers to questions 14 and 15 of the survey.
The statistics of the CFA endorse the theoretical model derived from the EFA. Indeed, the incremental fit indices are appropriate (AGFI = 0.8786; NFI = 0.8744; TLI = 0.9650; CFI = 0.9783; IFI = 0.9800) and the absolute fit indices are also good (GFI = 0.9375; RMSEA = 0.0403; AIC = 103.6463; chi-square/df = 1.1661). The validation of the psychometric dimensions of the survey has been tested based on Pearson’s correlation coefficients (Table 4). These correlations are weak between the different variables analyzed, but strong for each of the variables with respect to the survey. All the computed coefficients are statistically significant, with a significance level of 0.05.

2.3. Procedure

In this work, quantitative research is performed based on the answers to the survey that has been passed to the participating teachers. They were contacted by email and were given access to the survey instrument through GoogleFormsTM. The teachers answered to the survey voluntarily, freely, and anonymously. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the research project to which the authors belong. All the answers were validated (an answer was considered valid when it was complete). The validation of the instrument was conducted after the response collection process before statistical analysis of results.

2.4. Data Analysis

The validation of the survey was carried out by means of an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) of the answers, which served to identify the latent factors that explain the survey. Questions 14 and 15 of the survey have not been introduced in the EFA because the intention is assessing the last dependent variable of the study (perception of the need for speech-language intervention) by comparing the mean results of question 14 (perception before the pandemic) and question 15 (perception after the pandemic). The theoretical model derived from the EFA was confirmed by means of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) statistics. The validation of the psychometric dimensions was achieved through the computation of Pearson correlation coefficients of the different factors defined by the EFA among themselves and with respect to the global survey. The descriptive statistics of the answers of the first 13 questions of the survey (means and standard deviations) have then been analyzed, both globally and differentiating the participants according to their position in the classroom. The ANOVA test was applied to identify significant differences in the answers by position held. In the variables where such differences exist, the multifactor ANOVA test (MANOVA) has been applied to analyze whether, within each position, there are differences in the corresponding assessments due to the method by which the participants learned to read. Finally, the mean values of questions 14 and 15 were compared using the t-test and the ANOVA test (when the sample is differentiated by the position occupied in the classroom). In all tests, 0.05 has been taken as the level of statistical significance.

3. Results

In Table 5 it is shown that, in general, the survey respondents give an intermediate importance to literacy learning in early childhood education, but that they consider the synthetic method to be more efficient than the analytical method. Both in the assessment of literacy and in that of teaching methods, the deviations are high (above a quarter of the mean). On the other hand, the valuation of the use of digital resources for literacy learning is high and with a moderate deviation. The participants believe that the use of facemasks during the pandemic has had a very negative influence on the development of literacy in early childhood education students, with a low standard deviation. Participants’ assessment of the impact of the pandemic on the students’ literacy development during the pandemic is intermediate.
Differentiating the participants by the position they occupy in the classroom, significant differences are observed in the mean values of the importance of literacy in early childhood education and of the influence of the use of facemasks on the students’ literacy development (Table 6). SH and TP specialists are the ones who give the least importance to literacy work in the classroom, while teacher tutors and technicians give the highest valuations in this regard. Bilingualism teachers are the professionals who most intensely warn of the negative effects of facemasks on literacy.
Next, the behavior of the participants’ opinions is analyzed for the two independent variables that show significant differences according to the teacher’s position in the classroom (importance of literacy and negative influence of the use of facemasks). Specifically, Figure 4 and Figure 5 show the mean values of the participants for each of these variables, differentiating by the method with which they themselves learned to read. As can be observed, in all positions, those who learned to read with the analytical method give a higher value to literacy than those who learned with the synthetic method, except among TP specialists (Figure 4). These differences are statistically significant (MANOVA’s F = 4.1434, p-value = 0.0039). Among group tutors, there is no difference in the evaluation of the importance of literacy when the sample is differentiated by the method used to learn to read. However, the participants who learned to read with the synthetic method are those who perceive a more negative influence of the use of the facemasks on the development of literacy in all positions and with significant differences (MANOVA’s F = 4.0928, p-value = 0.0459), except for the bilingualism specialists, among whom no differences by learning method are appreciated (Figure 5).
In response to questions 14 and 15 (Table 1), that value the perception of the need for speech-language intervention, participants assessed with a mean of 3.30 their need for such speech therapy in the early childhood classroom before the pandemic, and with a 3.80 their perception of it after the pandemic. The t-test statistics (t = −3.3731, p-value = 0.0009) found the difference between the means to be significant. Therefore, the pandemic has led to a significant increase in teachers’ assessment of the need for speech-language intervention in the early childhood classroom. In this regard, the ANOVA test does not identify statistically significant differences by position in the classroom (Table 7), so that the increase in mean perception is approximately homogeneous across all positions.

4. Discussion

Numerous studies have shown that the use of face masks during the COVID-19 pandemic has caused difficulties in children to adapt to them, problems of functional discomfort [10], compatibility with the use of glasses or breathing difficulties [26,36] and even psychological problems, such as anxiety or sadness [5,6,18,37,38]. Educators have warned about the negative influence that the use of facemasks has, in particular, on the proper development of children’s social life and their interaction with others [39,40], on the development of communication skills [13,21], on sound recognition and vocal self-perception [11,14,19,20], and on the development of speech skills [11,12,41]. This paper has analyzed the influence of the COVID-19 pandemic and, particularly, the use of facemasks, on the development of literacy objectives in early childhood education students, from the point of view of educators. In the analysis, emphasis has been placed on the identification of differences in the perceptions expressed due to the different specialties exercised by teachers in the early childhood education classroom.
The results show that, in general, educators give intermediate–high assessments to literacy learning in early childhood education, with these valuations being higher if digital resources are introduced (Table 5). In addition, the pandemic has led to a significant increase in educators’ assessment of the need for speech-language intervention in the classroom. This increase is approximately homogeneous across the different positions occupied by teachers in the classroom (Table 7). The results regarding the use of digital resources are in line with previous work that confirms the good learning results that this type of tool achieves in children in early childhood education when applied to the specific field of literacy [16,42]. It is surprising that the perception of the importance of working on literacy is not more favorable than the results suggest, given that the specialized literature supports the importance of this area for language development from early childhood [43,44]. The high standard deviation of the responses on the importance of literacy (Table 5) shows, however, the lack of unanimity in the assessments. This lack of consensus is, in turn, a consequence of the gap that exists in this regard among the different early childhood education professionals (Table 6). Specifically, specialists in bilingualism, SH and TP give lower ratings to literacy teaching than teachers and generalist technicians. These results show that group tutors and technicians are probably more familiar with the so-called Critical Age Hypothesis (CAH), which postulates the existence of a critical age range outside which language learning is more difficult and laborious [45]. Although different critical periods are identified according to the different language skills, in any case the CAH indicates that these periods are located before the age of six, both for the acquisition and learning of the mother tongue [46,47] and for second languages [48,49].
In any case, this study shows that generalist early childhood education teachers give greater importance than specialists to literacy work in the early childhood education stage (Table 5). This may be because specialist teachers have much more limited contact with children, which may mean that they do not perceive their needs in literacy as directly. All this suggests the need to increase specific training on literacy learning in the curricula of early childhood education professionals. In the case of bilingualism specialists, it would be useful to delve deeper into the reasons behind their intermediate assessment of the importance of literacy (Table 6). Indeed, studies explain that there is a close relationship between bilingualism and literacy development, but the way in which literacy skills are acquired in a bilingual context is different from the way in which they are acquired in a monolingual context [50,51]. This fact may explain the position of bilingualism specialists, but further research is needed. It has also been shown that educators’ assessment of the importance of literacy depends strongly on the method with which they themselves learned to read (Figure 4), which is one of the original elements of this work. Thus, those who have learned with the analytical method value more, in general, the work of literacy in early childhood education, except in the case of TP specialists (Figure 4).
Regarding the impact of the pandemic and the use of facemasks, the participants believe that the pandemic has really had a negative impact on literacy learning in early childhood education (Table 5). They felt that the use of facemasks had a very negative impact in this regard. In addition, the EFA has identified that the assessments of the impact of facemasks at the visual and phonological levels are strongly correlated, which means that participants perceive that this negative influence is analogous at both levels (Table 2). All this is in line with the medical studies that have been done on the subject [36], which indicate that facemasks have impaired children’s communication in two main ways: (i) they prevent seeing the gestures of their interlocutors; and (ii) they attenuate the voice and the perceived nuances of pronunciation of sounds [52,53]. These effects of facemasks on children’s literacy development are to be expected, given the known impact that facemasks have on communication among medical professionals who use them regularly (mainly due to the decrease in visual and auditory perception that they entail) [12,54]. In this sense, there are studies that propose the use of transparent facemasks to solve, in part, the communication difficulties derived from the use of opaque masks [19,20,55]. On the other hand, other studies show that 2-year-old children can recognize familiar words when they hear them pronounced with an opaque facemask, but recognize them to a lesser extent when they are pronounced with a transparent facemask [16]. This implies that the use of transparent facemasks as a solution to the problems generated using opaque facemasks is debatable and, in any case, requires further studies. Furthermore, as far as the development of literacy in early childhood education is concerned, there are no specific studies that analyze the efficacy of the use of transparent facemasks, as far as it has been explored.
This study shows that, among Spanish educators, the perception of the use of facemasks in literacy learning depends strongly on the position of the educator in the classroom. It is noteworthy, in this sense, that bilingualism specialists are those who attribute a greater negative impact to the use of facemasks in the classroom (Table 6). This result is in line with works that demonstrate the importance of visual and phonological aspects for early second language learning [5,56,57,58]. In addition, it has been shown that the method with which the teachers surveyed learned to read conditions their perception of the influence of the facemask on literacy learning (Figure 5). Thus, those who learned with the synthetic method are those who attribute a greater negative impact in this regard. This fact is in line with other previous works that establish the existence of essential differences between the two learning methods analyzed [17], but it constitutes a novelty in the specialized literature because until now the impact of the method with which the teacher learned to read on the studied perception had not been explored. Therefore, this is an original aspect of the present study.
To comprehensively address the real affectation that the use of facemasks has on the development of literacy, it would be necessary to complete the results of the research presented here with other perspectives that address psychological and medical aspects linked to the development of children’s language. Furthermore, the study has been limited to the descriptive level. To analyze the reasons for the differences between the opinions of specialist and generalist teachers and between those who learned to read with each of the methods described (analytical or synthetic), a specific study would be necessary. On the other hand, the size of the sample used, and the distribution of the participants (mostly women) preclude a sufficiently significant quantitative study of the differences by age, experience or gender of the variables studied. Therefore, in order to study the existence of gaps in the perceptions of educators and to ensure greater representativeness of the sample, it is proposed to increase the sample size by ensuring a reasonably homogeneous distribution with respect to the sociological aspects mentioned. Finally, if the sample of professionals studied could be expanded to include teachers from different locations, it would be possible to analyze whether there is any type of geographic variable that influences the evaluations expressed.
On the other hand, it would be interesting to conduct a correlational study of teachers’ perceptions of the impact of the use of facemasks on children’s literacy learning and emotional life, in order to check whether teachers establish some kind of dependence between both areas, as the literature points out [7,18,25,30,31].
Based on the results of the study, it is suggested that teachers, specialists and generalists, work in a more coordinated manner in their respective didactic actions in the classroom. This would probably make it possible to homogenize the perceptions that the different professionals have of literacy and, therefore, the criteria for didactic action with respect to it. In addition, it would be advisable to reinforce the training in the field of literacy of pre-service teachers, both generalists and specialists.

5. Conclusions

Throughout the study, it was found that early childhood education professionals attribute a high importance to the learning of literacy in children at this stage and that the use of digital resources would increase the effectiveness of educational action in this regard. There is, among the respondents, the perception that the COVID-19 pandemic has slowed down the acquisition of literacy acquisition objectives. In this sense, the use of opaque facemasks is seen as a negative element for the development of the corresponding skills, both visually and phonologically. In the above perceptions, specialist teachers (TP, SH, and bilingualism specialists) value the importance of literacy learning less than generalists. However, those who attribute the greatest negative impact to the use of facemasks are the bilingualism specialists. Regarding the method (synthetic or analytical) that the participants used to learn to read, those who used the analytical method (except TP specialists) give more importance to literacy learning, but those who learned with the synthetic method identify a greater impact of the use of facemasks.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.V.; methodology, D.V. and J.-J.M.; formal analysis, D.V. and Á.A.-S.; investigation, J.-J.M.; data curation, Á.A.-S.; writing—original draft preparation, D.V., Á.A.-S., J.-J.M. and M.N.-S.; writing—review and editing, D.V., Á.A.-S., J.-J.M. and M.N.-S.; supervision, D.V., Á.A.-S., J.-J.M. and M.N.-S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Project “Influence of COVID-19 on teaching: digitization of laboratory practices at UCAV” (1 February 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

All participants were informed about the anonymous nature of their participation, why the research is being conducted, how their data will be used and that under no circumstances would their data be used to identify them. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Project “Influence of COVID-19 on teaching: digitization of laboratory practices at UCAV” (1 February 2022).

Data Availability Statement

The data are not publicly available because they are part of a larger project involving more researchers. If you have any questions, please ask the contact author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Lombardo, M.; Guseva, E.; Perrone, M.A.; Müller, A.; Rizzo, G.; Storz, M.A. Changes in eating habits and physical activity after COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns in Italy. Nutrients 2021, 13, 4522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Pérez, V.; Aybar, C.; Pavía, J.M. COVID-19 and changes in social habits. Restaurant terraces, a booming space in cities. The case of Madrid. Mathematics 2021, 9, 2133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bauza, V.; Sclar, G.D.; Bisoyi, A.; Majorin, F.; Ghugey, A.; Clasen, T. Water, sanitation, and hygiene practices and challenges during the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-sectional study in rural Odisha, India. Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 2021, 104, 2264–2274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Liu, C.; Diab, R.; Naveed, H.; Leung, V. Universal public mask wear during COVID-19 pandemic: Rationale, design and acceptability. Respirology 2020, 25, 895–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Singh, L.; Tan, A.; Quinn, P.C. Infants recognize word spoken through opaque masks but not through clear masks. Dev. Sci. 2021, 24, e13117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Grenville, E.; Dwyer, D.M. Face mask have emotion-dependent dissociable effects on accuracy and confidence in identifying facial expressions of emotion. Cogn. Res. 2022, 7, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bloom, L. Language Development and Emotional Expression. Pediatrics 1998, 102, 1272–1277. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9794968/ (accessed on 26 March 2022). [CrossRef]
  8. Harbi, A.I.; Saeed, S. Languaje development and acquisition in early childhood. EduLearn 2020, 14, 69–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chen, X.; Ran, L.; Liu, Q.; Hu, Q.; Du, X.; Tan, X. Hand hygiene, mask-wearing behaviors and its associated factors during the COVID-19 epidemic: A cross-sectional study among primary school students in Wuhan, China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Sukaj, S.; Trematerra, A.; Ciaburro, G.; Iannace, G. Use of masks inside the classrooms. In Proceedings of the INTER-NOISE and NOISE-CON Congress and Conference Proceedings, Washington, DC, USA, 1–4 August 2021; Volume 263, pp. 4020–4027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Lipps, E.; Caldwell-Kurtzman, J.; Motlagh-Zadeh, L.; Blankenship, C.M.; Moore, D.R.; Hunter, L.L. Impact of face masks on audiovisual word recognition in young children with hearing loss during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Early Hear. Detect. Interv. 2021, 6, 70–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Goldin, A.; Weinstein, B.E.; Shiman, N. How do medical masks degrade speech perception? Hear. Rev. 2020, 27, 8–9. Available online: https://hearingreview.com/hearing-loss/health-wellness/how-do-medical-masks-degrade-speech-reception (accessed on 26 March 2022).
  13. Swaminathan, D.; Meera, S.S. Masks Mask Communication–communicating with children in health care settings. Indian J. Pediatr. 2021, 88, 283–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ribeiro, V.V.; Dassie-Leite, A.P.; Pereira, E.C.; Santos, A.D.N.; Martins, P.; De-Alencar-Irineu, R. Effect of wearing a face mask on vocal self-perception during a pandemic. J. Voice 2020, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Zhang, H.; Koda, K. Early oral language in Chinese heritage language reading development. Foreign Lang. Ann. 2021, 54, 1107–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Nelson, R.L.; Damico, J.S. Qualitative research in literacy acquisition: A framework for investigating reading in children with language impairment. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2006, 20, 631–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Johnston, R.S.; McGeown, S.; Watson, J.E. Long-term effects of synthetic versus analytic phonics teaching on the reading and spelling ability of 10 year old boys and girls. Read. Writ. 2012, 25, 1365–1384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Marini, M.; Ansani, A.; Paglieri, F.; Caruana, F.; Viola, M. The impact of facemasks on emotion recognition, trust attribution and re-identification. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 5577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Atcherson, S.R.; McDowell, B.R.; Howard, M.P. Acoustic effects of non-transparent and transparent face coverings. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2021, 149, 2249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Thibodeau, L.M.; Thibodeau-Nielsen, R.B.; Tran, C.M.Q.; Jacob, R.T.S. Communicating during COVID-19: The effect of transparent masks for speech recognition in noise. Ear. Hear. 2021, 42, 772–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Wolfe, J.; Smith, J.; Neumann, S.; Miller, S.; Schafer, E.C.; Birath, A.L.; Childress, T.; McNally, C.; McNiece, C.; Madell, J.; et al. Optimizing communication in schools and other settings during COVID-19. Hear. J. 2020, 73, 40–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Rudner, M.; Mishra, S.; Stenfelt, S.; Lunner, T.; Rönnberg, J. Seeing the talker’s face improves free recall of speech for young adults with normal hearing but not older adults with hearing loss. Speech. Lang. Hear. Res. 2016, 59, 509–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Mickells, G.E.; Figueroa, J.; West, K.W.; Wood, A.; McElhanon, B.O. Adherence to masking requirement during the COVID-19 pandemic by early elementary school children. Sch. Health 2021, 91, 555–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Georgiou, G.P. Acoustic markers of vowels produced with different types of face masks. Appl. Acoust. 2022, 191, 108691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Spitzer, M. Masked education? The benefits and burdens of wearing face masks in schools during the current Corona pandemic. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2020, 20, 100138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Sortwell, A.; Ramirez-Campillo, R.; Knijnik, J.; Forte, P.; Marinho, D.; Ferraz, R.; Trimple, K. Commentary: Face masks in physical education classes during the COVID-19 delta variant wave: A call for awareness. Ger. J. Exerc. Sport Res. 2021, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Varea, V.; González-Calvo, G.; García-Monge, A. Exploring the changes of physical education in the age of COVID-19. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedag. 2022, 27, 32–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Eberhart, M.; Orthaber, S.; Kerbl, R. The impact of face masks on children—A mini review. Acta Paediatr. 2021, 110, 1778–1783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Carbon, C.-C.; Serrano, M. The impact of face masks on the emotional reading abilities of children—A lesson from a joint school–university project. i-Perception 2021, 12, 20416695211038265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gori, M.; Schiatti, L.; Bianca-Amadeo, M. Masking emotions: Face masks impair how we read emotions. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 669432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Zuo, B.; Wang, Q.; Qiao, Y.; Ding, Y.; Wen, F. Impact of divergent thinking training on teenagers’ emotion and self-efficacy during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 600533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Stites, M.L.; Sonneschein, S.; Galczyk, S.H. Preschool parents’ views of distance learning during COVID-19. Early Educ. Dev. 2021, 32, 923–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Duran, M. The effects of COVID-19 pandemic on preschool education. Int. J. Educ. Method. 2021, 7, 249–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Idoiaga-Mondragon, N.; Berasategi-Sancho, N.; Dosil-Santamaria, M.; Ozamiz-Etxebarria, N. Reopening of schools in the COVID-19 pandemic: The quality of life of teachers while coping with this new challenge in the north of Spain. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Yıldırım, B. Preschool education in Turkey during the COVID-19 pandemic: A phenomenological study. Early Child. Educ. J. 2021, 49, 947–963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Assathiany, R.; Salinier, C.; Béchet, S.; Dolard, C.; Kochert, F.; Bocquet, A.; Levy, C. Face masks in young children during the COVID-19 pandemic: Parents’ and pediatricians’ point of view. Front. Pediatr. 2021, 9, 676718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ludvigsson, J.F.; Engerstrom, L.; Nordenhall, C.; Larsson, E. Open schools, COVID-19, and child and teacher morbidity in Sweden. N. Engl. J. Med. 2021, 384, 669–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Galea, S.; Merchant, R.M.; Lurie, N. The mental health consequences of COVID-19 and physical distancing. The need for prevention and early intervention. JAMA Intern. Med. 2020, 180, 817–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Maftei, A.; Merlici, I.-A.; Roca, I.-C. Implications of the COVID-19 pandemic on children and adolescents: Cognitive and emotional representations. Children 2022, 9, 359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Litvinova, M.; Liu, Q.H.; Kulikov, E.S.; Ajelli, M. Reactive school closure weakens the network of social interactions and reduces the spread of influenza. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 13174–13181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Green, J.; Staff, L.; Bromley, P.; Jones, L.; Petty, J. The implications of face masks for babies and families during the CoVid-19 pandemic: A discussion paper. J. Neonatal Nurs. 2021, 27, 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Charney, S.A.; Camarata, S.M.; Chern, A. Potential impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on communication and language skills in children. Otolaryngol.-Head Neck Surg. 2021, 165, 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Fajriyah, L.; Fauziyah, P. The role of ICT to develop early literacy of children in era digital. In Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Early Childhood Education. Semarang Early Childhood Research and Education Talks (SECRET 2018), Kota Semarang, Indonesia, 8–9 August 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Strickland, D.S.; Morrow, L.M.; Neuman, S.B.; Roskos, K.; Schickedanz, J.A.; Vukelich, C. The role of literacy in early childhood education. Read. Teach. 2004, 58, 86–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ashton, J.; Fellowes, J.; Oakley, G. Language, literacy and early childhood education. J. Early Child. Lit. 2013, 13, 131–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Lenneberg, E. Biological Foundations of Language; John Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
  47. Abello-Contesse, C. Age and the critical period hypothesis. ELT J. 2009, 63, 170–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Nathan, L.; Stackhouse, J.; Goulandris, N.; Snowling, M.J. The development of early literacy skills among children with speech difficulties. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2004, 47, 377–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Hakuta, K.; Bialystok, E.; Wiley, E. Critical evidence: A test of the critical-period hypothesis for second-language acquisition. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Rakhimova, I. The essence of critical age hypothesis in second language acquisition. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Teaching Foreign Language, Tashkent, Uzbekistan, 30 April 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Bialystok, E.; Herman, J. Does bilingualism matter for early literacy? Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1999, 2, 35–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Bialystok, E. Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Lang. Learn. 2002, 52, 159–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Nobrega, M.; Opice, R.; Lauletta, M.M.; Nobrega, C.A. How face masks can affect school performance. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2020, 138, 110328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Rudge, A.M.; Sonneveldt, V.; Brookes, B.M. The Effects of Face Coverings and Remote Microphone Technology on Speech Perception in the Classroom; Moog Center for Deaf Education: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  55. Wittum, K.J.; Feth, L.; Hoglund, E. The effects of surgical masks on speech perception in noise. Proc. Mtgs. Acoust. 2013, 19, 060125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Hardison, D.M.; Pennington, M.C. Multimodal second-language communication: Research findings and pedagogical implications. RELC J. 2021, 52, 62–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Smilijanic, R.; Keerstock, S.; Meemann, K.; Ransom, S.M. Face masks and speaking style affect audio-visual word recognition and memory of native and non-native speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2021, 149, 4013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Schroeder, S.R.; Chen, P. Bilingualism and CoVid-19: Using a second language during a health crisis. J. Commun. Healthc. 2021, 14, 20–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Scheme of both synthetic and analytical methods for literacy learning.
Figure 1. Scheme of both synthetic and analytical methods for literacy learning.
Ejihpe 12 00048 g001
Figure 2. Research objective and variables.
Figure 2. Research objective and variables.
Ejihpe 12 00048 g002
Figure 3. Distributions of participants by: (a) position held, and (b) method by which they learned to read.
Figure 3. Distributions of participants by: (a) position held, and (b) method by which they learned to read.
Ejihpe 12 00048 g003
Figure 4. Mean values (out of 5) of the importance of literacy learning in early childhood education, differentiating by the position occupied in the classroom and the method by which the participants learned to read.
Figure 4. Mean values (out of 5) of the importance of literacy learning in early childhood education, differentiating by the position occupied in the classroom and the method by which the participants learned to read.
Ejihpe 12 00048 g004
Figure 5. Mean values (out of 5) of the influence of the facemasks on literacy, differentiating by the position occupied in the classroom and the method by which the participants learned to read.
Figure 5. Mean values (out of 5) of the influence of the facemasks on literacy, differentiating by the position occupied in the classroom and the method by which the participants learned to read.
Ejihpe 12 00048 g005
Table 1. Questions of the survey.
Table 1. Questions of the survey.
VariableQuestion NumberQuestion
Importance of literacy in early childhood education1Value the importance of the implementation of the literacy area in the early childhood education classroom
2Value the need for early childhood education students to begin to recognize phonemes and graphemes
3Value the importance of early childhood education students’ access to the next educational stage knowing how to read and write
4Value the degree of knowledge of literacy with which the children in your center enter primary education
Importance of the use of digital resources in the teaching of literacy5Value the importance of the use of digital resources for literacy learning
6Value the level of motivation generated in students using digital resources in the learning of literacy
Effectiveness of the analytical method7Value the efficacy of the analytical method as a method of teaching literacy
Effectiveness of the synthetic method8Value the effectiveness of the synthetic method as a method for teaching literacy
Level of impact of the pandemic on literacy9Value the linguistic delay that the students present when they join the school after the pandemic
10Compared to the pre-pandemic state, how do you value the achievement of objectives in learning to read and write?
Negative influence of the use of facemasks in the learning of literacy11Value the degree of negative influence that the use of the facemask has on the development of visual literacy
12Value the degree of negative influence that the use of the facemask has on the development of literacy at the phonological level
13Value the frequency with which you use support material (such as a transparent mask or microphone) to solve communication difficulties caused by the use of facemasks
Perception of the need for speech-language intervention in the classroom14Perception of the need for speech-language intervention in the classroom (pre-pandemic)
15Perception of the need for speech-language intervention in the classroom (post-pandemic)
Table 2. Factorial weights of the EFA.
Table 2. Factorial weights of the EFA.
QuestionFactor 1. Importance of LiteracyFactor 2. ICT in LiteracyFactor 3. Analytical MethodFactor 4. Synthetic MethodFactor 5. Impact of Pandemic on LiteracyFactor 6. Influence of Facemasks
Q. 10.930
Q. 20.644
Q. 30.837
Q. 40.942
Q. 5 0.991
Q. 6 0.703
Q. 7 0.743
Q. 8 0.769
Q. 9 0.754
Q. 10 0.975
Q. 11 0.741
Q. 12 0.801
Q. 13 0.962
Table 3. Cumulative proportion of explained variance of the principal component analysis.
Table 3. Cumulative proportion of explained variance of the principal component analysis.
Factor 1Factor 2Factor 3Factor 4Factor 5Factor 6
Proportion Variance0.1080.1320.1030.0930.1160.091
Cumulative Variance0.1080.2400.3430.4360.5520.643
Table 4. Pearson correlation coefficients.
Table 4. Pearson correlation coefficients.
Factor 1Factor 2Factor 3Factor 4Factor 5Factor 6Global
Factor 110.14130.21590.1995−0.13800.01970.7190
Factor 2 10.15580.05560.1483−0.09240.8986
Factor 3 10.17450.11720.19280.8975
Factor 4 10.00010.14010.8445
Factor 5 10.13790.7293
Factor 6 10.7196
Global 1
Table 5. Global mean values and standard deviations (over 5).
Table 5. Global mean values and standard deviations (over 5).
Mean ValuesStandard Deviations
Importance of literacy3.311.42
Importance of ICT in literacy4.120.84
Analytical method3.231.06
Synthetic method3.321.08
Impact of the pandemic on literacy3.760.93
Negative influence of facemasks4.580.78
Table 6. Mean values and standard deviations (over 5) when the participants are differentiated by their position.
Table 6. Mean values and standard deviations (over 5) when the participants are differentiated by their position.
FactorBilingualism SpecialistSH SpecialistTP SpecialistGroup TutorSupport TechnicianANOVAp-Value
Importance of literacy3.303.172.653.583.424.3020.0342 *
Importance of ICT in literacy4.204.334.004.504.080.88080.5012
Analytical method3.002.503.383.673.250.84720.5208
Synthetic method3.604.172.923.673.272.11330.1389
Impact of the pandemic on literacy3.604.003.853.833.730.36300.8308
Negative influence of facemasks4.904.424.544.754.574.25140.0363 *
* p < 0.05.
Table 7. Mean values and standard deviations (over 5) of the perception of the need for speech-language intervention in the classroom when the participants are differentiated by their position in the classroom.
Table 7. Mean values and standard deviations (over 5) of the perception of the need for speech-language intervention in the classroom when the participants are differentiated by their position in the classroom.
Need for Speech-Language InterventionBilingualism SpecialistSH SpecialistTP SpecialistGroup TutorSupport TechnicianANOVAp-Value
Pre-pandemic3.403.503.853.163.500.32040.8641
Post-pandemic4.404.004.003.684.00
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Vergara, D.; Antón-Sancho, Á.; Maldonado, J.-J.; Nieto-Sobrino, M. Impact of Using Facemasks on Literacy Learning: The Perception of Early Childhood Education Teachers. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2022, 12, 639-654. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060048

AMA Style

Vergara D, Antón-Sancho Á, Maldonado J-J, Nieto-Sobrino M. Impact of Using Facemasks on Literacy Learning: The Perception of Early Childhood Education Teachers. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2022; 12(6):639-654. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060048

Chicago/Turabian Style

Vergara, Diego, Álvaro Antón-Sancho, Juan-José Maldonado, and María Nieto-Sobrino. 2022. "Impact of Using Facemasks on Literacy Learning: The Perception of Early Childhood Education Teachers" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 12, no. 6: 639-654. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060048

APA Style

Vergara, D., Antón-Sancho, Á., Maldonado, J. -J., & Nieto-Sobrino, M. (2022). Impact of Using Facemasks on Literacy Learning: The Perception of Early Childhood Education Teachers. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 12(6), 639-654. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060048

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop