Next Article in Journal
Comparing the Effects of FIFO/DIDO Workers Being Home versus Away on Sleep and Loneliness for Partners of Australian Mining Workers
Previous Article in Journal
Lucid Dreaming and the Feeling of Being Refreshed in the Morning: A Diary Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Opinion

The Role of Daylight for Humans: Gaps in Current Knowledge

1
Sleep/Wake Research Centre, Massey University Wellington, Wellington 6021, New Zealand
2
Centre for Chronobiology, Psychiatric Hospital of the University of Basel, 4002 Basel, Switzerland
3
Transfaculty Research Platform Molecular and Cognitive Neurosciences (MCN), University of Basel, 4002 Basel, Switzerland
4
Chronobiology and Sleep Research Group, Institute of Pharmacology and Toxicology, University of Zürich, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland
5
Faculty for Health and Social Affairs, University of Applied Sciences for Economics and Management (FOM), 45141 Essen, Germany
6
SynOpus, 44789 Bochum, Germany
7
Psychiatric Center Copenhagen, University of Copenhagen, Rigshospitalet, 2100 Copenhagen, Denmark
8
Department of Integrative Physiology, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
9
Division of Endocrinology, Metabolism and Diabetes, University of Colorado, Aurora, CO 80045, USA
10
Departments of Radiation Sciences and Molecular Biology, Umeå University, 901 87 Umeå, Sweden
11
Wallenberg Centre for Molecular Medicine (WCMM), Umeå University, 901 87 Umeå, Sweden
12
Chronobiology, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 7XH, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Clocks & Sleep 2020, 2(1), 61-85; https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep2010008
Submission received: 31 October 2019 / Revised: 20 February 2020 / Accepted: 21 February 2020 / Published: 28 February 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Impact of Light & other Zeitgebers)

Abstract

:
Daylight stems solely from direct, scattered and reflected sunlight, and undergoes dynamic changes in irradiance and spectral power composition due to latitude, time of day, time of year and the nature of the physical environment (reflections, buildings and vegetation). Humans and their ancestors evolved under these natural day/night cycles over millions of years. Electric light, a relatively recent invention, interacts and competes with the natural light–dark cycle to impact human biology. What are the consequences of living in industrialised urban areas with much less daylight and more use of electric light, throughout the day (and at night), on general health and quality of life? In this workshop report, we have classified key gaps of knowledge in daylight research into three main groups: (I) uncertainty as to daylight quantity and quality needed for “optimal” physiological and psychological functioning, (II) lack of consensus on practical measurement and assessment methods and tools for monitoring real (day) light exposure across multiple time scales, and (III) insufficient integration and exchange of daylight knowledge bases from different disciplines. Crucial short and long-term objectives to fill these gaps are proposed.

1. Introduction: The Need to Identify Current Gaps of Knowledge in Daylight Research

Our rotating planet, with its alternating 24-h light–dark cycles, changing day lengths with seasons, and weather conditions, has major effects on the physiology and behaviour of organisms from prokaryotes to mammals. One adaptive response to this dynamically changing geophysical environment in most organisms has been the evolution of biological time-keeping systems, i.e., biological “clocks” capable of synchronisation (entrainment) to environmental time cues, providing the enormous advantage of anticipation to environmental changes, allowing corresponding adaptations in behaviour (e.g., shelter, reproduction) and physiology (e.g., metabolism, quiescence). Since the early 1980s, it is known that many aspects of biological clock function are highly conserved across species, for example, the intracellular structure of clocks themselves, a feedback loop encoded by dedicated “clock genes” first described in fruit flies [1,2] and later in mammals [3]. Another highly conserved evolutionary mechanism is (day-) light perception via specialised light-sensitive molecules in specific cells on the outer surface of multicellular organisms. Within the animal kingdom, opsins are the common photopigment molecules of all visual systems [4,5]. A unique ancestral photopigment melanopsin—highly conserved and considered the evolutionary linkage between invertebrate and vertebrate photopigments—plays a special role in so-called nonvisual light perception, including circadian photoentrainment [6,7,8]. The intrinsically photosensitive properties of melanopsin were first described in frog skin [9], and later in mammalian eyes [10,11,12], including humans [13]. These examples emphasise the fact that important time-keeping processes supporting adaptation to the natural day–night environment evolved over a large time scale (millions of years).
Only very recently on this time scale, i.e., less than 200 years ago, technical progress—the discovery of electric lighting (and electric power)—enabled, for the first time, relative independence from (natural) daylight. Even though this invention has been of great benefit for humankind, it has also thoroughly and irreversibly changed our home, social and work environments. There are two main reasons for these changes: Firstly, relative independence from the natural light–dark cycle fostered 24/7 work and living conditions throughout the developed world. Secondly, the use of electric lighting has not only been restricted to night time but has also made it possible to live and work independently of daylight during the day. There is now growing evidence that these changes have had broad consequences for health1 issues worldwide (see, e.g., [14]).
One aspect of these health consequences relates to inadequate “entrainment” of biological clocks to electric light. Entrainment is an active process by which environmental time cues, zeitgebers (German for “time cue”), such as the light–dark cycle, produce stable and appropriate timing of internal biological clocks [15,16,17]. Within the biological rhythm research community, there are concerns about the weakness of zeitgeber strength for circadian entrainment in electrically lit environments (with no or low natural daylight exposure). Circadian disruption, as a consequence of inadequate entrainment, could impact on robustness, regularity, and amplitude of biological rhythms in individuals and within different populations [18]. Adverse health effects, in which the lack of sufficient daylight has been directly (daily light and dark exposure) or indirectly (via circadian misalignment) involved, range from sleep problems and impaired daytime functioning to increased prevalence of chronic diseases such as depression, obesity, diabetes, cardiovascular diseases and cancer [19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26].
The contribution of circadian rhythm disruption to disease pathogenesis is likely to be multifactorial. In other words, when work, social and environmental pressures disrupt the entrainment of circadian rhythms by light and non-photic zeitgebers (food, physical activity), the out-of-sync circadian physiology may predispose to altered stress levels and lifestyle changes (fatty diet, alcohol consumption, smoking, self-medication, lack of exercise, short sleep). Together, this will alter multiple brain and body circuits that underpin physical and mental health, representing a multifactorial basis for any form of pathophysiology [27,28].
Scheduled daylight and mixed daylight/electric light exposure conditions may also optimise the effectiveness of certain medications/drugs and reduce side effects directly or indirectly via inducing robust circadian rhythmicity [29]. The latter would enable timed coordination between the kinetics of a drug’s actions and the host’s responsiveness. However, we are just at the beginning of understanding these points, and replication and further larger-scale studies are warranted.
To some individuals, the idea that the light environment has the potential to significantly influence human health outcomes may seem exaggerated. However, this is long established in the commercial application of light (photoperiod, intensity, and spectral balance of wavelengths) in growing plants, flowers, fruits, poultry and livestock [30], though with emerging negative consequences from climate change [31]. Humans may similarly require exposure to specific light patterns for optimal physiological and psychological well-being.
Given the primordial role of light for the circadian system and other non-visual functions and the regulatory role of the circadian system on a multitude of physiological processes, it is crucial to identify (and address) current gaps in daylight research. In this report, we use the term “daylight” to comprise mixed daylight/electric light conditions, which is thus implicitly included throughout the manuscript if not stated otherwise. With respect to health, daylight research should also include the development of coping strategies against chronically altered (day-) light conditions, such as experienced in atypical work schedules, including shift work. Daylight research may also elucidate how adequate daylight exposure could maintain health, prevent disease, and reduce circadian disruption. The main goals of daylight research in a human context are thus: (1) To elucidate the effectiveness of daylight to promote general health, quality of life and foster coping strategies. (2) To develop standardised tools to accurately and continuously measure daylight (and electric light) as well as methodologies to determine its effects on visual, psychological, and somatic functions. (3) To translate existing knowledge about human responses to daylight into appropriate designs for daylight-enhanced buildings and urban settings at the planning stage. This paper addresses these three goals by identifying main knowledge gaps, defining related, unanswered questions, and suggesting possible ways to fill these gaps and develop potential coping strategies.
To identify gaps of knowledge in daylight and to address them is complex because the implications of daylight go far beyond isolated and specialised research areas; they impact broadly on many disciplines such as science of the built environment (physics, engineering, architecture), environmental sciences, medicine, psychology, economics, occupational and social sciences. The unanswered questions related to daylight research are those which have been explored under electric light conditions (mostly in the laboratory) but have not yet been answered for daylight research (which is mainly related to daylight inside buildings). There are questions that are specific to daylight and cannot be (easily) mimicked by electric light (e.g., seasonal changes in day length or twilight conditions). Also, some questions and problems have to date not been addressed either with daylight or electric light. Our report is designed to highlight some of these questions and put daylight into the focus of research. Obviously, quantities and qualities can vary considerably between daylight and electric light, especially inside buildings, and to date, it is unknown whether daylight is superior to electric lighting with respect to the impact on humans (and if so why).
This paper was developed from an interdisciplinary workshop involving members of the Daylight Academy (DLA; www.daylight.academy) in November 2017 in Lausanne (Switzerland). The “missing links” in defining the biological value of daylight (and darkness at night) for human beings were discussed with respect to vast regional differences in daylight availability across the year from the equator to high latitudes. A second meeting took place in June 2018 in Berlin (Germany) to discuss the role of daylight and specify gaps and barriers to the advancement in knowledge. Therefore, the workshop outcome is obviously primarily focused on daylight. In addition, specific action points were named to achieve the goals. These goals include evidence-based knowledge-transfer for enabling practitioners to design living and working spaces and conditions in a regional-adapted manner for the benefit of the inhabitants.
We do not systematically review the literature on the current state of daylight research or carry out a meta-analysis. Since this paper is an enlarged output of two workshops on daylight and its impact on humans, it differs in its formal structure from an experimental study.
This report is envisaged as a framework for stimulating interdisciplinary research and collaborative approaches to the neglected area of daylight research in humans. During the DLA workshops, we came up with a list of gaps, which were retrospectively assigned to three main groups (Figure 1): (I) Lack of knowledge regarding the quantity and quality of daylight needed for “optimal” physiological and psychological functioning (optimal = relating to best possible quality of life for an individual); (II) Lack of consensus on practical measurement and assessment methods and tools for real (day-) light exposure across multiple time scales, especially under long time recording periods (e.g., circadian, seasonal contexts); and (III) insufficient integration and exchange of knowledge bases related to daylight from different disciplines. This grouping is not based on any a priori concept, but rather grew out of the two workshops and meetings within the DLA. A summary of the many physical differences between the properties of daylight and electric light have been recently published by members of the DLA [30,32].

2. Gaps Group I: Lack of Knowledge Regarding Quantity and Quality of Daylight Needed for “Optimal” Physiological and Psychological Functioning

We live in a global and increasingly digitised 24/7 society. Among the key gaps in daylight research is the lack of knowledge about timed combinations of quantity and quality of daylight (including mixed day/electric light conditions) required by a given individual each day. This information is necessary to cope with and mitigate challenges arising from electrically lit life and work conditions. The term “optimal” hereby refers not to an absolute value, but rather to the best possible quality of life for an individual under his or her environmental conditions. Lighting standards for the visual system have been developed; here, we focus primarily on the non-visual physiological and psychological functions. This first group of gaps in daylight research contains a variety of different subgroups: (Section 2.1) vision and visual comfort; (Section 2.2) physiology and behaviour; (Section 2.3) circadian entrainment; (Section 2.4) “optimal” (day-) light dose; (Section 2.5) light sensitivity; (Section 2.6) therapeutic aspects; (Section 2.7) risks; (Section 2.8) inter-individual differences; (Section 2.9) work conditions; and (Section 2.10) environmental factors.

2.1. Open Questions for the Effects of Daylight on Vision and Visual Comfort

The effects of electric light on vision (e.g., [33,34]), or light conditions for vision at workplaces (e.g., [35]) were extensively explored many decades ago, but it is still not clear whether there are daylight-specific influences on vision, visual capabilities, visual processing and comfort, which may arise from the physical differences between daylight and electric light [32]. More specifically:
  • Daylight impacts on vision and visual comfort with respect to colour perception [36], colour preferences (e.g., [37]), as well as homogeneity, dynamics, glare and flicker from mixed daylight/electric lighting conditions (e.g., [38]).
  • There is considerable literature on visual comfort and glare evaluations with daylight inside buildings, e.g., [39], but very little is known about mixed/synergistic effects of high visual comfort together with non-visual functions such as alertness, mood [40,41,42] and indoor temperature perception [43,44]. These mixed/synergistic effects are missing not only for daylight but also for mixed electric/daylight conditions.

2.2. How Does Daylight Impact on Physiology and Behaviour Beyond Vision?

There is now a large body of evidence from human studies on the non-visual physiological and psychological effects of electric light under controlled laboratory conditions (e.g., [45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62]). An increasing number of studies have also investigated the non-visual effects of daylight alone or with mixed daylight/electric light (e.g., [43,63,64,65,66,67,68]), which better reflects the situation at most work and residential places. A big difference with daylight is, of course, that there is also usually a view, i.e., being outdoors or looking out of the window into green (or built) spaces, to the sky, which per se is positively rated [69,70,71]. Some examples of knowledge gaps in daylight research for five non-visual functions are shown in Table 1.

2.3. Gaps of Knowledge on Circadian Entrainment

Most chronobiological research in humans over the past 50 years has mainly dealt with (controlled) electric light in the laboratory, “bunkers” or caves [90,91,92,93,94,95,96,97]. The biggest issue here is certainly removal from any natural photic context and reduced external stimuli. These studies were necessary to understand what we currently know, but now it is time to move to more naturalistic approaches, including studying daylight, and/or mixed electric light/daylight during the daytime [98]. Surprisingly, there are only a few studies looking at circadian entrainment in humans under natural conditions [78,79], and most of them have been short-term studies. There is evidence from a few isolated populations, still living without electric lighting, that the circadian phase of entrainment relative to the external light–dark cycle is indeed different from that in regions supplied with electric light sources [99]. There is a considerable gap of knowledge about the impact of natural light under real-life conditions, as summarised in Table 2.

2.4. What Is the “Optimal” Dose of Daylight with Respect to Intensity, Spectrum and Timing?

From laboratory studies with electric lighting interventions during the daytime, mixed dose-response effects have been reported for intensity, duration and spectral composition: Some studies found effects of daytime/evening electric light exposure on subjective and/or objective variables [52,86,89,115], while others did not [82,116]. Few studies have been conducted under daylight conditions only or mixed daylight/electric light conditions, and most of these are applied or semi-natural studies [40,65,78]. The general unanswered questions are related to daylight thresholds, duration/intensity and spectral responses, as well as the role of the dynamics of daylight. To date, these complex daylight properties have not been evaluated in a single model. There is evidence of a complex interaction between different subtypes of ipRGC (modulated by rod and cone input), which depend on the daylight spectrum and irradiance [117]. Finally, the main question also remains—why should daylight be different from findings with electric light? The outstanding questions are summarised in Table 3.

2.5. How Does Daylight Influence Light Sensitivity Functions?

There are a few field and laboratory studies showing indirectly that sensitivity to light increases when there is a lack of daylight or predominantly low indoor light exposure [126,127,128,129,130], but there is no consensus how to measure light sensitivity nor about the length of daylight nor the mixed electric light/daylight exposure necessary to counteract such increased sensitivity. This is of great importance since chronic exposure to low daylight levels during daytime may increase vulnerability to light in the evening/night due to increased sensitivity, as shown under electric light [80,131,132].
Another aspect of daylight and retinal functions is the question of underlying mechanisms and counterstrategies for the recent high increase of myopia cases in children (e.g., South East Asia). Growing evidence suggests that chronic low daylight exposure is one of the possible triggering factors; for a recent review, see [133].

2.6. Is Daylight Exposure Effective as a ‘Treatment’?

There is clear evidence for the therapeutic use of controlled bright and/or dynamic light exposure (with or without daylight) in hospitals and nursing homes [134,135,136,137,138], as well as light therapy lamps for a variety of psychiatric (e.g., seasonal and non-seasonal affective disorder, bipolar depression [123,124,125], ADHD [139], borderline personality disorder [140]), neurologic (Parkinson’s disease [141,142]), and medical disorders to improve sleep (e.g., [143]) or reduce fatigue [144]. Only a few studies have explored scheduled daylight exposure for therapeutic use to obtain information about duration and timing as related to improvement [145,146,147,148].
The question is why not use daylight instead of light therapy lamps since daylight is freely available and without energy costs, and there are other beneficial effects with daylight exposure such as a view, contact with nature, other people? There is, at present, insufficient evidence to replace light therapy lamps with timed daylight as treatment for the above indications. It would be important—and practical—to know if daylight is equally effective or even superior to electric light as therapy. One study in seasonal affective disorder suggested outdoor light is equivalent in response [145]. We need to know if regular daylight exposure or mixed electric light/daylight (and darkness at night) enhances recovery/promotes health in hospitalised patients more efficiently (e.g., post-surgery, intensive care [149,150,151]), and in healthcare institutions (e.g., psychiatric wards, nursing homes [152,153]) than with electric light alone. The question arises as to which daylight interventions are needed for which groups (e.g., patients with impaired vision/visual blindness, circadian rhythm sleep–wake disorders, depression and other psychiatric/neurologic diagnoses), adolescents or older people, shift workers, men and women?

2.7. Are There Risks of Daylight Exposures (e.g., Systemic Diseases, Dermatology, Ophthalmology)?

Much is known about the direct UV risk from sunlight for skin and eyes in humans, and current regulations are regularly evaluated [154]. National programs to reduce skin cancer have been launched worldwide. There is, however, only very little evidence as to whether having full sun protection all the time is beneficial for other physiological functions. A recent study (performed in South Asia) showed that wearing a sun hat and sunglasses while being outside still allows a good portion of daylight to be transmitted via the eyes [155], but this finding also needs to be shown for places further from the equator.
In addition: How can light-sensitive populations be protected from too much (day-) light (e.g., porphyria patients, patients taking light-sensitising medications, photophobic patients, patients with migraine, certain patients with retinal damage, patients with skin diseases), whilst still retaining enough exposure to ensure proper/adequate functional outcomes (e.g., vitamin D synthesis, sleep quality, waking functions, circadian entrainment and general health)?

2.8. Gaps of Knowledge Related to Individual Differences

We know of some factors that can explain inter-individual differences in response to light exposure, including age, chronotype, gender, genotype and many more. However, there is much more unknown, and it is not clear whether these inter-individual differences are manifested under daylight conditions. Here (Table 4), we list some of the known inter-individual “trait” differences mainly in response to electric light, which also need to be considered for daylight research (and where the mechanisms remain to be elucidated). Some of these inter-individual differences are only known for non-human species and have not yet been explored in humans. Beyond these inter-individual traits, it might also be important to know what the consequences of such differences are—for example, sex differences in the perception of light of a special quality might lead to implications on the societal aspect of gender for light applications.

2.9. Gaps of Knowledge in Daylight Research for Work Conditions

Many field studies have tested different electric light scenarios at work, but from most of the studies, it is not clear if and how daylight exposure in addition to electric light was considered for the analysis; therefore, many questions remain. In addition, many aspects related to work conditions overlap with general aspects, as mentioned in Section 2.1, Section 2.2, Section 2.3, Section 2.4, Section 2.5, Section 2.7 and Section 2.8. Table 5 summarises some specific work and daylight associated questions:

2.10. Gaps in Knowledge of Daylight Research Related to Environmental Factors

In our built environment, many factors influence the quantity and quality of daylight exposure. These mostly physical factors are per se well known, but the interactions with and consequences on humans of latitude, time zones, micro- and macroclimate, architecture and urban settings are only poorly understood (Table 6). Therefore both the spectrum and intensity of the daylight influx which reaches indoor rooms matters, and is, of course, dependent on architectural properties, building orientation, windows size, glazing, and geographical latitude. And lastly, how relevant is the concept of the “biophilia” hypothesis, which describes that humans tend to find connections with nature and living organisms, in this context (e.g., [206])?

3. Gaps Group II: Lack of Consensus on Practical Measurement and Assessment Methods and Tools for Monitoring Real (Day-) Light Exposure Across Multiple Time Scales

This crucial group of gaps describes the lack of appropriate methods in basic and applied (day)-light research. Many of these gaps apply generally to lighting research. The most important of these gaps are summarised in Table 7.

4. Gaps Group III: Insufficient Integration and Exchange of Daylight Knowledge Bases from Different Disciplines

The impact of daylight depends on many “human” factors but is also determined by environmental and technical aspects and interacts with a variety of drivers and constraints at different levels and dimensions (see Figure 2). As such, daylight research cannot be comprehensively done exclusively within a single research community; it must involve many disciplines. A fundamental key gap is related to insufficient transdisciplinary approaches, with greater participation of a broader range of experts from different disciplines such as physicists (simulation modelling), ecologists, landscape architects (ecological laws), engineers (glazing, building principles), biomedical disciplines (health), biologists and psychologists (physiology and behaviour), artists, media/journalists, social scientists (social-ecology connectedness), architects and economists, to be able to find solutions for the connectedness between daylight/nature/living space/humans.
Access to natural daylight is nowadays unfortunately not considered as an explicit aim by leading institutions worldwide, such as, for example, the WHO, where the “Healthy Cities” concept, although it mentions access to green spaces, does not explicitly require access to daylight [226]. Accessibility to daylight should be part of global discussions about sustainable living, health and well-being, and should be included in the UN 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development and the WHO’s “health for all” policy [227].

5. What Is Needed to Fill These Gaps and Achieve the Goals in Daylight Research?

The need to develop new tools, methods and approaches seems crucial, and here we highlight some possibilities. Table 8 and Table 9 summarise the short- and long-term objectives in daylight research, respectively.

6. Summary

Daylight research is much more complex and less controllable in the field than pure electric light research in the laboratory, and thus scientific methods and tools need to be developed to make daylight research accessible and studies comparable. Even more important is agreeing on methodology (SOPs) and running collaborative projects to provide large data sets.

7. Highlights

  • Recognising the importance of daylight for human health and well-being.
  • Daylight research needs to define requirements for optimal physiological and psychological functioning.
  • New techniques are required to monitor and assess (day-) light exposure in the field.
  • Interdisciplinary exchange of daylight knowledge is the key to integrating findings into practice, whether architectural or medical.

Author Contributions

M.M., A.W.-J. and D.J.S. drafted the manuscript and all co-authors (S.A.B., T.K., K.M., O.S., C.V., K.P.W.J., K.W.) commented and edited the text. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The workshops were supported by the Daylight Academy (https://daylight.academy/).

Acknowledgments

We thank Christian Cajochen, Manuel Spitschan and Daniel Hulliger for their helpful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship, and/or publication of this article. None of the authors had received monetary payments or gifts other than reimbursement of costs related to workshops’ participation. None of the authors is an employee of the Daylight Academy or the Velux Stiftung Switzerland, or is in any kind related to the VELUX® company. The Daylight Academy is a spin-off of the Velux Stiftung Switzerland. The Velux Stiftung Switzerland is a registered non-profit organization (see https://veluxstiftung.ch/) and its members had at no time influence on the content of this manuscript. M.M., A.W.-J, S.A.B., K.M., K.W. have otherwise no conflict of interest to declare; T.K. is founder of SynOpus, a business involved in consulting to companies with respect to work design and lighting; O.S. is listed as an inventor on the following patents: US8646939B2—Display system having circadian effect on humans. DE102010047207B4—Projection system and method for projecting image content. US8994292B2—Adaptive lighting system. WO2006013041A1—Projection device and filter therefor. WO2016092112A1—Method for the selective adjustment of a desired brightness and/or color of a specific spatial area, and data processing device therefor. O.S. has had the following commercial interests related to lighting: Investigator-initiated research grants from Derungs, Audi, VW, Porsche, Festo, ZDF and Toshiba. Speaker fees for invited seminars from Firalux and Selux; C.V., during the conduct of the study, received research support from the NIH, was a honorary (unpaid) scientific advisory board member of Circadian Light Therapy Inc., and served as a paid consultant to the US Department of Energy outside the submitted work; K.P.W.J., during the conduct of the study, was a scientific advisory board member of and received personal fees from Torvec and received personal fees from Circadian Therapeutics, Inc. and from Kellogg Company; K.P.W.J. received research support from the NIH, the Office of Naval Research, the PAC-12 conference, and Somalogic, Inc. outside the submitted work; D.J.S. is co-inventor on two light patents (EP 1614441A1 and EP3055022A).

References

  1. Zehring, W.; Wheeler, D.; Reddy, P.; Konopka, R.; Kyriacou, C.P.; Rosbash, M.; Hall, J. P-element transformation with period locus DNA restores rhythmicity to mutant, arrhythmic Drosophila melanogaster. Cell 1984, 39, 369–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bargiello, T.; Jackson, F.; Young, M. Restoration of circadian behavioural rhythms by gene transfer in Drosophila. Nature 1984, 312, 752–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Vitaterna, M.; King, D.; Chang, A.-M.; Kornhauser, J.; Lowrey, P.; McDonald, J.; Dove, W.; Pinto, L.; Turek, F.; Takahashi, J. Mutagenesis and mapping of a mouse gene, clock, essential for circadian behavior. Science 1994, 264, 719–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Shichida, Y.; Matsuyama, T. Evolution of opsins and phototransduction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 2881–2895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Nilsson, D. Eye evolution and its functional basis. Vis. Neurosci. 2013, 30, 5–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Bellingham, J.; Foster, R.G. Opsins and mammalian photoentrainment. Cell Tissue Res. 2002, 309, 57–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Koyanagi, M.; Kubokawa, K.; Tsukamoto, H.; Shichida, Y.; Terakita, A. Cephalochordate melanopsin: Evolutionary linkage between invertebrate visual cells and vertebrate photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Curr. Biol. 2005, 15, 1065–1069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Peirson, S.; Halford, S.; Foster, R. The evolution of irradiance detection: Melanopsin and the non-visual opsins. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 2849–2865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Provencio, I.; Jiang, G.; De Grip, W.J.; Hayes, W.P.; Rollag, M.D. Melanopsin: An opsin in melanophores, brain, and eye. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1998, 95, 340–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Hattar, S.; Liao, H.; Takao, M.; Berson, D.; Yau, K. Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: Architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity. Science 2002, 295, 1065–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Berson, D.M.; Dunn, F.A.; Takao, M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science 2002, 295, 1070–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Ruby, N.; Brennan, T.; Xie, X.; Cao, V.; Franken, P.; Heller, H.; O’Hara, B. Role of melanopsin in circadian responses to light. Science 2002, 298, 2211–2213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Provencio, I.; Rodriguez, I.R.; Jiang, G.; Hayes, W.P.; Moreira, E.F.; Rollag, M.D. A novel human opsin in the inner retina. J. Neurosci. 2000, 20, 600–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Davies, T.; Smyth, T. Why artificial light at night should be a focus for global change research in the 21st century. Glob. Chang. Biol. 2018, 24, 872–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Aschoff, J.; Wever, J. The Circadian System of Man. In Biological Rhythms; Aschoff, J., Ed.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1981; pp. 311–331. [Google Scholar]
  16. Pittendrigh, C.S. Circadian Systems: Entrainment. In Biological Rhythms; Aschoff, J., Ed.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1981; pp. 95–124. [Google Scholar]
  17. Roenneberg, T.; Daan, S.; Merrow, M. The art of entrainment. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2003, 18, 183–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Vetter, C. Circadian disruption: What do we actually mean? Eur. J. Neurosci. 2018, 51, 531–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Abbott, S.; Malkani, R.; Zee, P. Circadian disruption and human health: A bidirectional relationship. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2018, 51, 567–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Zimmet, P.; Alberti, K.; Stern, N.; Bilu, C.; El-Osta, A.; Einat, H.; Kronfeld-Schor, N. The circadian syndrome: Is the metabolic syndrome and much more! J. Intern. Med. 2019, 286, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Logan, R.; McClung, C. Rhythms of life: Circadian disruption and brain disorders across the lifespan. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2019, 20, 49–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Masri, S.; Sassone-Corsi, P. The emerging link between cancer, metabolism, and circadian rhythms. Nat. Med. 2018, 24, 1795–1803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Kervezee, L.; Kosmadopoulos, A.; Boivin, D. Metabolic and cardiovascular consequences of shift work: The role of circadian disruption and sleep disturbances. Eur. J. Neurosci 2018, 51, 396–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. McHill, A.; Wright, K.J. Role of sleep and circadian disruption on energy expenditure and in metabolic predisposition to human obesity and metabolic disease. Obes Rev. 2017, 18, 15–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Eckel, R.H.; Depner, C.; Perreault, L.; Markwald, R.; Smith, M.; McHill, A.; Higgins, J.; Melanson, E.; Wright, K.J. Morning circadian misalignment during short sleep duration impacts insulin sensitivity. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 3004–3010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Depner, C.; Melanson, E.; Eckel, R.; Snell-Bergeon, J.; Perreault, L.; Bergman, B.; Higgins, J.; Guerin, M.; Stothard, E.; Morton, S.; et al. Ad libitum weekend recovery sleep fails to prevent metabolic dysregulation during a repeating pattern of insufficient sleep and weekend recovery sleep. Curr. Biol. 2019, 29, 957–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Foster, R.G.; Wulff, K. The rhythm of rest and excess. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 6, 407–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Rijo-Ferreira, F.; Takahashi, J. Genomics of circadian rhythms in health and disease. Genome Med. 2019, 11, 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Cederroth, C.; Albrecht, U.; Bass, J.; Brown, S.; Dyhrfjeld-Johnsen, J.; Gachon, F.; Green, C.; Hastings, M.; Helfrich-Forster, C.; Hogenesch, J.; et al. Medicine in the fourth dimension. Cell Metab. 2019, 30, 238–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Solt, J. Changing perspectives on daylight: Science, technology and culture. In Sponsored Supplement to Science/AAS; Science/AAAS Custom Publishing Office: Washington, DC, USA, 2017; pp. 1–45. [Google Scholar]
  31. Walker, W.; Melendez-Fernandez, O.; Nelson, R.; Reiter, R. Global climate change and invariable photoperiods: A mismatch that jeopardizes animal fitness. Ecol. Evol. 2019, 9, 10044–10054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Knoop, M.; Stefani, O.; Bueno, B.; Matusiak, B.; Hobday, R.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Martiny, K.; Kantermann, T.; Aarts, M.; Zemmouri, N.; et al. Daylight: What makes the difference? Lighting Res. Technol. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Bartley, S.; Ball, R. Effects of intermittent photic input on brightness, hue, saturation, visual acuity, and stereopsis. Am. J. Optom. Arch. Am. Acad. Optom. 1969, 46, 315–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Guth, S.; Eastman, A. Brightness difference in seeing. Am. J. Optom. Arch. Am. Acad. Optom. 1954, 31, 567–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Nelson, J. Ideal seeing conditions; the study of the human visual system as a basis for prescribing lighting. Br. Ind. Med. 1945, 2, 224–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Hunt, R. The effects of daylight and tungsten light-adaptation on color perception. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 1950, 40, 362–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Luckiesh, M. A note on color preference. Am. J. Psychol. 1916, 27, 251–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Boyce, P. Human Factors in Lighting, 2nd ed.; CRC Press: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hopkinson, R. Glare from daylighting in buildings. Appl. Ergonom. 1972, 3, 206–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Borisuit, A.; Linhart, F.; Scartezzini, J.-L.; Münch, M. Effects of realistic office day- and electric lighting conditions on visual comfort, alertness and mood. Lighting Res. Technol. 2014, 47, 192–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Pierson, C.; Wienold, J.; Bodart, M. Review of factors influencing discomfort glare perception from daylight. Leukos 2018, 14, 111–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Veitch, J.; Newsham, G.; Boyce, P.; Jones, C. Lighting appraisal, well-being, and performance in open-plan offices: A linked mechansisms approach. Lighting Res. Technol. 2008, 40, 133–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Chinazzo, G.; Wienold, J.; Andersen, M. Daylight affects human thermal perception. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 13690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Te Kulve, M.; Schlangen, L.; van Marken Lichtenbelt, W. Interactions between the perception of light and temperature. Indoor Air 2018, 28, 881–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Cajochen, C.; Münch, M.; Kobialka, S.; Kräuchi, K.; Steiner, R.; Oelhafen, P.; Orgül, S.; Wirz-Justice, A. High sensitivity of human melatonin, alertness, thermoregulation and heart rate to short wavelength light. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2005, 90, 1311–1316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Lockley, S.W.; Evans, E.E.; Scheer, F.A.J.L.; Brainard, G.C.; Czeisler, C.A.; Aeschbach, D. Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep 2006, 29, 161–168. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  47. Revell, V.; Skene, D. Light-induced melatonin suppression in humans with polychromatic and monochromatic light. Chronobiol. Int. 2007, 24, 1125–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Rahman, S.; Flynn-Evans, E.; Aeschbach, D.; Brainard, G.; Czeisler, C.; Lockley, S. Diurnal spectral sensitivity of the acute alerting effects of light. Sleep 2014, 37, 271–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Souman, J.; Tinga, A.; te Pas, S.; van Ee, R.; Vlaskamp, B. Acute alerting effects of light: A systematic literature review. Behav. Brain Res. 2018, 337, 228–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Cajochen, C.; Jud, C.; Münch, M.; Kobialka, S.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Albrecht, U. Evening exposure to blue light stimulates the expression of the clock gene PER2 in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006, 23, 1082–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Santhi, N.; Groeger, J.; Archer, S.; Gimenez, M.; Schlangen, L.; Dijk, D. Morning sleep inertia in alertness and performance: Effect of cognitive domain and white light conditions. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e79688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Prayag, A.; Jost, S.; Avouac, P.; Dumortier, D.; Gronfier, C. Dynamics of non-visual responses in humans: As fast as lightning? Front. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Vandewalle, G.; Maquet, P.; Dijk, D. Light as a modulator of cognitive brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 429–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Münch, M.; Scheuermaier, K.; Zhang, R.; Dunne, S.; Guzik, A.; Silva, E.J.; Ronda, J.; Duffy, J. Effects on subjective and objective alertness and sleep in response to evening light exposure in older subjects. Behav. Brain Res. 2011, 224, 272–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Chellappa, S.L.; Steiner, R.; Blattner, P.; Oelhafen, P.; Götz, T.; Cajochen, C. Non-visual effects of light on melatonin, alertness and cognitive performance: Can blue-enriched light keep us alert? PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e16429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Rüger, M.; Gordijn, M.C.M.; Beersma, D.G.M.; de Vries, B.; Daan, S. Time-of-day-dependent effects of bright light exposure on human psychophysiology: Comparison of daytime and nighttime exposure. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2006, 290, R1413–R1420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Jung, C.M.; Khalsa, S.B.S.; Scheer, F.A.J.L.; Cajochen, C.; Lockley, S.W.; Czeisler, C.A.; Wright, K. Acute effects of bright light exposure on cortisol levels. J. Biol. Rhythms 2010, 25, 208–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Münch, M.; Kobialka, S.; Steiner, R.; Oelhafen, P.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Cajochen, C. Wavelength-dependent effects of evening light exposure on sleep architecture and sleep EEG power density in men. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2006, 290, R1421–R1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Brainard, G.; Hanifin, J.; Greeson, J.; Byrne, B.; Glickman, G.; Gerner, E.; Rollag, M. Action spectrum for melatonin regulation in humans: Evidence for a novel circadian photoreceptor. J. Neurosci. 2001, 21, 6405–6412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Thapan, K.; Arendt, J.; Skene, D.J. An action spectrum for melatonin suppression: Evidence for a novel non-rod, non-cone photoreceptor system in humans. J. Physiol. London 2001, 535, 261–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Gooley, J.; Rajaratnam, S.; Brainard, G.; Kronauer, R.; Czeisler, C.; Lockley, S. Spectral responses of the human circadian system depend on the irradiance and duration of exposure to light. Sci. Transl. Med. 2010, 2, 31ra33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Zaidi, F.; Hull, J.; Peirson, S.; Wulff, K.; Aeschbach, D.; Gooley, J.; Brainard, G.; Gregory-Evans, K.; Rizzo Iii, J.; Czeisler, C.; et al. Short-wavelength light sensitivity of circadian, pupillary, and visual awareness in humans lacking an outer retina. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 2122–2128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Boubekri, M.; Cheung, I.N.; Reid, K.; Wang, C.; Zee, P. Impact of windows and daylight exposure on overall health and sleep quality of office workers: A case-control pilot study. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2014, 10, 603–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Figueiro, M.; Rea, M. Office lighting and personal light exposures in two seasons: Impact on sleep and mood. Lighting Res. Technol. 2016, 48, 352–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Viola, A.U.; James, L.; Schlangen, L.; Dijk, D. Blue-enriched white light in the workplace improves self-reported alertness, performance and sleep quality. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2008, 34, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Vetter, C.; Juda, M.; Lang, D.; Wojtysiak, A.; Roenneberg, T. Blue-enriched office light competes with natural light as a zeitgeber. Scand J. Work Environ. Health 2011, 34, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Maierova, L.; Borisuit, A.; Scartezzini, J.-L.; Jaeggi, S.; Schmidt, C.; Munch, M. Diurnal variations of hormonal secretion, alertness and cognition in extreme chronotypes under different lighting conditions. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Begeman, S.; Beld, G.; Tenner, A. Daylight, artificial light and people in an office environment, overview of visual and biological responses. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 1997, 20, 231–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Beute, F.; de Kort, Y. The natural context of wellbeing: Ecological momentary assessment of the influence of nature and daylight on affect and stress for individuals with depression levels varying from none to clinical. Health Place 2018, 49, 7–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Veitch, J.; Bisegna, F.; Hubalek, S.; Knoop, M.; Koga, Y.; Noguchi, H.; Schierz, C.; Thorns, P.; Vries, A. Research Roadmap for Healthful Interior Lighting Applications; CIE: Vienna, Austria, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  71. Heschong, L. Daylighting and human performance. ASHRAE J. 2002, 44, 65–67. [Google Scholar]
  72. Cheung, I.; Zee, P.; Shalman, D.; Malkani, R.; Kang, J.; Reid, K. Morning and evening blue-enriched light exposure alters metabolic function in normal weight adults. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0155601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Melanson, E.; Ritchie, H.; Dear, T.; Catenacci, V.; Shea, K.; Connick, E.; Moehlman, T.; Stothard, E.; Higgins, J.; McHill, A.; et al. Daytime bright light exposure, metabolism, and individual differences in wake and sleep energy expenditure during circadian entrainment and misalignment. Neurobiol. Sleep Circadian Rhythm. 2018, 4, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Danilenko, K.; Mustafina, S.; Pechenkina, E. Bright light for weight loss: Results of a controlled crossover trial. Obes Facts 2013, 6, 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Wams, E.; Woelders, T.; Marring, I.; van Rosmalen, L.; Beersma, D.; Gordijn, M.; Hut, R. Linking light exposure and subsequent sleep: A field polysomnography study in humans. Sleep 2017, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Cajochen, C.; Freyburger, M.; Basishvili, T.; Garbazza, C.; Rudzik, F.; Renz, C.; Kobayashi, K.; Shirakawa, Y.; Stefani, O.; Weibel, J. Effect of daylight LED on visual comfort, melatonin, mood, waking performance and sleep. Lighting Res. Technol. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Cajochen, C.; Reichert, C.; Maire, M.; Schlangen, L.; Schmidt, C.; Viola, A.; Gabel, V. Evidence that homeostatic sleep regulation depends on ambient lighting conditions during wakefulness. Clocks Sleep 2019, 1, 517–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Wright, K., Jr.; McHill, A.; Birks, B.; Griffin, B.; Rusterholz, T.; Chinoy, E. Entrainment of the human circadian clock to the natural light-dark cycle. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 1554–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  79. Stothard, E.; McHill, A.; Depner, C.; Birks, B.; Moehlman, T.; Ritchie, H.; Guzzetti, J.; Chinoy, E.; LeBourgeois, M.; Axelsson, J.; et al. Circadian entrainment to the natural light-dark cycle across seasons and the weekend. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, 508–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Münch, M.; Nowozin, C.; Regente, J.; Bes, F.; De Zeeuw, J.; Hadel, S.; Wahnschaffe, A.; Kunz, D. Blue-enriched morning light as a countermeasure to light at the wrong time: Effects on cognition, sleepiness, sleep, and circadian phase. Neuropsychobiology 2016, 74, 207–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Cajochen, C.; Zeitzer, J.M.; Czeisler, C.A.; Dijk, D. Dose-response relationship for light intensity and alertness and its ocular and EEG correlates. Sleep Res. Onl. 1999, 2, 517. [Google Scholar]
  82. Smolders, K.; Peeters, S.; Vogels, I.; de Kort, Y. Investigation of dose-response relationships for effects of white light exposure on correlates of alertness and executive control during regular daytime working hours. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2018, 33, 649–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Sahin, L.; Wood, B.; Plitnick, B.; Figueiro, M. Daytime light exposure: Effects on biomarkers, measures of alertness, and performance. Behav. Brain Res. 2014, 274, 176–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Allen, A.; Hazelhoff, E.; Martial, F.; Cajochen, C.; Lucas, R. Exploiting metamerism to regulate the impact of a visual display on alertness and melatonin suppression independent of visual appearance. Sleep 2018, 41, zsy100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Rahman, S.A.; St Hilaire, M.A.; Lockley, S. The effects of spectral tuning of evening ambient light on melatonin suppression, alertness and sleep. Physiol. Behav. 2017, 177, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. De Zeeuw, J.; Papakonstantinou, A.; Nowozin, C.; Stotz, S.; Zaleska, M.; Hädel, S.; Bes, F.; Münch, M.; Kunz, D. Living in biological darkness: Objective sleepiness and the pupillary light responses are affected by different metameric lighting conditions during daytime. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2019, 34, 410–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Youngstedt, S.; Elliott, J.; Kripke, D. Human circadian phase-response curves for exercise. J. Physiol. London 2019, 597, 2253–2268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  88. Barger, L.; Wright, K.J.; Hughes, R.; Czeisler, C. Daily exercise facilitates phase delays of circadian melatonin rhythm in very dim light. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2004, 286, R1077–R1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  89. Knaier, R.; Meister, S.; Aeschbacher, T.; Gemperle, D.; Rossmeissl, A.; Cajochen, C.; Schmidt-Trucksass, A. Dose-response relationship between light exposure and cycling performance. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2016, 26, 794–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Aschoff, J.; Wever, R. Spontanperiodik des Menschen bei Auschluss aller Zeitgeber. Die Naturwissenschaften 1962, 49, 337–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Gronfier, C.; Wright, K.P., Jr.; Kronauer, R.E.; Czeisler, C.A. Entrainment of the human circadian pacemaker to longer-than-24-h days. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 9081–9086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Wright, K.P., Jr.; Hughes, R.J.; Kronauer, R.E.; Dijk, D.J.; Czeisler, C.A. Intrinsic near-24-h pacemaker period determines limits of circadian entrainment to a weak synchronizer in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 14027–14032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Boivin, D.B.; Duffy, J.F.; Kronauer, R.E.; Czeisler, C.A. Dose-response relationships for resetting of human circadian clock by light. Nature 1996, 379, 540–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Scheer, F.A.; Wright, K.P., Jr.; Kronauer, R.E.; Czeisler, C.A. Plasticity of the intrinsic period of the human circadian timing system. PLoS ONE 2007, 2, e721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Eastman, C.; Miescke, K. Entrainment of circadian rhythms with 26-h bright light and sleep-wake schedules. Am J. Physiol. 1990, 259, R1189–R1197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Wever, R.A. The Circadian System of Man: Results of Experiments under Temporal Isolation; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  97. Czeisler, C.A.; Richardson, G.S.; Zimmerman, J.Z.; Moore-Ede, M.C.; Weitzman, E.D. Entrainment of human circadian rhythms by light-dark cycles: A reassessment. Photochem. Photobiol. 1981, 34, 239–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Emens, J.S.; Yuhas, K.; Rough, J.; Kochar, N.; Peters, D.; Lewy, A. Phase angle of entrainment in morning- and evening-types under naturalistic conditions. Chronobiol. Int. 2009, 26, 474–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  99. Moreno, C.; Vasconcelos, S.; Marqueze, E.; Lowden, A.; Middleton, B.; Fischer, F.; Louzada, F.; Skene, D. Sleep patterns in Amazon rubber tappers with and without electric light at home. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 14074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  100. Woelders, T.; Wams, E.J.; Gordijn, M.; Beersma, D.; Hut, R. Integration of color and intensity increases time signal stability for the human circadian system when sunlight is obscured by clouds. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 15214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Roenneberg, T.; Foster, R.G. Twilight times: Light and the circadian system. Photochem. Photobiol. 1997, 66, 549–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Danilenko, K.V.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Kräuchi, K.; Weber, J.M.; Terman, M. The human circadian pacemaker can see by the dawn`s early light. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2000, 15, 437–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Walmsley, L.; Hanna, L.; Mouland, J.; Martial, F.; West, A.; Smedley, A.; Bechtold, D.; Webb, A.; Lucas, R.; Brown, T. Colour as a signal for entraining the mammalian circadian clock. PLoS Biol. 2015, 13, e1002127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Mouland, J.W.; Martial, F.; Watson, A.; Lucas, R.; Brown, T. Cones support alignment to an inconsistent world by suppressing mouse circadian responses to the blue colors associated with twilight. Curr. Biol. 2019, 29, 4260–4267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Brainard, G.; Rollag, M.; Hanifin, J.; van den Beld, G.; Sanford, B. The effect of polarized versus nonpolarized light on melatonin regulation in humans. Photochem. Photobiol. 2000, 71, 766–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Dacey, D.; Liao, H.; Peterson, B.; Robinson, F.; Smith, V.; Pokorny, J.; Yau, K.; Gamlin, P. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature 2005, 433, 749–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Rüger, M.; Gordijn, M.C.M.; Beersma, D.G.M.; DeVries, B.; Daan, S. Nasal versus temporal illumination of the human retina: Effects on core body temperature, melatonin, and circadian phase. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2005, 20, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  108. Visser, E.K.; Beersma, D.G.M.; Daan, S. Melatonin suppression by light in humans is maximal when the nasal part of the retina is illuminated. J. Biol. Rhythm. 1999, 14, 116–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  109. Glickman, G.; Hanifin, J.; Rollag, M.; Wang, J.; Cooper, H.; Brainard, G. Inferior retinal light exposure is more effective than superior retinal exposure in suppressing melatonin in humans. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2003, 18, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Cajochen, C.; Di Biase, R.; Imai, M. Interhemispheric EEG asymmetries during unilateral bright-light exposure and subsequent sleep in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2008, 294, R1053–R1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Wehr, T.A. Effect of seasonal changes in daylength on human neuroendocrine function. Horm. Res. 1998, 49, 118–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Rabstein, S.; Burek, K.; Lehnert, M.; Beine, A.; Vetter, C.; Harth, V.; Putzke, S.; Kantermann, T.; Walther, J.; Wang-Sattler, R.; et al. Differences in twenty-four-hour profiles of blue-light exposure between day and night shifts in female medical staff. Sci. Total Environ. 2019, 653, 1025–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Lowden, A.; Ozturk, G.; Reynolds, A.; Bjorvatn, B. Working Time society consensus statements: Evidence based interventions using light to improve circadian adaptation to working hours. Ind. Health 2019, 57, 213–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Boivin, D.B.; Boudreau, P.; James, F.O.; Kin, N.M. Photic resetting in night-shift work: Impact on nurses’ sleep. Chronobiol. Int. 2012, 29, 619–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Nowozin, C.; Wahnschaffe, A.; Rodenbeck, A.; de Zeeuw, J.; Hadel, S.; Kozakov, R.; Schopp, H.; Munch, M.; Kunz, D. Applying melanopic lux to measure biological light effects on melatonin suppression and subjective sleepiness. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2017, 14, 1042–1052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Lok, R.; Woelders, T.; Gordijn, M.; Hut, R.; Beersma, D.G. White light during daytime does not improve alertness in well-rested individuals. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2018, 33, 637–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Mure, L.; Vinberg, F.; Hanneken, A.; Panda, S. Functional diversity of human intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Science 2019, 366, 1251–1255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Kaynezhad, P.; Tachtsidis, I.; Jeffery, G. Optical monitoring of retinal respiration in real time: 670 nm light increases the redox state of mitochondria. Exp. Eye Res. 2016, 152, 88–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  119. Sivapathasuntharam, C.; Sivaprasad, S.; Hogg, C.; Jeffery, G. Improving mitochondrial function significantly reduces the rate of age related photoreceptor loss. Exp. Eye Res. 2019, 185, 107691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Chang, A.; Santhi, N.; St Hilaire, M.; Gronfier, C.; Bradstreet, D.; Duffy, J.; Lockley, S.; Kronauer, R.; Czeisler, C. Human responses to bright light of different durations. J. Physiol. London 2012, 590, 3103–3112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  121. Spitschan, M.; Bock, A.; Ryan, J.; Frazzetta, G.; Brainard, D.; Aguirre, G. The human visual cortex response to melanopsin-directed stimulation is accompanied by a distinct perceptual experience. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 12291–12296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  122. Faulkner, S.; Bee, P.; Meyer, N.; Dijk, D.; Drake, R. Light therapies to improve sleep in intrinsic circadian rhythm sleep disorders and neuro-psychiatric illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2019, 46, 108–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Geoffroy, P.; Schroder, C.; Reynaud, E.; Bourgin, P. Efficacy of light therapy versus antidepressant drugs, and of the combination versus monotherapy, in major depressive episodes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2019, 48, 101213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Gottlieb, J.; Benedetti, F.; Geoffroy, P.; Henriksen, T.; Lam, R.; Murray, G.; Phelps, J.; Sit, D.; Swartz, H.; Crowe, M.; et al. The chronotherapeutic treatment of bipolar disorders: A systematic review and practice recommendations from the ISBD task force on chronotherapy and chronobiology. Bipolar. Disord. 2019, 21, 741–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  125. Penders, T.; Stanciu, C.; Schoemann, A.; Ninan, P.; Bloch, R.; Saeed, S. Bright light therapy as augmentation of pharmacotherapy for treatment of depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Prim. Care Companion CNS Disord. 2016, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Beaulieu, C.; Rufiange, M.; Dumont, M.; Lachapelle, P. Modulation of ERG retinal sensitivity parameters with light environment and photoperiod. Doc. Ophthalmol. 2009, 118, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Danilenko, K.; Plisov, I.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Hebert, M. Human retinal light sensitivity and melatonin rhythms following four days in near darkness. Chronobiol. Int. 2009, 26, 93–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Kawasaki, A.; Wisniewski, S.; Healey, B.; Pattyn, N.; Kunz, D.; Basner, M.; Munch, M. Impact of long-term daylight deprivation on retinal light sensitivity, circadian rhythms and sleep during the Antarctic winter. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 16185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  129. Owen, J.; Arendt, J. Melatonin suppression in human subjects by bright and dim light in antarctica: Time and season-dependent effects. Neurosci. Lett. 1992, 137, 181–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Jasser, S.A.; Hanifin, J.; Rollag, M.; Brainard, G. Dim light adaptation attenuates acute melatonin suppression in humans. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2006, 21, 394–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Hébert, M.; Martin, S.K.; Eastman, C.I. The effects of prior light history on the suppression of melatonin by light in humans. J. Pineal Res. 2002, 33, 198–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  132. Chang, A.; Scheer, F.; Czeisler, C.; Aeschbach, D. Direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans depend on prior light history. Sleep 2013, 36, 1239–1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Chakraborty, R.; Ostrin, L.; Nickla, D.; Iuvone, P.; Pardue, M.; Stone, R. Circadian rhythms, refractive development, and myopia. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2018, 38, 217–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Riemersma-van der Lek, R.F.; Swaab, D.F.; Twisk, J.; Hol, E.M.; Hoogendijk, W.J.G.; Van Someren, E.J.W. Effect of bright light and melatonin on cognitive and noncognitive function in elderly residents of group care facilities: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2008, 299, 2642–2655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Bromundt, V.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Boutellier, M.; Winter, S.; Haberstroh, M.; Terman, M.; Munch, M. Effects of a dawn-dusk simulation on circadian rest-activity cycles, sleep, mood and well-being in dementia patients. Exp. Gerontol. 2019, 124, 110641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Wahnschaffe, A.; Nowozin, C.; Haedel, S.; Rath, A.; Appelhof, S.; Munch, M.; Kunz, D. Implementation of dynamic lighting in a nursing home: Impact on agitation but not on rest-activity patterns. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2017, 14, 1076–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Figueiro, M.; Plitnick, B.; Roohan, C.; Sahin, L.; Kalsher, M.; Rea, M. Effects of a tailored lighting intervention on sleep quality, rest-activity, mood, and behavior in older adults with Alzheimer disease and related dementias: A randomized clinical trial. Clin. Sleep Med. 2019, 15, 1757–1767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  138. Figueiro, M.; Plitnick, B.; Lok, A.; Jones, G.; Higgins, P.; Hornick, T.; Rea, M.S. Tailored lighting intervention improves measures of sleep, depression, and agitation in persons with Alzheimer’s disease and related dementia living in long-term care facilities. Clin. Interv. Aging 2014, 9, 1527–1537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  139. Fargason, R.; Fobian, A.; Hablitz, L.; Paul, J.; White, B.; Cropsey, K.; Gamble, K. Correcting delayed circadian phase with bright light therapy predicts improvement in ADHD symptoms: A pilot study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2017, 91, 105–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  140. Bromundt, V.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Kyburz, S.; Opwis, K.; Dammann, G.; Cajochen, C. Circadian sleep-wake cyles, well-being, and light therapy in borderline personality disorder. J. Pers. Disord. 2012, 27, 680–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  141. Rutten, S.; Vriend, C.; van den Heuvel, O.; Smit, J.; Berendse, H.; van der Werf, Y. Bright light therapy in Parkinson’s disease: An overview of the background and evidence. Parkinson’s Dis. 2012, 2012, 767105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  142. Smilowska, K.; van Wamelen, J.; Schoutens, A.; Meinders, M.; Bloem, B. Blue light therapy glasses in Parkinson’s disease: patients’ experience. Parkinson’s Dis. 2019, 2019, 1906271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  143. Burkhalter, H.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Denhaerynck, K.; Fehr, T.; Steiger, J.; Venzin, R.; Cajochen, C.; Weaver, T.; De Geest, S. The effect of bright light therapy on sleep and circadian rhythms in renal transplant recipients: A pilot randomized, multicentre wait-list controlled trial. Transpl. Int. 2015, 28, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Johnson, J.; Garland, S.; Carlson, L.; Savard, J.; Simpson, J.; Ancoli-Israel, S.; Campbell, T. Bright light therapy improves cancer-related fatigue in cancer survivors: A randomized controlled trial. J. Cancer Surviv. 2018, 12, 206–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Wirz-Justice, A.; Graw, P.; Kräuchi, K.; Sarrafzadeh, A.; English, J.; Arendt, J.; Sand, L. Natural’ light treatment of seasonal affective disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 1996, 37, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Gbyl, K.; Ostergaard Madsen, H.; Dunker Svendsen, S.; Petersen, P.; Hageman, I.; Volf, C.; Martiny, K. Depressed patients hospitalized in southeast-facing rooms are discharged earlier than patients in Northwest-facing rooms. Neuropsychobiology 2016, 74, 193–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Gbyl, K.; Ostergaard Madsen, H.; Dunker Svendsen, S.; Petersen, P.; Hageman, I.; Volf, C.; Martiny, K. Erratum. Neuropsychobiology 2017, 75, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Benedetti, F.; Colombo, C.; Barbini, B.; Campori, E.; Smeraldi, E. Morning sunlight reduces length of hospitalization in bipolar depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2001, 62, 221–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Gimenez, M.; Geerdinck, L.; Versteylen, M.; Leffers, P.; Meekes, G.; Herremans, H.; de Ruyter, B.; Bikker, J.; Kuijpers, P.; Schlangen, L. Patient room lighting influences on sleep, appraisal and mood in hospitalized people. J. Sleep Res. 2017, 26, 236–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  150. Luetz, A.; Grunow, J.; Morgeli, R.; Rosenthal, M.; Weber-Carstens, S.; Weiss, B.; Spies, C. Innovative ICU solutions to prevent and reduce delirium and post-intensive care unit syndrome. Semin. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2019, 40, 673–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Beauchemin, K.M.; Hays, P. Dying in the dark: Sunshine, gender and outcomes in myocardial infarction. J. R. Soc. Med. 1998, 91, 352–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  152. Ritchie, H.; Stothard, E.; Wright, K.J. Entrainment of the human circadian clock to the light-dark cycle and its impact on patients in the ICU and nursing home settings. Curr. Pharma. Des. 2015, 21, 3438–3442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Munch, M.; Schmieder, M.; Bieler, K.; Goldbach, R.; Fuhrmann, T.; Zumstein, N.; Vonmoos, P.; Scartezzini, J.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Cajochen, C. Bright light delights: Effects of daily light exposure on emotions, rest-activity cycles, sleep and melatonin secretion in severely demented patients. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2017, 14, 1063–1075. [Google Scholar]
  154. Gies, P.; van Deventer, E.; Green, A.; Sinclair, C.; Tinker, R. Review of the global solar UV index 2015 workshop report. Health Phys. 2018, 114, 84–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  155. Lanca, C.; Teo, A.; Vivagandan, A.; Htoon, H.; Najjar, R.; Spiegel, D.; Pu, S.; Saw, S. The effects of different outdoor environments, sunglasses and hats on light levels: Implications for myopia prevention. Transl. Vis. Sci. Technol. 2019, 8, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  156. Phillips, A.; Vidafar, P.; Burns, A.; McGlashan, E.; Anderson, C.; Rajaratnam, S.; Lockley, S.; Cain, S. High sensitivity and interindividual variability in the response of the human circadian system to evening light. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 12019–12024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  157. McGlashan, E.; Poudel, G.; Vidafar, P.; Drummond, S.; Cain, S. Imaging individual differences in the response of the human suprachiasmatic area to light. Front. Neurol. 2018, 9, 1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  158. Watson, L.; Phillips, A.K.; Hosken, I.; McGlashan, E.; Anderson, C.; Lack, L.; Lockley, S.; Rajaratnam, S.M.; Cain, S. Increased sensitivity of the circadian system to light in delayed sleep-wake phase disorder. J. Physiol. London 2018, 596, 6249–6261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  159. Akacem, L.; Wright, K., Jr.; LeBourgeois, M. Sensitivity of the circadian system to evening bright light in preschool-age children. Physiol. Rep. 2018, 6, e13617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  160. Higuchi, S.; Nagafuchi, Y.; Lee, S.I.; Harada, T. Influence of light at night on melatonin suppression in children. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2014, 99, 3298–3303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Nagare, R.; Rea, M.S.; Plitnick, B.; Figueiro, M. Nocturnal melatonin suppression by adolescents and adults for different levels, spectra, and durations of light exposure. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2019, 34, 178–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Chellappa, S.; Bromundt, V.; Frey, S.; Steinemann, A.; Schmidt, C.; Schlote, T.; Goldblum, D.; Cajochen, C. Association of intraocular cataract lens replacement with circadian rhythms, cognitive function, and sleep in older adults. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2019, 137, 878–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  163. Turner, P.L.; Mainster, M.A. Circadian photoreception: Ageing and the eye’s important role in systemic health. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 2008, 92, 1439–1444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  164. Münch, M.; Ladaique, M.; Roemer, S.; Hashemi, K.; Kawasaki, A. Melanopsin-mediated acute light responses measured in winter and in summer: Seasonal variations in adults with and without cataracts. Front. Neurol. 2017, 8, 464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  165. Najjar, R.; Chiquet, C.; Teikari, P.; Cornut, P.; Claustrat, B.; Denis, P.; Cooper, H.; Gronfier, C. Aging of non-visual spectral sensitivity to light in humans: Compensatory mechanisms? PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e85837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  166. Daneault, V.; Vandewalle, G.; Hébert, M.; Teikari, P.; Mure, L.; Doyon, J.; Gronfier, C.; Cooper, H.; Dumont, M.; Carrier, J. Does pupil constriction under blue and green monochromatic light exposure change with age? J. Biol. Rhythm. 2012, 27, 257–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  167. Park, J.; Moura, A.; Raza, A.; Rhee, D.; Kardon, R.; Hood, D. Toward a clinical protocol for assessing rod, cone, and melanopsin contributions to the human pupil response. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2011, 52, 6624–6635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  168. Wang, B.; Shen, C.; Zhang, L.; Qi, L.; Yao, L.; Chen, J.; Yang, G.; Chen, T.; Zhang, Z. Dark adaptation-induced changes in rod, cone and intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cell (ipRGC) sensitivity differentially affect the pupil light response (PLR). Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 2015, 253, 1997–2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  169. Yuhas, P.; Shorter, P.; McDaniel, E.; Earley, M.; Hartwick, A. Blue and red light-evoked pupil responses in photophobic subjects with TBI. Optom. Vis. Sci. 2017, 94, 108–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  170. Kardon, R.; Hong, S.; Kawasaki, A. Entrance pupil size predicts retina illumination in darkly pigmented eyes but not lightly pigmented eyes. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2013, 54, 5559–5567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  171. Kankipati, L.; Girkin, C.; Gamlin, P. The post-illumination pupil response is reduced in glaucoma patients. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2011, 52, 2287–2292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  172. Münch, M.; Léon, L.; Collomb, S.; Kawasaki, A. Comparison of acute non-visual bright light responses in patients with optic nerve disease, glaucoma and healthy controls. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 15185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  173. Rukmini, A.; Milea, D.; Baskaran, M.; How, A.; Perera, S.; Aung, T.; Gooley, J. Pupillary responses to high-irradiance blue light correlate with glaucoma severity. Ophthalmology 2015, 122, 1777–1785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  174. Perez-Rico, C.; de la Villa, P.; Arribas-Gomez, I.; Blanco, R. Evaluation of functional integrity of the retinohypothalamic tract in advanced glaucoma using multifocal electroretinography and light-induced melatonin suppression. Exp. Eye Res. 2010, 91, 578–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  175. Maynard, M.; Zele, A.; Feigl, B. Melanopsin-mediated post-illumination pupil response in early age-related macular degeneration. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2015, 56, 6906–6913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  176. Barboni, M.T.S.; Bueno, C.; Nagy, B.; Maia, P.; Vidal, K.; Alves, R.; Reiter, R.J.; do Amaral, F.; Cipolla-Neto, J.; Ventura, D. Melanopsin system dysfunction in Smith-Magenis syndrome patients. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2018, 59, 362–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  177. Bullock, B.; McGlashan, E.M.; Burns, A.; Lu, B.; Cain, S. Traits related to bipolar disorder are associated with an increased post-illumination pupil response. Psychiatry Res. 2019, 278, 35–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  178. Roecklein, K.; Wong, P.; Ernecoff, N.; Miller, M.; Donofry, S.; Kamarck, M.; Wood-Vasey, W.; Franzen, P. The post illumination pupil response is reduced in seasonal affective disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2013, 210, 150–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  179. Dumpala, S.; Zele, A.; Feigl, B. Outer retinal structure and function deficits contribute to circadian disruption in patients with type 2 diabetes. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2019, 60, 1870–1878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  180. McGlashan, E.; Nandam, L.; Vidafar, P.; Mansfield, D.; Rajaratnam, S.; Cain, S. The SSRI citalopram increases the sensitivity of the human circadian system to light in an acute dose. Psychopharmacology 2018, 235, 3201–3209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  181. Turco, M.; Biscontin, A.; Corrias, M.; Caccin, L.; Bano, M.; Chiaromanni, F.; Salamanca, M.; Mattei, D.; Salvoro, C.; Mazzotta, G.; et al. Diurnal preference, mood and the response to morning light in relation to polymorphisms in the human clock gene PER3. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 6967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  182. Azzi, A.; Dallmann, R.; Casserly, A.; Rehrauer, H.; Patrignani, A.; Maier, B.; Kramer, A.; Brown, S. Circadian behavior is light-reprogrammed by plastic DNA methylation. Nat. Neurosci. 2014, 17, 377–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  183. Roecklein, K.; Rohan, K.; Duncan, W.; Rollag, M.; Rosenthal, N.; Lipsky, R.; Provencio, I. A missense variant (P10L) of the melanopsin (OPN4) gene in seasonal affective disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2009, 114, 279–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  184. Lee, S.; Hida, A.; Tsujimura, S.; Morita, T.; Mishima, K.; Higuchi, S. Association between melanopsin gene polymorphism (I394T) and pupillary light reflex is dependent on light wavelength. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2013, 32, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  185. Pereira, D.; Tufik, S.; Louzada, F.; Benedito-Silva, A.; Lopez, A.R.; Lemos, N.; Korczak, A.; D’Almeida, V.; Pedrazzoli, M. Association of the length polymorphism delayed sleep-phase syndrome: Does in the human Per3 gene with the latitude have an influence upon it? Sleep 2005, 28, 29–32. [Google Scholar]
  186. Mendes, M.; Darling, A.; Hart, K.; Morse, S.; Murphy, R.J.; Lanham-New, S. Impact of high latitude, urban living and ethnicity on 25-hydroxyvitamin D status: A need for multidisciplinary action? J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 2019, 188, 95–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  187. Grant, W.; Bhattoa, H.; Pludowski, P. Determinants of Vitamin D Deficiency From Sun Exposure: A Global Perspective. In Vitamin D, 4th ed.; Feldman, D., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 79–90. [Google Scholar]
  188. Darling, A.; Hart, K.; Arber, S.; Berry, J.; Morgan, P.; Middleton, B.; SA, L.-N.; Skene, D. 25-Hydroxyvitamin D status, light exposure and sleep quality in UK dwelling South Asian and Caucasian postmenopausal women. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 2019, 189, 265–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  189. van der Meijden, W.; Van Someren, J.; Te Lindert, B.; Bruijel, J.; van Oosterhout, F.; Coppens, J.; Kalsbeek, A.; Cajochen, C.; Bourgin, P.; Van Someren, E. Individual differences in sleep timing relate to melanopsin-based phototransduction in healthy adolescents and young adults. Sleep 2016, 39, 1305–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  190. Leger, D.; Bayon, V.; Elbaz, M.; Philip, P.; Choudat, D. Underexposure to light at work and its dissociation to insomnia and sleepiness: A cross-sectional study of 13 296 workers of one transportation company. J. Psychosom. Res. 2011, 70, 29–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  191. Rufiange, M.; Beaulieu, C.; Lachapelle, P.; Dumont, M. Circadian light sensitivity and rate of retinal dark adaptation in indoor and outdoor workers. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2007, 22, 454–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  192. Daugaard, S.; Markvart, J.; Bonde, J.; Christoffersen, J.; Garde, A.; Hansen, A.; Schlunssen, V.; Vestergaard, J.; Vistisen, H.; Kolstad, H. Light exposure during days with night, outdoor, and indoor work. Ann. Work Expo. Health 2019, 63, 651–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  193. Chellappa, S.; Steiner, R.; Oelhafen, P.; Cajochen, C. Sex differences in light sensitivity impact on brightness perception, vigilant attention and sleep in humans. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 14215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  194. Abramov, I.; Gordon, J.; Feldman, O.; Chavarga, A. Sex and vision II: Color appearance of monochromatic lights. Biol. Sex Differ. 2012, 3, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  195. Rao, S.; Chun, C.; Fan, J.; Kofron, J.; Yang, M.B.; Hegde, R.; Ferrara, N.; Copenhagen, D.; Lang, R. A direct and melanopsin-dependent fetal light response regulates mouse eye development. Nature 2013, 494, 243–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  196. Cambras, T.; Canal, M.; Cernuda-Cernuda, R.; Garcia-Fernandez, J.; Diez-Noguera, A. Darkness during early postnatal development is required for normal circadian patterns in the adult rat. Chronobiol. Int. 2015, 32, 178–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  197. Kerkhof, G.; Korving, H.; Willemse-vd Geest, H.; Rietveld, W. Diurnal differences between morning-type and evening-type subjects in self-rated alertness, body temperature and the visual and auditory evoked potential. Neurosci. Lett. 1980, 1, 11–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  198. Horne, J.A.; Östberg, O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol. 1976, 4, 97–110. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  199. Porcheret, K.; Wald, L.; Fritschi, L.; Gerkema, M.; Gordijn, M.; Merrrow, M.; Rajaratnam, S.; Rock, D.; Sletten, T.; Warman, G.; et al. Chronotype and environmental light exposure in a student population. Chronobiol. Int. 2018, 35, 1365–1374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  200. Roenneberg, T. Having trouble typing? What on earth Is chronotype? J. Biol. Rhythm. 2015, 30, 487–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  201. Roenneberg, T.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Merrow, M. Life between clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2003, 18, 80–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  202. Goulet, G.; Mongrain, V.; Desrosiers, C.; Paquet, J.; Dumont, M. Daily light exposure in morning-type and evening-type individuals. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2007, 22, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  203. Shochat, T.; Santhi, N.; Herer, P.; Flavell, S.; Skeldon, A.; Dijk, D. Sleep timing in late autumn and late spring associates with light exposure rather than sun time in college students. Front. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  204. Dumont, M.; Blais, H.; Roy, J.; Paquet, J. Controlled patterns of daytime light exposure improve circadian adjustment in simulated night work. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2009, 24, 427–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  205. Matusiak, B.S.; Klöckner, C.A. How we evaluate the view out through the window. Archit. Sci. Rev. 2016, 59, 203–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  206. Ulrich, R. Biophilia, biophobia, and natural landscapes. In The Biophilia Hypothesis; Kellert, S.R., Wilson, E., Eds.; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1993; pp. 73–137. [Google Scholar]
  207. Vondrasova, D.; Hajek, I.; Illnerova, H. Exposure to long summer days affects the human melatonin and cortisol rhythms. Brain Res. 1997, 759, 166–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  208. Hadlow, N.; Brown, S.; Wardrop, R.; Conradie, J.; Henley, D. Where in the world? Latitude, longitude and season contribute to the complex co-ordinates determining cortisol levels. Clin. Endocrinol. Oxf 2018, 89, 299–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  209. Eastman, C.I. Natural summer and winter sunlight exposure patterns in seasonal affective disorder. Physiol. Behav. 1990, 48, 611–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  210. Graw, P.; Recker, S.; Sand, L.; Kräuchi, K.; Wirz-Justice, A. Winter and summer outdoor light exposure in women with and without seasonal affective disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 1999, 56, 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  211. Hadlow, N.; Brown, S.; Wardrop, R.; Henley, D. The effects of season, daylight saving and time of sunrise on serum cortisol in a large population. Chronobiol. Int. 2014, 31, 243–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  212. Roenneberg, T.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Skene, D.J.; Ancoli-Israel, S.; Wright, K.P.; Dijk, D.J.; Zee, P.; Gorman, M.R.; Winnebeck, E.; Klerman, E. Why should we abolish daylight saving time? J. Biol. Rhythm. 2019, 34, 227–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  213. Roenneberg, T.; Winnebeck, E.; Klerman, E. Daylight saving time and artificial time zones-a battle between biological and social times. Front. Physiol. 2019, 10, 944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  214. Kantermann, T.; Juda, M.; Merrow, M.; Roenneberg, T. The human circadian clock’s seasonal adjustment is disrupted by daylight saving time. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 1996–2000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  215. Borisenkov, M.; Tserne, T.; Panev, A.; Kuznetsova, E.; Petrova, N.; Timonin, V.; Kolomeichuk, S.; Vinogradova, I.; Kovyazina, M.; Khokhlov, N.; et al. Seven-year survey of sleep timing in Russian children and adolescents: Chronic 1-h forward transition of social clock is associated with increased social jetlag and winter pattern of mood seasonality. Biol. Rhythm. Res. 2017, 48, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  216. Andersen, M. Unweaving the human response in daylighting design. Build. Environ. 2015, 91, 101–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  217. van Duijnhoven, J.; Aarts, M.P.; Aries, M.B.; Rosemann, A.; Kort, H. Systematic review on the interaction between office light conditions and occupational health: Elucidating gaps and methodological issues. Indoor Built Environ. 2019, 28, 152–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  218. Dubois, M.; Cantin, F.; Johnsen, K. The effect of coated glazing on visual perception: A pilot study using scaleamodels. Lighting Res. Technol. 2007, 39, 283–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  219. Dong, B.; Prakash, V.; Feng, F.; O’Neill, Z. A review of smart building sensing system for better indoor environment control. Energy Build. 2019, 199, 29–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  220. Pasanen, T.; Tyrvainen, L.; Korpela, K. The relationship between perceived health and physical activity indoors, outdoors in built environments, and outdoors in nature. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 2014, 6, 324–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  221. Takayama, N.; Korpela, K.; Lee, J.; Morikawa, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Park, B.J.; Li, Q.; Tyrvainen, L.; Miyazaki, Y.; Kagawa, T. Emotional, restorative and vitalizing effects of forest and urban environments at four sites in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 7207–7230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  222. Donovan, G.; Gatziolis, D.; Douwes, J. Relationship between exposure to the natural environment and recovery from hip or knee arthroplasty: A New Zealand retrospective cohort study. BMJ. Open 2019, 9, e029522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  223. Nang, E.; Abuduxike, G.; Posadzki, P.; Divakar, U.; Visvalingam, N.; Nazeha, N.; Dunleavy, G.; Christopoulos, G.; Soh, C.; Jarbrink, K.; et al. Review of the potential health effects of light and environmental exposures in underground workplaces. Tunn. Undergr. Space Technol. 2019, 84, 201–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  224. Spitschan, M.; Stefani, O.; Blattner, P.; Gronfier, C.; Lockley, S.; Lucas, R. How to report light exposure in human chronobiology and sleep research experiments. Clocks Sleep 2019, 1, 280–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  225. International Commissoin on Illumination. CIE System for Metrology of Optical Radiation for ipRGC-Influenced Responses to Light; CIE: Vienna, Austria, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  226. World Health Organization. Implementation framework for phase VII (2019–2014) of the WHO European Healthy Cities Network: Goals, Requirements and Strategic Approaches. Available online: http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/environment-and-health/urban-health/who-european-healthy-cities-network (accessed on 10 October 2019).
  227. World Health Organization. Health 21-Health for all in the 21st Century: An Introduction; World Health Organization Europe: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  228. Lucas, R.; Peirson, S.; Berson, D.; Brown, T.; Cooper, H.; Czeisler, C.; Figueiro, M.; Gamlin, P.; Lockley, S.; O’Hagan, J.; et al. Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci. 2014, 37, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  229. Markvart, J.; Hansen, Å.; Christoffersen, J. Comparison and correction of the light sensor output from 48 wearable light exposure devices by using a side-by-side field calibration method. Leukos 2015, 11, 155–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  230. Figueiro, M.; Hamner, R.; Bierman, A.; Rea, M. Comparisons of three practical field devices used to measure personal light exposures and activity levels. Lighting Res. Technol. 2013, 45, 421–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The identified three main groups of gaps of knowledge in daylight research.
Figure 1. The identified three main groups of gaps of knowledge in daylight research.
Clockssleep 02 00008 g001
Figure 2. Illustration of daylight and other factors as determinants of human health. There are two main dimensions at the individual level, which are impacted by daylight: human needs and individual differences. There are three dimensions of reciprocal actions that modify the impact of daylight on humans: (1) drivers and constraints, (2) environmental conditions and (3) scientific and technical developments.
Figure 2. Illustration of daylight and other factors as determinants of human health. There are two main dimensions at the individual level, which are impacted by daylight: human needs and individual differences. There are three dimensions of reciprocal actions that modify the impact of daylight on humans: (1) drivers and constraints, (2) environmental conditions and (3) scientific and technical developments.
Clockssleep 02 00008 g002
Table 1. Some open questions in mixed electric light/daylight research related to metabolic functions, sleep, alertness and cognition as well as physical activity.
Table 1. Some open questions in mixed electric light/daylight research related to metabolic functions, sleep, alertness and cognition as well as physical activity.
(i) Metabolic functions
How does electric light and/or daylight affect metabolic functions? This question is closely related to meal timing, caloric intake and meal composition, or weight loss. A few studies have investigated these aspects under laboratory conditions [72,73,74], but not yet under daylight conditions. The question is whether daylight exposure specifically affects the timing of meals, post-prandial responses, temperature regulation, metabolism, body composition and the gut microbiome?
(ii)Sleep
Does daylight and/or mixed daylight/electric light conditions during the day mediate better sleep quality at night, and if yes, how?There is some evidence that indoor and/or outdoor bright light exposure during the day leads to longer sleep duration and increases sleep quality as was shown in laboratory and field studies [63,64,75]. Electric light exposure (with a spectral power distribution closer to daylight than standard LED-light) during daytime enhances slow wave sleep (=‘deep sleep’) [76] and brighter ambient light (compared to dim light) increased homeostatic sleep pressure during wakefulness [77]. The following questions relate to daylight and sleep:
  • Does insufficient sleep (partial and/or chronic sleep deficit) counteract beneficial daylight exposure effects?
  • Can sufficient daytime light exposure offset negative consequences of electric light exposure at night—with respect to entrainment, sleep, performance and health outcomes [78,79,80]?
(iii) Alertness andcognition
How do daylight-specific properties affect alertness and cognitive functions?Many laboratory studies with steady state electric lighting showed light-dependent alertness [45,46,49,81,82,83] and cognitive repercussions [46,53,55]. A few studies have looked at spectrally tuned electrical light conditions and alertness and cognition [76,84,85,86].
(iv)Physicalactivity
Does physical activity and daylight interact to induce larger phase shifts of the circadian clock?From studies in the laboratory there is a phase-response curve of physical activity with both phase delays and advances at specific times of day [87]. Activity facilitates phase delays in very dim light [88]. The question relates to the interaction of exercise with (day-) light exposure as shown with cycling performance [89].
Table 2. Gaps in knowledge about the impact of natural light under real-life conditions.
Table 2. Gaps in knowledge about the impact of natural light under real-life conditions.
Daylight and Circadian Entrainment
(i) Daylight properties:
Quality/quantity
Which combinations of daylight qualities and quantities are relevant for circadian entrainment (alone and in combination with electric light sources) and dependent on time of day?
Spectrum/colourWhich frequency bands of the electromagnetic spectrum are relevant when and how important is the integration of colour [100]?
DynamicsDo ultradian variations/dynamics of daylight play a role in circadian entrainment?
TwilightWhat is the role of twilight (dawn and dusk) for circadian entrainment in humans [101,102]? For example, is the colour signal of the “blue hour” as important for humans as it is for mice [103,104]?
Polarisation of daylightWhat is the role of polarised daylight? Direct sunlight is not polarised but daylight from a particular region of the sky is partially polarised. In contrast to bees, humans can hardly perceive polarised light, however a physiological influence of polarised light on humans cannot yet be excluded. A study by Brainard et al. showed no difference between unpolarised and polarised light on melatonin suppression [105].
Role of light distribution in the visual field, direction of lightA further characteristic of daylight is its large-area expansion with an unobstructed view of the sky. Non-human primates have ipRGcs distributed over the entire retina with a density of 3-5 cells/mm², and a maximum concentration of 20-25 cells/mm² around the fovea [106]. Since rods, cones and the ipRGCs in the eye are distributed over large areas of the retina, it is assumed that the non-visual effect of light is greatest when the light comes from a large source, such as an indirect illumination of a large bright area. In nature, this light comes from the sky. If only a small area of the retina is illuminated, as is the case with directed light from a spot, a weaker non-visual effect is assumed. The direction of light seems to be crucial at least for electric light that was shown to affect melatonin suppression [107,108,109] and sleep [110].
Season, latitude and day lengthDoes photoperiod length impact on circadian entrainment? From the literature it is known that there are differences in humans under purely natural seasonal lighting conditions when compared to mixed natural/electric light conditions [78,79,111].
(ii) Effect on peripheral tissuesHow does electric light and/or daylight (indirectly) influence peripheral clock tissue function (e.g., metabolism, immune function, cardiovascular function, cell repair, detoxification, mitochondrial turnover)?
(iii) Circadian misalignment/disruptionHow can electric and/or daylight help to mitigate circadian misalignment/circadian disruption (e.g., in shift workers [112,113,114], adolescents)?
Table 3. Unanswered questions related to properties of daylight and or electric light indoor lighting conditions.
Table 3. Unanswered questions related to properties of daylight and or electric light indoor lighting conditions.
Spectral rangeDoes the fact that the spectral power distribution of daylight goes beyond the visible range make a difference for non-visual functions, when compared to electric light? For example, the infrared (IR) portion that is always present in daylight is non-existent in electric light (except for banned incandescent light sources). It might have an important role for the retina, since a large body of literature suggests connections between long-wavelength radiation and (beneficial) physiological functions in the retina (see e.g., [118,119]). In addition, there is probably a specific role of the spectral power distribution and colour of twilight, as shown in rodents [103,104].
Duration and thresholds
-
What are daylight or mixed electric/daylight exposure durations [52,120] and thresholds for non-visual functions, under real life circumstances?
-
What is the threshold of daylight/electric light conditions in the evening/night that does not interfere with sleep onset/propensity?
Dose-response relationshipsWhat are the dose-response relationships for daylight and mixed daylight/electric light exposures for non-visual functions? In particular, with regard to the interaction of circadian and homeostatic processes with environmental conditions – that include electric light.
Irradiance and spectral compositionDaylight is spatially and temporally variable. These changes take place over very wide frequency bands:
-
What are these frequency bands, irradiances and are these variations required for the circadian system and other non-visual functions?
-
What is the influence of different contrasts of daylight or mixed electric light/daylight for photoreceptors (i.e., high vs. low melanopic irradiance) as was shown for electric light on visual perception [121], pupil light reflex and alertness [84,86]?
24 h-DynamicsHow do the 24-h dynamics of daylight impact on non-visual functions?
Light historyHow can prior light history (see Table 4) of any light exposure be incorporated into the above-mentioned outcomes?
Therapeutic use of lightLight treatment has been established for winter and other depressive disorders and circadian sleep disturbances [122,123,124,125]. What should the daylight exposure recommendations be for these different therapeutic interventions (regarding intensity, spectrum, timing and prior light exposure)?
Table 4. Inter-individual trait and state differences in response to electric light or mixed electric light/daylight.
Table 4. Inter-individual trait and state differences in response to electric light or mixed electric light/daylight.
(i) Physiological differences:
-
Light sensitivity in adults [156,157,158] and children/adolescents [159,160,161], and elderly (less light transmission with age due to e.g., yellowing of the lens, cataracts [162,163,164,165], reduced pupil size [163,166]),
-
Pupil responses to light in healthy persons [167,168,169,170] and patients with eye disease [171,172,173,174,175]
-
Light responses depending on generalised medical and psychiatric status [176,177,178,179] and medication [180]
(ii) Genetic differences:
-
Known for the clock gene PER3 [181] and epigenetic modulators [182]
-
Genetic missense variant for melanopsin gene in seasonal affective disorder patients [183]
-
Melanopsin gene polymorphism [184] revealed changes in light sensitivity- Is there genetic adaptation of light sensitivity to geographical latitude, as was suggested for a polymorphism in PER3 gene length depending on latitude [185]?
(iii) Cultural, behavioural differences:
-
Cultural and social differences (e.g., clothing) [186,187]
-
What are the societal and ethnic/cultural differences in outdoor-related behaviour [188]?
-
How do various climate constraints limit going outdoors [186]?
(iv) Mixed physiological/behavioural effects:
-
Different photic histories, due to inter-individual differences in sleep/wake patterns [158,189] and/or
-
work/social schedules [126,128,190,191,192] (see also below – morning/evening types),
-
Sex/gender [193,194]
-
Does (day-) light exposure during pregnancy and early postnatal periods play a role in individual eye development
-
and circadian behaviour as shown in mice and rats [195,196]?
-
Morning/evening types [197,198,199] (i.e., chronotypes [200,201]—which may be considered a behavioural trait or as
-
proxy for phase of entrainment) and their preferences/choices for light [67,202,203]
Table 5. Identified workplace daylight as well as mixed electric light/daylight associated questions.
Table 5. Identified workplace daylight as well as mixed electric light/daylight associated questions.
Daylight conditions for individuals at workplacesHow much and which qualities of daylight do different individuals/groups receive at their work- and living places (24/7), and how does this relate to their health status?
Daylight exposure as a countermeasure (for shift workers)Can daylight be a means to counteract the detrimental effects of “light at the wrong time of day”, such as occurs with light in the evening or with (night) shift work? The reason might be that light during the day has a desensitisation effect for light exposure in the evening (see Table 3 and Table 4: light history), and in night shift workers bright light exposure after daytime sleep could help readjustment to the daytime work hours [204], see also recent recommendations of the ‘Working Time Society’ (WTS/ICOH; [113]).
Daylight and visual comfort and non-visual functions at workplacesAre existing workspace regulations on glare and visual comfort at workplaces sufficient to concomitantly provide good and ‘biologically relevant’ daylight conditions [192]?
View/windowHow important is the view out of the window and the environment outside the window [205] for workplace-related variables such as performance and alertness?
Table 6. Gaps in knowledge of daylight research related to environmental factors.
Table 6. Gaps in knowledge of daylight research related to environmental factors.
Impactof latitude, time zones, climate
Seasonal changesDoes exposure to seasonal changes in day length have consequences for human physiology and health, as some of the existing literature suggests [79,111,207,208]?
LatitudeIn those most vulnerable e.g., living at high latitudes, light therapy and improved home/work lighting has been shown to be useful for winter depression and sleep disorders, given that there is insufficient daylight for some months of the year [209,210].
Location within time zoneDoes the location within a time zone modify daylight’s effect on an individual’s circadian phase [208,211]?
Daylight saving time (DST)Is there a long-term effect of daylight saving time [212,213,214,215] on wake and sleep physiology?
Micro/macroclimateHow does ambient temperature, humidity, and air pollution modulate daylight’s effects on physiology?
Rural vs. urban environmentsIs there a difference in daylight exposure dose between those living in rural vs. urban environments?
Architecture, urban design
Daylight conditions in buildingsWhat role do the complex daylight conditions in buildings, such as building orientation, window positioning, glazing, play on visual comfort/adversity (glare), productivity and performance, circadian entrainment and health in general [32,216,217]? Could architecture be deployed to accentuate spatial-temporal modulation of daylight to stimulate retinal photoreception?
GlazingHow do single vs. double vs. triple glazing systems, electrochromic windows, and new technologies such as smart windows with integrated micro-daylighting systems influence health [218]? How effective is dynamic glazing in order to address heat emission and light?
Floor orientation, spatial distribution of daylightHow does the location of apartments (floor and geographical orientation) within a given building, and the indoor spatial distribution of daylight, affect human health?
Daylight enhanced qualitiesWhat design can provide daylight-enhanced qualities in single buildings and urban settings?
Complementation with electrical light/daylight systemsHow can the geographical orientation of a building, which determines its indoor light intensity, be compensated/complemented with daylight systems/electric light?
(Day-) light control systemsHow should modern (light) sensor and controller technology be used to support health, performance, and well-being of its inhabitants?
Self-control of (day-) lightHow much self-control over sensor control is necessary and desired [219]?
Alienation from nature What are the consequences of losses of daylight/weather/seasonal effects due to urban densification, loss of daylight recreation areas, daylight restriction due to high buildings etc. on mood, health and quality of life [69,220,221,222,223]?
Role of viewLow daylight exposure and daylight deprivation usually also means deprivation from a view. This could have additional and far-reaching negative consequences, although research to date is scarce [223].
Table 7. Gaps in knowledge related to measurement tools and methods.
Table 7. Gaps in knowledge related to measurement tools and methods.
Automated integration of daylight and electric light in buildingsLack of sophisticated and automated integration of daylight and electric light in buildings and algorithms to detect the two separately (this would also support reduction of lighting derived energy costs)
Monitoring spectral irradianceLack of validated, commercially available and affordable wearable devices for continuously monitoring spectral irradiance (at eye level). This could also be used as a “dosimeter” in research, therapies, living/working spaces and for lifestyle applications.
Tools for circadian phase assessmentsLack of practical means to make circadian phase assessments in daily life, the clinic, and elsewhere (comparing electric and daylight conditions).
Tools for mental health evaluationsLong-term daily mental health evaluations; approaches developed so far are wrist-worn diaries with visual analogue scales (also via mobile derived apps).
Standard operation procedures for the use of daylight treatment and daylight exposure (with regards to non-visual light responses)Lack of standard operation procedures (SOPs) and definitions of daylight treatment responses for different individuals and patients. There is a need for large-scale field studies in schools, institutions (e.g., hospitals, prisons, care homes), shift- and non-shift workplaces, people working underground and people frequently traveling across times zones (with different overlay stays) using the same SOPs.
Norms and metricsNo (internationally accepted) consensus on the parameters to be measured and reported, and at what level of accuracy the monitoring tools can achieve this (see Section 2.3). A recent tutorial paper summarises the most important requirements [224]. One question that also arises: What is a suitable light metric to measure “naturalness” of light? How can we compare electric light sources to daylight? Some existing official metrics are summarised in the most recent international CIE standard, even though the D65/D55 parameters do not reflect the spectrum of daylight [225] and need to be revised to incorporate seasonal and geographical changes.
Large scale lighting digitalisationLack of large scale/practical biomedical digital techniques to design, monitor, predict and validate individually tailored daylight exposure/electric light regimens.
Table 8. Short-term objectives in daylight research.
Table 8. Short-term objectives in daylight research.
CriteriaDefine criteria for (day) light measures (see e.g., the new CIE standard S026). Universally agree to use this new standard, which is facilitated by the CIE-S-026-EDI-Toolbox-beta version E1.051.xls based on Ref. [228] and a more recent Tutorial [224].
Daytime biomarkers for physiology and behaviourReplicate physiological, cognitive and emotional outcome (bio-) markers, and validate more than once in different laboratories, in order that they be implemented as reliable markers for describing the effect of daylight on physiology and behaviour.
Devices to monitor daylightDevelop robust, validated and commercially affordable devices to monitor spectral daylight exposure (representing light exposure at eye level in a vertical direction) along with temperature and humidity continuously indoors and outdoors. The need to calibrate and correct outputs from light recordings has been shown mostly for wrist worn devices [229,230].
Application in different populationsThe questions discussed in Section 2 and Section 3 additionally need to be carried out in men and women, different ages (children, elderly) and ethnic groups, monitored across different environments (school/workplace/home); seasons; as well as in different patient groups (such as disorders of the nervous system, eyes).
Standard/exemplary data setsCollect and evaluate different data sets to assess how much and which qualities of daylight different groups of individuals/patients receive at their work- and living places (24/7).
Status quo in real life Define the status quo in daylight: irradiance measurements (including melanopic irradiance), interviews/focus groups with target populations (e.g., care home residents, prison inmates, dermatologists, ophthalmologists, shift- and night workers, tourist industry, people working underground (at e.g., train stations, miners), and related disciplines.
Table 9. Long-term objectives in daylight research.
Table 9. Long-term objectives in daylight research.
Laboratory and field studiesDo prospective longitudinal and multicentre studies using the established SOPs under field and laboratory conditions in a sufficiently large sample, performed in both males and females.
Combined day- and electric light interventionsCompare light interventions with daylight exposure, and not only electric light OR daylight but also the (dynamic) mixture of both should be studied, since this is the norm. Additionally, a suitable scheduled length of daily light exposure (daily accumulation) requires monitoring over a long duration (chronic/seasonal light exposure).
Guidelines/recommendations
for research and clinical studies
Propose guidelines/recommendations on how to set-up daylight research studies and clinical trials with daylight interventions.
Guidelines/recommendations
for short- and long-term interventions
Propose guidelines/recommendations on how to measure/monitor daylight interventions and related physiological and behavioural outcomes over time.
Tools for inter-individual differences
and circadian phase
Develop reliable tools to assess inter-individual differences and internal circadian phase for users and practitioners.
Instruments for light monitoring
in the field
Further validate instruments which monitor individual light and colour perception/preferences under daylight conditions in the field.
Daylight recommendations for task
and biological requirements
Develop daylight recommendations, which relate to both task requirements and physiological/psychological aspects.
EducationEducate professionals, government policy makers, and the public in ‘light hygiene’ e.g., sufficient vs. inadequate vs. too much daylight exposure.
Architecture/building scienceEarly stage planning of building/window positioning optimised for better daylight exposure and spatial-temporal modulation. Develop real mixed electric light/daylight simulation tools. Multimodal approach for optimised light exposure, heat emission, noise, air quality.
DatabaseCreate an open access daylight database (wiki/online depository).
ModellingDevelop mathematical models that help predict biologically appropriate daylight exposure (e.g., characteristics such as timing, dose, spectral composition and light exposure pattern) for promoting circadian entrainment, sleep, performance and health in addition to the required standards for visual function at the level of the individual as well as in different populations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Münch, M.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Brown, S.A.; Kantermann, T.; Martiny, K.; Stefani, O.; Vetter, C.; Wright, K.P., Jr.; Wulff, K.; Skene, D.J. The Role of Daylight for Humans: Gaps in Current Knowledge. Clocks & Sleep 2020, 2, 61-85. https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep2010008

AMA Style

Münch M, Wirz-Justice A, Brown SA, Kantermann T, Martiny K, Stefani O, Vetter C, Wright KP Jr., Wulff K, Skene DJ. The Role of Daylight for Humans: Gaps in Current Knowledge. Clocks & Sleep. 2020; 2(1):61-85. https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep2010008

Chicago/Turabian Style

Münch, Mirjam, Anna Wirz-Justice, Steven A. Brown, Thomas Kantermann, Klaus Martiny, Oliver Stefani, Céline Vetter, Kenneth P. Wright, Jr., Katharina Wulff, and Debra J. Skene. 2020. "The Role of Daylight for Humans: Gaps in Current Knowledge" Clocks & Sleep 2, no. 1: 61-85. https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep2010008

APA Style

Münch, M., Wirz-Justice, A., Brown, S. A., Kantermann, T., Martiny, K., Stefani, O., Vetter, C., Wright, K. P., Jr., Wulff, K., & Skene, D. J. (2020). The Role of Daylight for Humans: Gaps in Current Knowledge. Clocks & Sleep, 2(1), 61-85. https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep2010008

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop