Evaluating the Consistency of Subjective Activity Assessments and Their Relation to Cognition in Older Adults
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.1.1. ACC Study
2.1.2. TRAILLs Study
2.2. Procedure
2.2.1. ACC Study
2.2.2. TRAILLs Study
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Activity Questionnaires
ACC Study
TRAILLs Study
2.3.2. Cognitive Tests
ACC Study
2.4. Data Preparation and Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Differences between Estimated Weekly Activity and Daily Reported Activity
3.1.1. ACC
3.1.2. TRAILLs
3.2. Predictors of Cognitive Performance
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Hultsch, D.F.; Hertzog, C.; Small, B.J.; Dixon, R.A. Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol. Aging 1999, 14, 245–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, J.Y.J.; Zhou, D.H.D.; Li, J.; Zhang, M.; Deng, J.; Tang, M.; Gao, C.; Lian, Y.; Chen, M. Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: The Chongqing aging study. Neurology 2006, 66, 911–913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hertzog, C.; Kramer, A.; Wilson, R.; Lindenberger, U. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2009, 9, 1–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bielak, A.A.M. How can we not ‘lose it’ if we still don’t understand how to ‘use it’? Unanswered questions about the influence of activity participation on cognitive performance in older age—A mini-review. Gerontology 2010, 56, 507–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salthouse, T.A. Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 1, 68–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bielak, A.A.M. Different perspectives on measuring lifestyle engagement: A comparison of activity measures and their relation with cognitive performance in older adults. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2017, 24, 435–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Matton, L.; Wijndaele, K.; Duvigneaud, N.; Duquet, W.; Philippaerts, R.; Thomis, M.; Lefevre, J. Reliability and validity of the Flemish Physical Activity Computerized Questionnaire in adults. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2007, 78, 293–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, D.I.; Taler, V.; Davidson, P.S.; Messier, C. Measuring the impact of exercise on cognitive aging: Methodological issues. Neurobiol. Aging 2012, 33, 622.e29–622.e43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heesch, K.C.; van Uffelen, J.G.Z.; Hill, R.L.; Brown, W.J. What do IPAQ questions mean to older adults? Lessons from cognitive interviews. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2010, 7, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Salthouse, T.A.; Berish, D.E.; Miles, J.D. The role of cognitive stimulation on the relations between age and cognitive functioning. Psychol. Aging 2002, 17, 548–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Craik, F.I.M. Age related changes in human memory. In Cognitive Aging; Park, D., Schwarz, N., Eds.; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2000; pp. 75–92. [Google Scholar]
- Mather, M.; Carstensen, L. Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 496–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Robinson, M.D.; Clore, G.L. Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychol. Bull. 2002, 128, 934–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Parisi, J.M. Engagement in adulthood: Perceptions and participation in daily activities. Act. Adapt. Aging 2010, 34, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cacioppo, J.T.; Petty, R.E.; Feinsteinm, J.A.; Jarvis, W.B.G. Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 119, 197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verplanken, B. Involvement and need for cognition as moderators of beliefs—attitude—intention consistency. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 28, 115–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forstmeier, S.; Maercker, A. Motivational reserve: Lifetime motivational abilities contribute to cognitive and emotional health in old age. Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 886–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Juster, F.; Stafford, F. Work and Leisure: On the Reporting of Poll Results: Comment. Public Opin. Q. 1991, 55, 357–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ariel, R.; Price, J.; Hertzog, C. Age-related associative memory deficits in value-based remembering: The contribution of agenda-based regulation and strategy use. Psychol. Aging 2015, 30, 795–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dudukovic, N.M.; DuBrow, S.; Wagner, A.D. Attention during memory retrieval enhances future remembering. Mem. Cogn. 2009, 37, 953–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Delaney, P.F.; Verkoeijen, P.P.J.L.; Spirgel, A. Chapter 3—Spacing and Testing Effects: A Deeply Critical, Lengthy, and at Times Discursive Review of the Literature. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Ross, B.H., Ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010; Volume 53, pp. 63–147. [Google Scholar]
- Almeida, D.M.; Wethington, E.; Kessler, R.C. The daily inventory of stressful events. Assessment 2002, 9, 41–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parisi, J.M.; Stine-Morrow, E.A.L.; Noh, S.R.; Morrow, D.G. Predispositional engagement, activity engagement, and cognition among older adults. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2009, 16, 485–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hess, T.M.; Growney, C.M.; O’Brien, E.L.; Neupert, S.D.; Sherwood, A. The role of cognitive costs, attitudes about aging, and intrinsic motivation in predicting engagement in everyday activities. Psychol. Aging 2018, 33, 953–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Curtis, R.G.; Windsor, T.D.; Mogle, J.A.; Bielak, A.A.M. There’s more than meets the eye: Complex associations of daily pain, physical symptoms, and self-efficacy with activity in middle and older adulthood. Gerontology 2017, 63, 157–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th ed.; Pearson: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Reitan, R.M. Trail Making Test; Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory: Tuscon, AZ, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, A. A Symbol Digit Modalities Test: Manual; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Ekstrom, R.B.E.A. Manual for Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests; Education Testing Service: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, M. Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: A Handbook; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Hess, T.M.; Smith, B.T.; Sharifian, N. Aging and effort expenditure: The impact of subjective perceptions of task demands. Psychol. Aging 2016, 31, 653–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Szanton, S.L.; Walker, R.K.; Roberts, L.; Thorpe, R.J.; Wolff, J.; Agree, E.; Roth, D.L.; Gitlin, L.N.; Seplaki, C. Older adults’ favorite activities are resoundingly active: Findings from the NHATS study. Geriatr. Nurs. 2015, 36, 131–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ferreira, N.; Owen, A.; Mohan, A.; Corbett, A.; Ballard, C. Associations between cognitively stimulating leisure activities, cognitive function and age-related cognitive decline. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2015, 30, 422–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vance, D.E.; Wadley, V.G.; Ball, K.K.; Roenker, D.L.; Rizzo, M. The effects of physical activity and sedentary behavior on cognitive health in older adults. J. Aging Phys. Act. 2005, 13, 294–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bugg, J.M.; DeLosh, E.L.; Clegg, B.A. Physical activity moderates time-of-day differences in older adults’ working memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 2006, 32, 431–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newson, R.S.; Kemps, E.B. The Influence of Physical and Cognitive Activities on Simple and Complex Cognitive Tasks in Older Adults. Exp. Aging Res. 2006, 32, 341–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghisletta, P.; Bickel, J.-F.; Lövdén, M. Does activity engagement protect against cognitive decline in old age? Methodological and analytical considerations. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2006, 61, 253–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lövden, M.; Ghisletta, P.; Lindenberger, U. Social participation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old age. Psychol. Aging 2005, 20, 423–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jopp, D.; Hertzog, C. Activities, self-referent memory beliefs, and cognitive performance: Evidence for direct and mediated relations. Psychol. Aging 2007, 22, 811–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D.; Slovic, P.; Tversky, A. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Timmers, C.; Maeghs, A.; Vestjens, M.; Bonnemayer, C.; Hamers, H.; Blokland, A. Ambulant Cognitive Assessment Using a Smartphone. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 2014, 21, 136–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spooner, D.M.; Pachana, N.A. Ecological validity in neuropsychological assessment: A case for greater consideration in research with neurologically intact populations. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2006, 21, 327–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sliwinski, M.J.; Mogle, J.A.; Hyun, J.; Munoz, E.; Smyth, J.M.; Lipton, R.B. Reliability and Validity of Ambulatory Cognitive Assessments. Assessment 2016, 25, 14–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Activity Items | Weekly Estimate M (SD) | Daily Report (Weekly Sum) M (SD) | Mean Difference M (SD) | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Actively Watching TV/movies | 7.76 (4.29) | 11.52 (3.56) | −3.77 (4.32) | <0.001 |
Reading | 9.69 (3.21) | 12.88 (0.68) | −3.20 (3.17) | <0.001 |
Writing | 4.97 (3.93) | 9.96 (4.06) | −4.98 (4.74) | <0.001 |
Interacting with close friends | 7.65 (4.08) | 12.37 (2.29) | −4.72 (4.24) | <0.001 |
Interaction with not close friends | 5.37 (3.99) | 12.14 (2.20) | −6.77 (4.38) | <0.001 |
Meeting new people | 1.29 (1.86) | 5.77 (4.44) | −4.47 (4.46) | <0.001 |
Actively listening to information | 5.89 (3.87) | 12.08 (2.53) | −6.18 (4.15) | <0.001 |
Attending community events | 2.66 (2.50) | 6.17 (4.91) | −3.52 (4.89) | <0.001 |
Going out shopping | 3.00 (2.43) | 9.54 (3.76) | −6.54 (4.11) | <0.001 |
Light-intensity activity/exercise | 5.79 (3.94) | 11.49 (3.14) | −5.70 (4.46) | <0.001 |
Medium-intensity activity/exercise | 3.96 (3.11) | 8.68 (4.75) | −4.72 (4.77) | <0.001 |
Vigorous-intensity activity/exercise | 2.14 (2.93) | 3.80 (4.92) | −1.66 (5.07) | <0.001 |
Total activity | 60.20 (19.91) | 116.41 (16.80) | −56.21 (22.97) | <0.001 |
Activity Items | Weekly Estimate M (SD) | Daily Report (Weekly Sum) M (SD) | Mean Difference M (SD) | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
TV, Movies, Videos | 7.42 (3.71) | 15.68 (9.18) | −8.26 (7.82) | <0.001 |
Actively Reading | 5.65 (3.51) | 9.17 (6.98) | −3.52 (6.07) | <0.001 |
Actively Writing | 4.54 (3.74) | 6.73 (7.42) | −2.19 (6.63) | <0.001 |
Going out shopping | 2.19 (1.81) | 4.83 (5.36) | −2.64 (5.37) | <0.001 |
Light physical activity | 3.88 (3.66) | 2.97 (4.34) | 0.92 (5.19) | 0.023 |
Medium-intensity activity | 3.02 (2.89) | 5.85 (6.00) | −2.84 (5.83) | <0.001 |
Vigorous-intensity activity | 1.92 (2.57) | 3.79 (3.98) | −1.86 (4.27) | <0.001 |
Total activity | 28.62 (11.57) | 49.02 (22.48) | −20.40 (20.20) | <0.001 |
Dependent Variables | β | p | |
---|---|---|---|
Fluid factor a | Age | −1.19 | <0.001 |
Education | 1.79 | 0.003 | |
Weekly Estimate | −0.00 | 0.392 | |
Daily Report | 0.08 | 0.971 | |
Verbal factor b | Age | −0.31 | 0.020 |
Education | 1.47 | <0.001 | |
Weekly Estimate | 0.07 | 0.180 | |
Daily Report | 0.03 | 0.649 | |
Memory factor c | Age | −1.09 | <0.001 |
Education | 0.30 | 0.585 | |
Weekly Estimate | −0.03 | 0.673 | |
Daily Report | 0.09 | 0.303 |
Dependent Variables | β | p | |
---|---|---|---|
Fluid factor a | Age | −1.18 | 0.000 |
Education | 1.76 | 0.004 | |
Weekly Estimate | |||
Cognitive Activity | 0.20 | 0.770 | |
Social Activity | 0.97 | 0.330 | |
Physical Activity | −1.27 | 0.089 | |
Daily Report | |||
Cognitive Activity | 0.03 | 0.973 | |
Social Activity | −0.33 | 0.701 | |
Physical Activity | 1.16 | 0.062 | |
Verbal factor b | Age | −0.29 | 0.028 |
Education | 1.33 | 0.000 | |
Weekly Estimate | |||
Cognitive Activity | 0.35 | 0.364 | |
Social Activity | −0.22 | 0.694 | |
Physical Activity | 0.57 | 0.179 | |
Daily Report | |||
Cognitive Activity | 1.63 | 0.005 | |
Social Activity | −0.75 | 0.117 | |
Physical Activity | −0.02 | 0.946 | |
Memory Factor c | Age | −1.07 | 0.000 |
Education Weekly Estimate | 0.19 | 0.727 | |
Cognitive Activity | 1.04 | 0.095 | |
Social Activity | −1.81 | 0.050 | |
Physical Activity | 0.052 | 0.940 | |
Daily Report | |||
Cognitive Activity | 1.00 | 0.281 | |
Social Activity | 0.69 | 0.379 | |
Physical Activity | −0.14 | 0.799 |
Dependent Variables | β | p | |
---|---|---|---|
Fluid factor a | Age | −1.22 | <0.001 |
Education | 1.85 | 0.002 | |
Total Activity Difference Score | −0.04 | 0.601 | |
Verbal factor b | Age | −0.34 | 0.009 |
Education | 1.54 | <0.001 | |
Total Activity Difference Score | 0.03 | 0.494 | |
Memory factor c | Age | −1.11 | <0.001 |
Education | 0.35 | 0.526 | |
Total Activity Difference Score | −0.06 | 0.369 |
Dependent Variables | Predictors | β | p |
---|---|---|---|
Fluid factor a | Age | −1.19 | 0.000 |
Education | 1.77 | 0.000 | |
Cognitive Activity Difference | 0.23 | 0.707 | |
Social Activity Difference | 0.56 | 0.432 | |
Physical Activity Difference | −1.16 | 0.033 | |
Verbal factor b | Age | −0.35 | 0.008 |
Education | 1.55 | 0.000 | |
Cognitive Activity Difference | −0.13 | 0.719 | |
Social Activity Difference | 0.30 | 0.478 | |
Physical Activity Difference | 0.15 | 0.640 | |
Memory Factor c | Age | −1.09 | 0.000 |
Education | 0.35 | 0.520 | |
Cognitive Activity Difference | 0.50 | 0.384 | |
Social Activity Difference | −1.2 | 0.059 | |
Physical Activity Difference | 0.06 | 0.914 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Hatt, C.R.; Brydges, C.R.; Mogle, J.A.; Sliwinski, M.J.; Bielak, A.A.M. Evaluating the Consistency of Subjective Activity Assessments and Their Relation to Cognition in Older Adults. Geriatrics 2021, 6, 74. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics6030074
Hatt CR, Brydges CR, Mogle JA, Sliwinski MJ, Bielak AAM. Evaluating the Consistency of Subjective Activity Assessments and Their Relation to Cognition in Older Adults. Geriatrics. 2021; 6(3):74. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics6030074
Chicago/Turabian StyleHatt, Cassandra R., Christopher R. Brydges, Jacqueline A. Mogle, Martin J. Sliwinski, and Allison A. M. Bielak. 2021. "Evaluating the Consistency of Subjective Activity Assessments and Their Relation to Cognition in Older Adults" Geriatrics 6, no. 3: 74. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics6030074
APA StyleHatt, C. R., Brydges, C. R., Mogle, J. A., Sliwinski, M. J., & Bielak, A. A. M. (2021). Evaluating the Consistency of Subjective Activity Assessments and Their Relation to Cognition in Older Adults. Geriatrics, 6(3), 74. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics6030074