Next Article in Journal
Gender Differences in Head and Neck Posture Among Smartphone Users While Walking: Insights from Field Observations in Taipei
Previous Article in Journal
Impact of Four Weeks of TOGU Training on Neuromuscular Control and Golf Swing Performance
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Gender Differences for the Relative Age Effect on Physical Skills and Emotional Intelligence in Child Volleyball and Soccer Athletes

by
Juan D. Ávila-Martínez
1,
Michael A. Castro-Malaver
1,
Boryi A. Becerra-Patiño
2,3,*,
Juliana Varón-Murcia
2,
Stefania Cárdenas-Contreras
2 and
José Pino-Ortega
4
1
Sports and Physical Activity Sciences, Faculty of Physical Education, National Pedagogical University, Bogotá 111166, Colombia
2
Faculty of Physical Education, National Pedagogical University, Bogotá 111166, Colombia
3
Management and Pedagogy of Physical Activity and Sport (GPAFD), Faculty of Physical Education, National Pedagogical University, Bogotá 111166, Colombia
4
Faculty of Sports Sciences, University of Murcia, 30720 Santiago de la Ribera, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2024, 9(4), 244; https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk9040244
Submission received: 8 April 2024 / Revised: 6 May 2024 / Accepted: 7 May 2024 / Published: 20 November 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Athletic Training and Human Performance)

Abstract

:
Background/Objectives. The goal of this study was to assess the relative age effect and its relationship with the development of physical abilities and emotional intelligence in Colombian child athletes according to gender and the sport practiced. Methods. A cross-sectional correlational design was used. A total of 135 Colombian soccer and volleyball athletes, 62 boys and 73 girls, voluntarily participated with an average age of 13.25 ± 0.59 years. Physical skills such as SJ, CMJ, CMJA, 5, 10, and 15 m speed, CODS agility, and emotional intelligence were evaluated. Results. Statistically significant differences were found in the mood variables between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.047, ηp2 = 0.08) and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.035, ηp2 = 0.08). These differences are also present in the interpersonal variable between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.12), Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.12), and, finally, in the total emotional quotient between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.13), Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.13), and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.13). Conclusions. Based on the findings of this research, it can be concluded that the relative age effect appears to be sensitive to CODS agility, 15 m speed, and SJ power variables concerning gender and sport. Jump height for CMJ concerning gender and CMJA jump concerning sport were considered. Emotional intelligence appears to be a variable sensitive to the relative age effect.

1. Introduction

Over the past 20 years, changes in school, social, and performance education have led to significant growth in sports, transitioning from recreational to high-performance sports [1]. Various factors, including the athletes’ financial situation, the age at which they start their sports journey, and competition or playing experience, are considered in the study of this phenomenon [2]. The age at which individuals start sports or intensive training influences talent selection and detection [1]. Athletes’ selection becomes problematic when deciding which ones participate in national representation programs [3]. Although some processes have been developed under the relative age effect (RAE) approach and others from biological maturation, it is necessary to recognize that there is no “gold standard” to help determine the processes of sports talent as they depend on interindividual characteristics, contextual factors, sport-specific characteristics, and long-term preparation processes according to Sweeney et al. [4]. This is reflected in the findings of the study conducted by Bezuglov et al. [5] when they determined that, although the evaluated footballers were more mature than late players (p < 0.001), in the end, there were no significant differences between them for variables related to strength, speed, or specific sports skills. Likewise, in another study with volleyball players, it was determined that there were no differences in physiological markers according to the RAE [6]. However, recent research has defined that age and maturation are indicators to consider in youth soccer and are influenced by gender and athletes’ competitive levels [7]. On the other hand, some studies have defined that the RAE generates bias in talent selection due to an inequality of opportunities in initial categories [8]. According to [9], the long-term goals of sports programs are to promote talents toward a specialty, developing crucial factors such as physical, technical, tactical, and psychological qualities connected with genetic variables and maturation development (biological age).
Therefore, an athlete born in the early months of the year can compete against one born later, leading to identified variability in biological age, i.e., differences in maturation among individuals of the same cohort [10]. Other studies determined a selection bias in RAE and maturation in football, suggesting that both processes should be analyzed independently [11,12]. Thus, chronological, biological, and cognitive age constitute RAE. However, based on biological and chronological age, three classifications can be distinguished: (i) children with early or premature maturation, developing before the present age; (ii) the development of individuals according to their age, with the peak acceleration occurring in boys at fourteen years and in girls at twelve; and (iii) late developments concerning the subject’s chronological age, as their biological age may be up to a year less than their chronological age [13].
Brown [14] emphasized that individuals in the late maturation stage are characterized by low stature but with a normal growth speed. These differences are crucial in studying the RAE. It can be evaluated by specific quartiles: 1 January to 31 March, 1 April to 30 June, 1 July to 30 September, and 1 October to 31 December [15,16]. In the same category, a chronological difference of 11 months could exist, generating differences in the physical and anthropometric conditions of individuals. RAE has studied playing positions in women’s soccer [17] as well as the influence of competitive-level and playing positions in young players [18]. Similarly, studies have focused on identifying and developing sports talents [19], the study of levels of sports experience in young soccer players [20], and the relationship between relative age, maturation, anthropometry, and physical fitness in young soccer players [21].
The systematic review by Sarmento et al. [19] concludes that studies focused on evaluating psychological and environmental factors that may affect talent development in soccer are needed. On the other hand, RAE has been studied for its effect on anthropometric and physiological characteristics in young volleyball players [22], vertical jump performance, and its effect on playing position and competitive levels [23], and the differences in position concerning physical and physiological characteristics [24]. Thus, soccer and volleyball are team cooperation–opposition sports with certain similarities in their game actions (jumps, direction changes, accelerations, decelerations, etc.), where multiple physical [25] and emotional [26] capabilities are manifested. The most relevant physical capacities in soccer and volleyball are speed, strength, and agility [27]. However, the emotional manifestation of athletes is crucial for improving the initiation and adherence to sports practice [28]. Emotions are a relevant indicator of sports performance [29], as they relate to perceptual, cognitive, motivational, and motor expression processes. This demonstrates the importance of research to further understand the existing differences among players about the RAE based on psychological variables, such as emotional intelligence (EI).
It has been studied that feelings and decisions could regulate the manifestation of sports performance [30], and these are closely related to moods and emotions [31]. Along these same lines, there is a meta-analytic study that determined the majority of profile mood scales (POMS) are predictors of performance in a wide variety of sports that are based on athletic performance [32]. These findings reaffirm the importance of the mood variable in sports and its link with the evaluation of EI. In this regard, the influence of EI on highly competitive sports has been studied [33] and its relationship with age and subjective well-being [34]. Ultimately, the cognitive aspect of an athlete is essential for their development; however, to date, no studies have addressed EI with RAE, as research has focused on comparing the physical, anthropometric, technical, and tactical characteristics of athletes without considering the athlete’s emotional aspect. A recent study on the analysis of RAE in sports determined that there is a greater scientific production of studies that study men (74.26%), while the production of male–female studies (20.46%) and women is low (7.01%) [35]. Another study addressed gender differences in preferences for team sports in adolescents, highlighting that the sports with the greatest preference are soccer and volleyball [36], which highlights the importance of making comparisons between sports based on the concept of gender. To our knowledge, no previous study evaluated the RAE on physical abilities and EI in young soccer and volleyball athletes in relation to gender. The present study aims to address this research gap. For this reason, the objective of the present study was to assess the RAE and its relationship with the development of physical abilities and EI in Colombian child athletes according to gender and the sport practiced.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design

A cross-sectional correlational study design was used. Under the described experimental design and taking a type I error of 5% and a nominal power of 80%, the sample size was determined by means of the partial Eta2 statistic, taking as a reference the detection of small differences between quartiles, controlling for sport and gender in a significant way (Eta2 < 0.1). Below is the detail of the sample size for different values of Eta2. According to the given conditions, at least a sample size of 103 participants was required for the study; in the end, 135 participants were taken.
The research was woven through a series of organized, systematic, secure, and reliable processes, ensuring the integrity of all participants. Various criteria [37], characteristics, and protocols were considered for selection, evaluation, and data collection. Each athlete provided assent, and after the objectives, the scope and procedures of the study were thoroughly explained, and parents gave informed consent for voluntary participation. Each procedure followed principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki [38], and it adhered to resolution 8430 of the Ministry of Health of Colombia [39], declaring the study as low risk according to Colombian regulations based on non-invasive procedures. The study received approval from the National Pedagogical University’s ethics committee (340ETIC-2024).

2.2. Sample

The evaluated sample consisted of 135 Colombian athletes, including 62 boys and 73 girls, with an average age of 13.25 ± 0.59 years, a body mass of 51.10 ± 8.96 kg, and a height of 160.94 ± 8.57 cm. They were assessed for female soccer (n: 33), male soccer (n: 33), female volleyball (n: 40), and male volleyball (n: 29). Inclusion criteria were as follows: (i) absence of any injury, pathology, or surgery preventing participation; (ii) assent and informed consent signed by both the athlete and parents; (iii) age within the range of 12 to 13 years; (iv) completion of all tests; (v) engagement in volleyball and soccer; and (vi) a minimum of two years of experience in the corresponding sport, especially for familiarization and specificity of the actions performed such as jumps, CODS agility, and speed. Table 1 shows the basic characteristics of the sample evaluated.

2.3. Instruments

For weight determination, an Omron scale (Kyoto, Japan) with an accuracy of 0.1 kg was employed. The selected method for strength evaluation through the Squat Jump (SJ), Counter Movement Jump (CMJ), and Countermovement Jump with Arm Swing (CMJA) jumps was the mobile application My Jump 2, recording the video by camera, demonstrating a high intraclass correlation coefficient (0.997, p < 0.001), Bland–Altman bias = 1.1 ± 0.5 cm, p < 0.001, and validity for height r = 0.995, p < 0.001 [40,41]. CODS agility was assessed through the iPhone application COD-Timer, showing a total time measurement (r = 0.964; 95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.95–1.00; standard error of the estimate = 0.03 s; p < 0.001) [42]. To measure running mechanics, the iPhone application Runmatic was employed, showing a correlation coefficient (r = 0.94–0.99, p < 0.001) [43]. For EI evaluation, The Bar-On EQ-i: YV was used, specifically the 60-item version, considering inconsistency indices in each response [44]. The test demonstrated reliability through Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the total scale of 0.76, and reliability indices for each factor ranged from 0.63 to 0.80 [45].

2.4. Procedure

Various protocols were used for the evaluation of physical capacities and EI. The evaluation was divided into four moments: (i) female volleyball; (ii) male volleyball; (iii) male soccer; and (iv) female soccer.
Each moment also spanned four days for the assessment of all variables, each separated by 48 h after the previous training session or competition match. On the first day, basic anthropometric measurements, including body mass and height, were taken. For the evaluation of physical capacities, the My Jump Lab application recorded the necessary information based on leg length (cm), trochanter length to the ground (cm), and squat length with a 90° angle. The second day involved the assessment of SJ, CMJ, and CMJA jumps [46]. The third day included speed tests at 5, 10, and 15 m, along with CODS agility. Finally, on the fourth day, an EI evaluation was conducted [44]. Each physical evaluation was performed by four researchers with a minimum of three months of experience using the My Jump Lab mobile application. In the application of The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory: Youth Version (Bar-On EQ-i: YV), each of the four researchers was present to address any arising concerns. All data were collected under standard environmental conditions regarding time (4:00 to 6:00 pm) and temperature (between 15° and 22°) from August to October 2023. Each athlete was summoned 15 min before the start of training to undergo an 8 min warm-up protocol, which was standardized to include joint movements and lateral and frontal displacements. The facilities used were the soccer and volleyball fields regularly utilized by clubs for their training.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Data analysis was conducted by study groups, initially involving a descriptive analysis divided into univariate, bivariate, and multivariate analyses. For quantitative variables, the central tendency was determined by the mean, dispersion by the standard deviation, and position measures by analyzing extremes and quartiles. In bivariate descriptive analysis, relationships between sets of variables were sought. It was necessary to classify variables into qualitative and quantitative, where qualitative variables included the year, quartile, sport, and gender, and a nested variable named the year quartile. In this sense, bivariate descriptive analysis was conducted in three sections.
For two qualitative variables, contingency tables were used. In the case of two quantitative variables, Pearson’s correlation was employed, and for one qualitative and one quantitative variable, univariate analysis was replicated, discriminating by category. For inferential analysis, considering the structure of groups in the variables, multivariate analysis of variance was utilized [47], using Pillai’s trace statistic [48], with prior suitability analysis (univariate and multivariate outlier analysis, univariate and multivariate normality, multicollinearity, and covariance homogeneity). In the case of finding a significant RAE effect, post hoc tests were applied to identify variables and age quartiles with a significant effect. Estimates of effect size for the main effects were calculated using partial eta squared (ηp2), interpreted as follows: 0.01—small; 0.06—medium; ≥0.14—large [49]. Finally, the analyses were executed using software version 4.1.0.

3. Results

The following Table 2, Table 3, Table 4 and Table 5 show the description of the variables from the average and standard deviation in relation to the evaluated variables of strength, speed, CODS agility, and EI, as well as for gender, sport, quartile, and year of birth.

3.1. Significant Differences for the CODS Agility Profile According to RAE

For the CODS agility variable, large significant differences were found for the right COD deficit between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.16), Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.049, ηp2 = 0.16), and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.16). Similarly, for the right time variable, significant differences were found in Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.18), Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.18), and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.18), and all these differences had a large effect size. On the contrary, in the right average speed variable, the effect size was medium; nevertheless, significant evidence was found when comparing Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.12) and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.12).
Statistically significant differences were evident in the left COD left deficit variable in Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.13), Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.044, ηp2 = 0.13), and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.13); this variable had a medium effect size. For the left time variable, the same significant differences were found as in the left COD deficit variable, except for the Q1 and Q4 comparison (p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.14). Finally, for left average speed, large significant differences were found in Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.13), Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.034, ηp2 = 0.13), and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.13) (Table 6).
When comparing the right COD deficit and right time with the sport, there were significant differences in volleyball players, with both variables showing differences between Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.022), all with a large effect size. Large significant differences were found in certain quartiles when comparing CODS agility variables with the female gender, such as right COD deficit in Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.29), left COD deficit in Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.031, ηp2 = 0.29), right time in Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.044, ηp2 = 0.29), as well as right time between Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.29) and left time in Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.29). Finally, in the right average speed variable, differences were found between Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.20), and in the left average speed variable, differences were between Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.03, ηp2= 0.30).

3.2. Significant Differences in the EI Profile According to the RAE

For EI, significant differences were observed in the mood variable between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.047, ηp2 = 0.08) and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.035, ηp2 = 0.08). Likewise, there were large significant differences in the interpersonal variable between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.12) and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.12). Finally, in the total emotional quotient, large significant differences were observed between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.13), Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.13), and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.13). For the positive impression variable, significant differences were found between Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.06), but with a small effect size (Table 7).
In the mood variable, large significant differences in the EI profile for soccer were found between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.05), Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.05), and Q3 and Q4 (p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.05). The interpersonal variable showed that large significant differences in the EI profile about soccer were found in Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.01) and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.01). Additionally, there were large significant differences in the EI profile for soccer that were found in the interpersonal variable in Q1 and Q3 (p = 0.001) and Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.033). Regarding the total emotional quotient, there was a medium effect size with large significant differences in Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.012, ηp2 = 0.13) and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.037, ηp2 = 0.13) (Table 8).
Continuing with the EI profile for the sport of volleyball, there were large significant differences in the interpersonal variable between Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.035, ηp2 = 0.01) and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.01). In the total emotional quotient variable, there were significant differences between Q2 and Q3 (p = 0.037). When comparing the EI profile by gender, significant differences for females were evident in the mood variable between Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.047) and the interpersonal variable in Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.018). Regarding the total emotional quotient, there were significant differences in Q1 and Q4 (p = 0.038, ηp2 = 0.21) and Q2 and Q4 (p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.21) along with a large effect size. There were significant differences when comparing men with the adaptability variable (p = 0.049, ηp2 = 0.11), with a medium effect size in this comparison.

3.3. Significant Differences for the SJ Profile

There were significant differences between sports for the profile exhibited by SJ jump variables and controlling for age quartiles (p = 0.029). For the power variable, Q2 in volleyball achieved higher results than Q3 in soccer, which had the lowest result for this variable. There were large significant differences between genders for the SJ jump profile by quartiles (p = 0.000), where male athletes showed greater strength in the jump than female athletes, especially in Q4, where males had higher results.

3.4. Significant Differences for the CMJ Profile

Within the CMJ jump profile, there were relevant differences between genders for the profile exhibited by CMJ jump variables, controlling for age quartiles (p = 0.000). Considering the analysis, both the female and male gender in Q4 had the highest results for the jump height variable, while Q3 had the lowest values for this variable (Figure 1).

3.5. Significant Differences for the CMJA Profile

For the CMJA profile controlled by quartiles, large significant differences between sports were evident (p = 0.023). In the jump height variable, Q2 volleyball players obtained higher results than Q4 soccer players. Contrary to this, Q1 in soccer achieved superior results to Q4 in volleyball. Regarding gender, large significant differences were found (p = 0.000), as male athletes in Q1 had a higher jump height than female athletes in Q4 (Figure 2).

3.6. Significant Differences in Speed Profile

There were large significant differences between sports in speed variables when controlling for quartiles (p = 0.000). In the 15 m speed variable, athletes from all quartiles in soccer had shorter times compared to volleyball athletes. Specifically, in the first quartile (Q1) of soccer, the time was notably shorter than in the fourth quartile (Q4) of volleyball.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to assess the RAE and its relationship with the development of physical capacities and EI in team sports (soccer and volleyball) and gender in Colombian athletes born in 2009 and 2010. Based on the findings of this research, it can be concluded that the RAE appears to be sensitive to CODS agility, 15 m speed, and SJ power variables concerning gender and sport. The jump height for CMJ concerning gender and the CMJA jump concerning sport were also considered. When comparing these CODS agility variables by gender, females exhibited the most significant differences in the right COD deficit for Q2 and Q4, the right time in Q2 and Q4, and the right average speed variable between Q2 and Q4. Likewise, EI appears to be a variable sensitive to the relative age effect.
The impact of relative age has been studied from a broad perspective, emphasizing its significance, with limited exploration regarding its connection to EI. This study serves as a precedent for understanding this phenomenon from physical and emotional perspectives, delving into how this effect manifests in these capacities.
Following Smith et al. [50], the RAE is observed between the 12 and 15 years of age range in female sports. This aligns with our findings, indicating the presence of this effect in both female and male team sports, which is consistent with other studies [51,52]. Similarities were found when comparing genders, with males achieving better results due to significant differences. This phenomenon was present for CODS agility variables in females (p < 0.006).
CODS agility showed significant differences within the same year, revealing that athletes born in the first two quartiles performed better than those in the last quartiles. Similar results were found by [53], who conducted a 30 m agility test. Moreover, [54] found significant differences between genders for speed (20 m) and CODS agility, corroborating the findings of this study. In line with the study by Ek et al. [55], the comparison between sports for the countermovement jump did not show significant differences between quartiles of relative age, expressing p < 0.12. However, contrary to this, the current study determined differences between sports for countermovement jumps in SJ and CMJA regarding power and jump height. The study by Sandercock et al. [56], which found better results for athletes born in November in cardiorespiratory fitness, strength, and power, did not align with the current study’s results. In this study, soccer players born between January and March (Q1) achieved superior results compared to Q4 in volleyball. When considering gender, significant differences (p = 0.000) were found, with male athletes in Q1 achieving greater jump heights than female athletes in Q4.
According to Yoshimura et al. [57], athletes who practiced volleyball had better results for the SJ jump profile when compared to other sports. These results did not correspond to those of the present study, where it was shown that for the profile exhibited by the SJ jumping variables, athletes who practiced soccer had better results compared to those who played volleyball (p = 0.029).
Warneke et al. [58] found significant differences between ages (10 and 14 years) for speed measured at 10 m and 20 m, stating that older participants achieved better results. However, the current research revealed no significant differences in speed measured at 5, 10, and 15 m when comparing birth years (2009 and 2010). Still, there were differences in speed variables concerning quartiles (p = 0.000). For the 15 m speed variable, athletes in all quartiles of soccer exhibited shorter times compared to those in volleyball. Significant differences between quartiles of relative age were found in the EI profile, with p < 0.001, indicating that athletes in Q1 and Q2 achieved better results in the total emotional quotient (eta squared 0.13). This contradicted [59], who found that relative age was an inconsistent predictor of perceptual–cognitive variable performance.
The study conducted by Soylu et al. [60] concluded that EI levels vary depending on age and positioning on the field, thus determining that psychological and physiological responses are equally necessary for football’s specific performance. A similar case to the results of the present study was identified, where differences in physical capacities were found in relation to SJ, CMJ, CMJA, CODS agility, and especially with EI variables related to interpersonal characteristics, intrapersonal characteristics, positive impression, total emotional quotient, and mood.
The variable related to EI in female soccer players allowed the specific character of the athlete concerning the specific demands of the sport to be determined, concluding that EI has a significant impact on human adaptability and the effectiveness of psychosocial functioning, so its study allows development paths for athletes to be identified [61]. With this, and following the findings of the present study, all evaluated EI variables turned out to be significant in response to the RAE. With this consideration, the results obtained offer the opportunity to propose specific actions that favor long-term sports development based on physical and psychological variables.
A study developed to understand EI and the practice of physical sports activity determined that children who practiced organized sports showed better skills in intrapersonal, interpersonal, adaptability, mood states, and EI. Similarly, studies on gender and age found that girls have higher levels of EI in the interpersonal dimension and mood states, while boys have higher levels of interpersonal intelligence and stress management [62]. In that sense, the results of the present study, by recognizing physical abilities and EI but in response to the RAE, found differences in mood states between Q1 and Q4, the interpersonal dimension between Q1 and Q4, and the emotional quotient for Q1–Q4 and Q2–Q4. Meanwhile, in the case of men, there were differences in adaptability. This corroborates the importance of recognizing psychological variables such as EI for evaluating athletes’ sports performance based on gender, sport, and year of birth.
Maghsoudipour et al. [63] used the Bar-On EI test on 20-year-old track and field athletes, concluding that EI, especially interpersonal skills, empathy, and independence, influence athlete performance. Similar results were found in the current study, where EI was proven to be sensitive to the RAE. On the contrary, [64] found no significant correlation between EI and sports performance (p > 0.05). In contrast, the current study determined that EI and physical abilities are sensitive to the RAE in soccer and volleyball players. There are differences in some EI components (empathy, interpersonal relationships, and independence) that were found to correlate with reaction time, similar to the current study’s findings, where significant differences were found for EI variables (mood, positive impression, interpersonal, intrapersonal, and total emotional quotient) along with CODS agility test times for both the left and right sides.
Additionally, this study identified, through the BarOn test, that sports practice is influenced by stress management and intrapersonal emotions. This indicates that athletes with higher scores in these categories could influence their results and achievements. This aligns with the current study’s results, demonstrating that better stress control and expression with others fall within the first and second quartiles of both branches, even when represented in sports. These findings are consistent with the results reported by Kopp et al. [33], who emphasized the use of EI in studies for performance comparisons between sports and performance levels, often based on birth years or nested quartiles, as observed in the current study concerning gender.
The consideration of EI can help establish the implementation of sports development programs since it is a determining variable to achieve results in sports performance [65]. This consideration is corroborated by the findings of the present study when considering that EI is a variable sensitive to RAE in young soccer and volleyball players.

Limitations and Future Directions

The main limitations are related to the number of Q4 athletes born in 2010 for both sports and genders. Based on the results of this study, some recommendations were identified for future lines of research. Firstly, the influence of EI on sports performance in various sports continues to be a challenge for the different areas that contribute to the long-term sports process, especially sports psychology. On the other hand, it is suggested that future research can incorporate the study of EI together with other variables associated with sports performance, such as physical and technical–tactical capacities.
It is suggested that studies continue evaluating and developing scientific production comparing physical and, especially, emotional capacities in a larger population and different sports. Given the limited investigations related to EI and considering the findings of this research, statistically significant differences were evidenced in EI concerning the RAE. Finally, it is necessary to note that these findings can help enhance the understanding of other variables influencing sports performance in relation to EI. Longitudinal studies with the variables investigated in this research and the integration of technical–tactical variables are suggested to determine the changes athletes experience in their sports performance.

5. Conclusions

The EI seems to be a variable sensitive to the RAE. Significant differences were found for nested quartiles, gender, and sport in interpersonal variables for Q1 and Q4, Q2 and Q4, the total emotional quotient between Q2 and Q4, mood in Q1 and Q4, Q2 and Q4, and positive impression between Q2 and Q3.
In conclusion, this study focused on considering physical (SJ, CMJ, CMJA, CODS agility, and speed) and psychological (EI) athletes according to gender variables to understand the effect of RAE on soccer and volleyball players, demonstrating that mainly emotional variables are more sensitive to this effect for the evaluated athletes.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.D.Á.-M. and J.V.-M.; methodology, M.A.C.-M., S.C.-C. and B.A.B.-P.; software, B.A.B.-P.; validation, J.D.Á.-M., B.A.B.-P. and J.P.-O.; formal analysis, M.A.C.-M. and B.A.B.-P.; investigation, M.A.C.-M., J.V.-M. and J.D.Á.-M.; resources, J.D.Á.-M. and S.C.-C.; data curation, J.D.Á.-M. and M.A.C.-M.; writing—original draft preparation, M.A.C.-M. and J.V.-M.; writing—review and editing, B.A.B.-P. and J.P.-O.; visualization, J.P.-O. and B.A.B.-P.; supervision, B.A.B.-P. and J.P.-O.; project administration, B.A.B.-P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in strict adherence to the principles outlined in the most recent version of the Helsinki Declaration, ensuring the ethical considerations and welfare of the participants. Resolution 8430 of the Colombian Ministry of Health was also taken into consideration, which establishes the guidelines for research using non-invasive procedures. The study received approval from the National Pedagogical University’s ethics committee (340ETIC-2024).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors without undue reservation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Fernandez Ortega, J.A.; Rodriguez Buitrago, J.A.; Sanchez Rodriguez, D.A. Central aspects of the identification and development of sports talents: A systematic review. Retos 2021, 39, 915–928. [Google Scholar]
  2. Feeley, B.T.; Agel, J.; LaPrade, R.F. When Is It Too Early for Single Sport Specialization. Am. J. Sports Med. 2016, 44, 234–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Rubajczyk, K.; Rokita, A. The Relative Age Effect and Talent Identification Factors in Youth Volleyball in Poland. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Sweeney, L.; Taylor, J.; MacNamara, Á. Push and Pull Factors: Contextualising Biological Maturation and Relative Age in Talent Development Systems. Children 2023, 10, 130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Bezuglov, E.; Malyakin, G.; Emanov, A.; Malyshev, G.; Shoshorina, M.; Savin, E.; Lazarev, A.; Morgans, R. Are Late-Born Young Soccer Players Less Mature Than Their Early-Born Peers, Although No Differences in Physical and Technical Performance Are Evident? Sports 2023, 11, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Ntozis, C.; Cherouveim, E.; Gountas, K.; Bakodimos, G.; Apostolidis, N.; Tsolakis, C. Relative age effect in Greek female young volleyball players: Data from the national talent identification program. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2021, 21, 1867–1975. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ginés, H.J.; Huertas, F.; García Calvo, T.; Ponce-Bordón, J.C.; Figueiredo, A.J.; Ballester, R. Age and Maturation Matter in Youth Elite Soccer, but Depending on Competitive Level and Gender. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Pérez-González, B.; León-Quismondo, J.; Bonal, J.; Burillo, P.; Fernández-Luna, Á. The New Generation of Professional Soccer Talent Is Born under the Bias of the RAE: Relative Age Effect in International Male Youth Soccer Championships. Children 2021, 8, 1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Till, K.; Lloyd, R.S.; McCormack, S.; Williams, G.; Baker, J.; Eisenmann, J.C. Optimising long-term athletic development: An investigation of practitioners’ knowledge, adherence, practices and challenges. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0262995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Laureys, F.; Middelbos, L.; Rommers, N.; De Waelle, S.; Coppens, E.; Mostaert, M.; Deconoinck, F.; Lenoir, M. The effects of age, boiological maturation and sex on the development of executive functions in adolescents. Front. Physiol. 2021, 12, 703312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hill, M.; Scott, S.; Malina, R.M.; McGee, D.; Cumming, S.P. Relative age and maturation selection biases in academy football. J. Sports Sci. 2020, 38, 1359–1367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Toselli, S.; Mauro, M.; Grigoletto, A.; Cataldi, S.; Benedetti, L.; Nanni, G.; Di Miceli, R.; Aiello, P.; Gallamini, D.; Fischetti, F.; et al. Maturation Selection Biases and Relative Age Effect in Italian Soccer Players of Different Levels. Biology 2022, 11, 1559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Isorna, M.; Vaquero, R.; Vinuesa, J.; Rial, A. The Effect of Relative Age on Participation in the National Sports Training Plan and the Achievement of Sporting Success in Canoeing. Apunt. Educ. Física Y Deportes 2014, 115, 46–53. [Google Scholar]
  14. Brown, K.A.; Patel, D.R.; Darmawan, D. Participation in sports in relation to adolescent growth and development. Transl. Pediatr. 2017, 6, 150–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Cobley, S.; Baker, J.; Wattie, N.; McKenna, J. Annual age-grouping, and athlete development. Sports Med. 2009, 39, 235–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Helsen, W.F.; van Winckel, J.; Williams, A.M. The relative age effect in youth soccer across Europe. J. Sports Sci. 2005, 23, 629–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Becerra-Patiño, B.; Escorcia-Clavijo, J. Effect of relative age in the FIFA Women’s World Cup U-20 and senior elite categories: Differences in playing positions and obtained results. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2023, 23, 613–621. [Google Scholar]
  18. Peña-González, I.; Javaloyes, A.; Sarabia, J.M.; Moya-Ramón, M. Relative age-related differences between different competitive levels and field positions in young soccer players. Res. Sports Med. 2021, 29, 254–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sarmento, H.; Anguera, M.; Pereira, A.; Araujo, D. Talent Identification and Development in Male Football: A Systematic Review. Sports Med. 2018, 48, 907–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Práxedes, A.; Moreno, A.; García-González, L.; Pizarro, D.; Del Villar, F. The Relative Age Effect on Soccer Players in Formative Stages with Different Sport Expertise Levels. J. Hum. Kinet. 2017, 60, 167–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Lovell, R.; Towlson, C.; Parkin, G.; Portas, M.; Vaeyens, R.; Cobley, S. Soccer player characteristics in English lower-league development programmes: The relationships between relative age, maturation, anthropometry and physical fitness. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0137238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Papadopoulou, S.; Papadopoulou, S.; Rosemann, T.; Knechtle, B.; Nikolaidis, P. Relative age effect on youth female volleyball players: A pilot study on its prevalence and relationship with anthropometric and physiological characteristics. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Sattler, T.; Hadzic, V.; Dervisevic, E.; Markovic, G. Vertical jump performance of professional male and female volleyball players: Effects of playing position and competition level. J. Strength. Cond. Res. 2015, 29, 1486–1493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Nikolaidis, P.; Afonso, J.; Buśko, K.; Ingebrigtsen, J.; Chtourou, H.; Martin, J. Positional differences of physical traits and physiological characteristics in female volleyball players—The role of age. Kinesiology 2015, 47, 75–81. [Google Scholar]
  25. Becerra-Patiño, B.A.; Sarria-Lozano, J.C.; Palomino, F.J. Characterization of variables associated with sports performance: Interdisciplinarity in women’s soccer in Colombia. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2023, 23, 76–95. [Google Scholar]
  26. Barberis, N.; Calaresi, D.; Gugliandolo, M.C. Relationships among Trait EI, Need Fulfilment, and Performance Strategies. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2019, 4, 50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Zwierko, M.; Jedziniak, W.; Popowczak, M.; Rokita, A. Reactive Agility in Competitive Young Volleyball Players: A Gender Comparison of Perceptual-Cognitive and Motor Determinants. J. Hum. Kinet. 2023, 4, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Eime, R.M.; Young, J.A.; Harvey, J.T.; Charity, M.; Payne, W. A systematic review of the psychological and social benefits of participation in sport for children and adolescents: Informing development of a conceptual model of health through sport. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2013, 10, 98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Laborde, S.; Dosseville, F.; Allen, M.S. Emotional intelligence in sport and exercise: A systematic review. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2016, 26, 862–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Jekauc, D.; Brand, R. Editorial: How do Emotions and Feelings Regulate Physical Activity? Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Tenenbaum, G.; Basevitch, I.; Gershgoren, L.; Filho, E. Emotions–decision-making in sport: Theoretical conceptualization and experimental evidence. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2013, 11, 151–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Lochbaum, M.; Zanatta, T.; Kirschling, D.; May, E. The Profile of Moods States and Athletic Performance: A Meta-Analysis of Published Studies. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2021, 11, 50–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Kopp, A.; Jekauc, D. The Influence of Emotional Intelligence on Performance in Competitive Sports: A Meta-Analytical Investigation. Sports 2018, 6, 175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Chen, Y.; Peng, Y.; Fang, P. Emotional Intelligence Mediates the Relationship between Age and Subjective Well-Being. Int. J. Aging. Hum. Dev. 2016, 83, 91–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Becerra Patiño, B.A.; Varón-Murcia, J.J.; Cárdenas-Contreras, S.; Castro-Malaver, M.A.; Ávila-Martínez, J.D. Scientific production on the relative age effect in sport: Bibliometric analysis of the last 9 years (2015–2023). Retos 2024, 52, 623–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Křen, F.; Kudláček, M.; Wąsowicz, W.; Groffik, D.; Frömel, K. Gender differences in preferences of individual and team sports in Polish adolescents. Acta Gymnica. 2012, 42, 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Chiwaridzo, M.; Ferguson, G.D.; Smits-Engelsman, B.C. A systematic review protocol investigating tests for physical or physiological qualities and game-specific skills commonly used in rugby and related sports and their psychometric properties. Syst. Rev. 2016, 5, 122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. World Medical Association. Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J. Am. Coll. Dent. 2014, 81, 14–18. [Google Scholar]
  39. Ministry of Health of Colombia. Resolution Number 8430. 1993. Available online: https://www.minsalud.gov.co/sites/rid/Lists/BibliotecaDigital/RIDE/DE/DIJ/RESOLUCION-8430-DE-1993.pdf (accessed on 7 April 2024).
  40. Balsalobre, F.C.; Glaister, M.; Lockey, R. The validity and reliability of an iPhone app for measuring vertical jump performance. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2015, 33, 1574–1579. [Google Scholar]
  41. Gallardo-Fuentes, F.; Gallardo-Fuentes, J.; Ramírez-Campillo, R.; Balsalobre-Fernández, C.; Martínez, C.; Caniuqueo, A.; Cañas, R.; Banzer, W.; Loturco, I.; Nakamura, F.; et al. Intersession and Intrasession Reliability and Validity of the My Jump App for Measuring Different Jump Actions in Trained Male and Female Athletes. J. Strength. Cond. Res. 2016, 30, 2049–2056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Balsalobre-Fernandez, C.; Bishop, C.; Beltrán-Garrido, J.; Cecilia-Gallego, P.; Cuenca-Amigó, A.; Romero-Rodríguez, D.; Madruga-Parera, M. The validity and reliability of a novel app for the measurement of change of direction performance. J. Sports Sci. 2019, 37, 2420–2424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Balsalobre-Fernández, C.; Agopyan, H.; Morin, J.B. The validity and reliability of an iPhone app for measuring running mechanics. J. Appl. Biomech. 2017, 33, 222–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Bar-On, R.; Parker, J. EQ-i: YV Bar on Emotional Quotient Inventory: Youth Version; Technical Manual; Multi-Health System, Inc.: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  45. Gilar-Corbi, R.; Valdés, M.V.; Navas, L.; Holgado-Tello, F.P.; Castejón, J.L. Validation of the Bar-On EQ-i: YV (S) Inventory in Its Spanish Version: Gender-Based Invariance Analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 1643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Rodríguez Lorenzo, L.; Fernández Del Olmo, M.; Sánchez Molina, J.; Martín Acero, R. Observable Criteria for Assessing Vertical Jump Tests Performance: SJ, CMJ, CMJA, 1RJA. J. Sports Train. 2020, 34, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  47. Ateş, C.; Kaymaz, Ö.; Kale, H.E.; Tekindal, M.A. Comparison of Test Statistics of Nonnormal and Unbalanced Samples for Multivariate Analysis of Variance in terms of Type-I Error Rates. Comput. Math. Methods Med. 2019, 18, 2173638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Dasgupta, S. Encyclopedia of Biostatistics. Pillai’s Trace Test; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lakens, D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Smith, K.L.; Weir, P.L.; Till, K.; Romann, M.; Cobley, S. Relative Age Effects Across and Within Female Sport Contexts: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sports Med. 2018, 48, 1451–1478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Prieto-Ayuso, A.; Martínez-Gorroño, M.E. Influence of Relative Age Effect in Basic Physical Abilities. Rev. Int. Med. Y Cienc. De Act. Física Y El Deporte 2017, 17, 413–434. [Google Scholar]
  52. Kelly, A.L.; Jackson, D.T.; Barrell, D.; Burke, K.; Till, K. The relative age effect in male and female English age-grade rugby union: Exploring the gender-specific mechanisms that underpin participation. Sci. Med. Footb. 2022, 6, 277–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Gil, S.M.; Badiola, A.; Bidaurrazaga-Letona, I.; Zabala-Lili, J.; Gravina, L.; Santos-Concejero, J.; Lekue, J.A.; Granados, C. Relationship between the relative age effect and anthropometry, maturity and performance in young soccer players. J. Sports Sci. 2014, 32, 479–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Albaladejo-Saura, M.; Vaquero-Cristóbal, R.; García-Roca, J.A.; Esparza-Ros, F. The Effect of Age, Biological Maturation and Birth Quartile in the Kinanthropometric and Physical Fitness Differences between Male and Female Adolescent Volleyball Players. Children 2022, 9, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Ek, S.; Wollmer, P.; Karlsson, M.K.; Peterson, T.; Thorsson, O.; Olsson, M.C.; Malmborg, J.S.; Dencker, M. Relative Age Effect of Sport Academy Adolescents, a Physiological Evaluation. Sports 2020, 8, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Sandercock, G.R.; Ogunleye, A.A.; Parry, D.A.; Cohen, D.D.; Taylor, M.J.; Voss, C. Athletic performance and birth month: Is the relative age effect more than just selection bias? Int. J. Sports Med. 2014, 35, 1017–1023. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  57. Yoshimura, M.; Umemura, Y. Comparison of Factors Related to Jump Performance in Volleyball Players and Swimmers. J. Exerc. Physiol. Online 2016, 19, 53–65. [Google Scholar]
  58. Warneke, K.; Wagner, C.M.; Konrad, A.; Kadlubowski, B.; Sander, A.; Wirth, K.; Keiner, M. The influence of age and sex on speed-strength performance in children between 10 and 14 years of age. Front. Physiol. 2023, 14, 1092874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Huertas, F.; Ballester, R.; Gines, H.J.; Hamidi, A.K.; Moratal, C.; Lupiáñez, J. Relative Age Effect in the Sport Environment. Role of Physical Fitness and Cognitive Function in Youth Soccer Players. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Soylu, Y. Comparison of emotional intelligence levels of soccer players according to age and playing position. Acta Gymnica. 2021, 51, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Rutkowska, K.; Bergier, J. Psychological Gender and Emotional Intelligence in Youth Female Soccer Players. J. Hum. Kinet. 2015, 47, 285–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Amado-Alonso, D.; León-del-Barco, B.; Mendo-Lázaro, S.; Sánchez-Miguel, P.A.; Iglesias Gallego, D. Emotional Intelligence and the Practice of Organized Physical-Sport Activity in Children. Sustainability 2019, 11, 1615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Maghsoudipour, M.; Shabani, H.; Najafabadi, M.G.; Bakhshi, E.; Coh, P. The relationship between emotional intelligence, reaction time, aerobic capacity and performance in female track and field athletes at the Universities of Tehran. Work 2018, 61, 173–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Ubago, J.J.L.; Gonzalez, V.G.; Puertas, M.P. Development of emotional intelligence through physical activity and sport practice. Syst. Rev. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Kopp, A.; Reichert, M.; Jekauc, D. Trait and Ability Emotional Intelligence and Its Impact on Sports Performance of Athletes. Sports 2021, 9, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. CMJ jump height by quartile and gender.
Figure 1. CMJ jump height by quartile and gender.
Jfmk 09 00244 g001
Figure 2. CMJA jump height by quartile and sport.
Figure 2. CMJA jump height by quartile and sport.
Jfmk 09 00244 g002
Table 1. Bivariate characterization of height, body mass, gender, sport, and quartile.
Table 1. Bivariate characterization of height, body mass, gender, sport, and quartile.
VariableHeightBody Mass
GenderFemaleMaleFemaleMale
SportVolleyball158.09 ± 5.70169.63 ± 8.5751.86 ± 9.4556.88 ± 9.95
Soccer156.53 ± 6.69161.18 ± 7.7848.36 ± 6.9147.83 ± 6.58
Year2009
QuartileQ1157.91 ± 8.52170.77 ± 9.5050.11 ± 5.8057.10 ± 9.55
Q2157.25 ± 5.23163.35 ± 9.2952.95 ± 5.0548.41 ± 6.94
Q3158.73 ± 4.71164.91 ± 8.0358.08 ± 12.352.34 ± 7.58
Q4157.21 ± 3.40176.22 ± 4.8149.08 ± 5.4057.68 ± 4.48
Year2010
QuartileQ1155.75 ± 7.77166.56 ± 7.3947.72 ± 5.5653.19 ± 9.45
Q2158.11 ± 5.66164.36 ± 6.7945.29 ± 3.1953.52 ± 11.9
Q3156.23 ± 7.88156.23 ± 8.6153.75 ± 13.545.57 ± 8.43
Table 2. Bivariate characterization of SJ, CMJ, and CMJA in response to gender, the sport practiced and quartile of athletes.
Table 2. Bivariate characterization of SJ, CMJ, and CMJA in response to gender, the sport practiced and quartile of athletes.
VariableJH SJJH CMJJH CMJA
GenderFemaleMenFemaleMenFemaleMen
SportVoleyball24.98 ± 4.125.66 ± 4.125.66 ± 4.137.19 ± 8.029.33 ± 4.643.15 ± 7.8
Soccer25.75 ± 4.727.91 ± 4.927.91 ± 4.931.39 ± 4.329.85 ± 5.335.40 ± 4.9
Year2009
QQ126.12 ± 2.536.65 ± 6.127.02 ± 3.437.72 ± 6.130.57 ± 4.043.70 ± 7.8
Q226.72 ± 4.430.71 ± 5.627.20 ± 4.433.28 ± 4.529.47 ± 4.637.55 ± 7.4
Q322.88 ± 5.834.78 ± 5.525.39 ± 5.436.89 ± 6.029.18 ± 6.241.64 ± 4.2
Q426.03 ± 2.839.10 ± 5.927.27 ± 6.043.40 ± 7.129.50 ± 3.146.89 ± 10.1
Year2010
QQ125.91 ± 4.629.65 ± 2.127.09 ± 4.632.13 ± 6.333.01 ± 7.538.79 ± 6.0
Q224.15 ± 4.132.28 ± 7.126.34 ± 4.134.78 ± 7.529.64 ± 4.338.80 ± 6.5
Q323.78 ± 4.625.82 ± 3.924.88 ± 4.127.56 ± 3.827.41 ± 4.331.60 ± 4.0
Q427.78 ± 6.3/29.7/27.70 ± 4.0/
JH SJ: Jump height squat jump; JH CMJ: jump height countermovement jump; JH CMJA: jump height countermovement jump with arms; and Q: quartile.
Table 3. Bivariate characterization of speed.
Table 3. Bivariate characterization of speed.
Var0–5 m5–10 m10–15 mTotal 15 m
GenderFemaleMenFemaleMenFemaleMenFemaleMen
SV1.70 ± 0.21.51 ± 0.30.96 ± 0.00.82 ± 0.00.89 ± 0.10.79 ± 0.03.55 ± 0.33.11 ± 0.4
So1.51 ± 0.21.37 ± 0.20.86 ± 0.00.82 ± 0.00.79 ± 0.00.76 ± 0.03.18 ± 0.22.95 ± 0.3
Year2009
QQ11.69 ± 0.31.45 ± 0.20.91 ± 0.00.81 ± 0.00.93 ± 0.00.73 ± 0.03.53 ± 0.42.99 ± 0.3
Q21.63 ± 0.21.39 ± 0.20.89 ± 0.00.80 ± 0.00.81 ± 0.00.76 ± 0.03.35 ± 0.22.92 ± 0.2
Q31.59 ± 0.41.39 ± 0.30.89 ± 0.10.76 ± 0.00.80 ± 0.00.78 ± 0.03.28 ± 0.42.95 ± 0.4
Q41.67 ± 0.21.51 ± 0.40.97 ± 0.00.81 ± 0.10.84 ± 0.10.77 ± 0.03.49 ± 0.43.09 ± 0.5
Year2010
QQ11.65 ± 0.21.52 ± 0.30.92 ± 0.10.85 ± 0.00.86 ± 0.10.78 ± 0.03.43 ± 0.33.16 ± 0.3
Q21.42 ± 0.21.41 ± 0.20.87 ± 0.00.82 ± 0.00.80 ± 0.00.75 ± 0.03.10 ± 0.32.96 ± 0.3
Q31.63 ± 0.31.39 ± 0.20.94 ± 0.00.86 ± 0.00.87 ± 0.10.83 ± 0.03.45 ± 0.43.08 ± 0.3
Q41.64 ± 0.3/0.93 ± 0.0/0.90 ± 0.1/3.47 ± 0.4/
Var: variable; S: sport; V: volleyball; So; soccer; and Q: quartile.
Table 4. Bivariate characterization of CODS agility.
Table 4. Bivariate characterization of CODS agility.
VarCODS Agility RightCODS Agility Left
GenderFemaleFemaleMenFemaleMen
SV0.96 ± 0.020.7 ± 1.8218.1 ± 1.2120.9 ± 1.7018.2 ± 1.29
So0.86 ± 0.018.4 ± 1.0116.8 ± 1.1218.4 ± 1.1216.9 ± 1.05
Year2009
QQ10.91 ± 0.019.8 ± 2.1117.0 ± 1.2120.1 ± 2.2616.8 ± 1.11
Q20.89 ± 0.018.5 ± 0.7717.0 ± 0.9718.4 ± 0.9717.0 ± 0.87
Q30.89 ± 0.119.7 ± 1.6518.0 ± 1.6919.4 ± 1.7518.0 ± 1.26
Q40.97 ± 0.020.7 ± 2.4317.7 ± 0.5220.6 ± 2.2117.6 ± 0.53
Year2010
QQ10.92 ± 0.119.1 ± 1.4917.6 ± 1.1519.4 ± 1.8717.9 ± 1.21
Q20.87 ± 0.018.8 ± 1.2816.9 ± 1.4019.5 ± 1.9017.1 ± 1.65
Q30.94 ± 0.020.1 ± 2.0217.9 ± 1.7120.2 ± 1.7818.2 ± 1.59
Q40.93 ± 0.020.9 ± 1.75/21.2 ± 2.03/
Var: variable; S: sport; V: volleyball; So; soccer; and Q: quartile.
Table 5. Characterization of EI variables.
Table 5. Characterization of EI variables.
VarTotal Emotional QuotientIntrapersonalInterpersonalAdaptabilityStress ManagementMood
GenFemaleMenFemaleMenFemaleMenFemaleMenFemaleMenFemaleMen
SV176 ± 21.0196 ± 21.393 ± 15.696 ± 11.5102 ± 9.2105 ± 12.5102 ± 20.1118 ± 16.989 ± 15.7992 ± 15.0399 ± 18.4100 ± 16.9
So181 ± 17.6202 ± 18.392 ± 13.0100 ± 11.2100 ± 13.4105 ± 13.9103 ± 17.2114 ± 22.792 ± 13.1791 ± 12.42104 ± 14.8111 ± 9.9
Year2009
QQ1189 ± 26.6215 ± 16.2100 ± 22.5105 ± 8.8105 ± 6.5107 ± 10.2110 ± 25.0120 ± 12.894 ± 7.6399 ± 13.99111 ± 32.6118 ± 7.5
Q2184 ± 10.8209 ± 19.393 ± 10.51007.5102 ± 11.2106 ± 10.3105 ± 19.8124 ± 23.694 ± 14.7395 ± 17.50107 ± 12.8111 ± 12.3
Q3174 ± 20.4192 ± 23.382 ± 12.898 ± 11.1106 ± 14.899 ± 15.8103 ± 18.5108 ± 20.587 ± 17.3094 ± 18.28101 ± 14.9105 ± 14.5
Q4169 ± 15.7206 ± 27.589 ± 12.899 ± 12.597 ± 10.497 ± 17.497 ± 16.7135 ± 11.887 ± 13.4594 ± 10.2795 ± 14.68105 ± 19.8
Year2010
QQ1182 ± 14.9191 ± 13.796 ± 13.195 ± 15.1106 ± 9.5107 ± 12.597 ± 19.2104 ± 22.295 ± 14.5889 ± 11.13105 ± 11.4100 ± 15.3
Q2187 ± 22.3201 ± 18.2100 ± 12.099 ± 14.0104 ± 13.9111 ± 11.6113 ± 18.2122 ± 15.690 ± 11.5687 ± 11.32103 ± 15.2105 ± 13.3
Q3173 ± 24.2189 ± 17.189 ± 16.397 ± 7.79100 ± 7.899 ± 12.0100 ± 16.8112 ± 17.486 ± 20.0789 ± 11.80100 ± 16.2100 ± 16.2
Q4163 ± 5.1/95 ± 11.9/89 ± 11.9/102 ± 18.3/86 ± 11.62/86 ± 16.51/
Var: variable; Gen: gender; S: sport; V: volleyball; So; soccer; Q: quartile; and EI: emotional intelligence.
Table 6. Results of the CODS agility profile according to the RAE by quartile.
Table 6. Results of the CODS agility profile according to the RAE by quartile.
VariableGroup 1Group 2p. AdjEffect SizeCI (95%)
COD right deficitQ1Q40.009 **0.16[0.07, 1.00]
COD right deficitQ2Q30.049 *0.16[0.07, 1.00]
COD right deficitQ2Q40.000 ***0.16[0.07, 1.00]
Right timeQ1Q40.009 **0.18[0.08, 1.00]
Right timeQ2Q30.018 *0.18[0.08, 1.00]
Right timeQ2Q40.000 ***0.18[0.08, 1.00]
Right average speedQ1Q40.006 **0.12[0.03, 1.00]
Right average speedQ2Q40.000 ***0.12[0.03, 1.00]
VariableGroup 1Group 2p. AdjEffect SizeCI (95%)
COD left deficitQ1Q40.026 *0.13[0.04, 1.00]
COD left deficitQ2Q30.044 *0.13[0.04, 1.00]
COD left deficitQ2Q40.003 **0.13[0.04, 1.00]
Left timeQ1Q40.027 *0.14[0.05, 1.00]
Left timeQ2Q30.044 *0.14[0.05, 1.00]
Left timeQ2Q40.003 **0.14[0.05, 1.00]
Left average speedQ1Q40.022 *0.13[0.04, 1.00]
Left average speedQ2Q30.034 *0.13[0.04, 1.00]
Left average speedQ2Q40.002 **0.13[0.04, 1.00]
*, significant difference at p < 0.05, **, significant difference at p < 0.01, and ***, significant difference at p < 0.001.
Table 7. Results of EI profile according to RAE.
Table 7. Results of EI profile according to RAE.
VariableGroup 1Group 2p. AdjEffect SizeCI (95%)
MoodQ1Q40.047 *0.08[0.01, 1.00]
MoodQ2Q40.035 *0.08[0.01, 1.00]
Positive impressionQ2Q30.024 *0.06[0.00, 1.00]
InterpersonalQ1Q40.003 **0.12[0.04, 1.00]
InterpersonalQ2Q40.009 **0.12[0.04, 1.00]
Total emotional quotientQ1Q40.013 *0.13[0.04, 1.00]
Total emotional quotientQ2Q30.024 *0.13[0.04, 1.00]
Total emotional quotientQ2Q40.005 **0.13[0.04, 1.00]
*, significant difference at p < 0.05, **, significant difference at p < 0.01.
Table 8. EI profile results according to RAE by sport (soccer).
Table 8. EI profile results according to RAE by sport (soccer).
VariableGroup 1Group 2p. AdjEffect SizeCI (95%)
MoodQ1Q40.004 **0.05[0.01, 1.00]
MoodQ2Q40.003 **0.05[0.01, 1.00]
MoodQ3Q40.006 **0.05[0.01, 1.00]
InterpersonalQ1Q40.004 **0.01[0.00, 1.00]
InterpersonalQ2Q40.014 *0.01[0.00, 1.00]
IntrapersonalQ1Q30.001 ***-[0.00, 1.00]
IntrapersonalQ2Q30.033 *-[0.00, 1.00]
Total emotional quotientQ1Q40.012 *0.13[0.04, 1.00]
Total emotional quotientQ2Q40.037 *0.13[0.04, 1.00]
*, significant difference at p < 0.05, **, significant difference at p < 0.01, and ***, significant difference at p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ávila-Martínez, J.D.; Castro-Malaver, M.A.; Becerra-Patiño, B.A.; Varón-Murcia, J.; Cárdenas-Contreras, S.; Pino-Ortega, J. Gender Differences for the Relative Age Effect on Physical Skills and Emotional Intelligence in Child Volleyball and Soccer Athletes. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2024, 9, 244. https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk9040244

AMA Style

Ávila-Martínez JD, Castro-Malaver MA, Becerra-Patiño BA, Varón-Murcia J, Cárdenas-Contreras S, Pino-Ortega J. Gender Differences for the Relative Age Effect on Physical Skills and Emotional Intelligence in Child Volleyball and Soccer Athletes. Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology. 2024; 9(4):244. https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk9040244

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ávila-Martínez, Juan D., Michael A. Castro-Malaver, Boryi A. Becerra-Patiño, Juliana Varón-Murcia, Stefania Cárdenas-Contreras, and José Pino-Ortega. 2024. "Gender Differences for the Relative Age Effect on Physical Skills and Emotional Intelligence in Child Volleyball and Soccer Athletes" Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology 9, no. 4: 244. https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk9040244

APA Style

Ávila-Martínez, J. D., Castro-Malaver, M. A., Becerra-Patiño, B. A., Varón-Murcia, J., Cárdenas-Contreras, S., & Pino-Ortega, J. (2024). Gender Differences for the Relative Age Effect on Physical Skills and Emotional Intelligence in Child Volleyball and Soccer Athletes. Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology, 9(4), 244. https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk9040244

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop