Next Article in Journal
Wearable Light Loggers in Field Conditions: Corneal Light Characteristics, User Compliance, and Acceptance
Previous Article in Journal
Timing Mechanisms for Circadian Seizures
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sleep Fragmentation Modulates the Neurophysiological Correlates of Cognitive Fatigue

by
Oumaïma Benkirane
1,2,†,
Peter Simor
1,3,4,†,
Olivier Mairesse
2 and
Philippe Peigneux
1,*
1
UR2NF—Neuropsychology and Functional Neuroimaging Research Unit, at CRCN—Centre for Research in Cognition and Neurosciences and UNI—ULB Neuroscience Institute, Université Libre de Bruxelles (ULB), 1050 Brussels, Belgium
2
BBCO—Brain, Body and Cognition, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, 1050 Brussel, Belgium
3
Institute of Psychology, ELTE, Eötvös Loránd University, 1053 Budapest, Hungary
4
Institute of Behavioural Sciences, Semmelweis University, 1085 Budapest, Hungary
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors have contributed equally to this work.
Clocks & Sleep 2024, 6(4), 602-618; https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep6040041
Submission received: 23 July 2024 / Revised: 1 October 2024 / Accepted: 16 October 2024 / Published: 22 October 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Human Basic Research & Neuroimaging)

Abstract

:
Cognitive fatigue (CF) is a critical factor affecting performance and well-being. It can be altered in suboptimal sleep quality conditions, e.g., in patients suffering from obstructive sleep apnea who experience both intermittent hypoxia and sleep fragmentation (SF). Understanding the neurophysiological basis of SF in healthy individuals can provide insights to improve cognitive functioning in disrupted sleep conditions. In this electroencephalographical (EEG) study, we investigated in 16 healthy young participants the impact of experimentally induced SF on the neurophysiological correlates of CF measured before, during, and after practice on the TloadDback, a working memory task tailored to each individual’s maximal cognitive resources. The participants spent three consecutive nights in the laboratory two times, once in an undisrupted sleep (UdS) condition and once in an SF condition induced by non-awakening auditory stimulations, counterbalanced and performed the TloadDback task both in a high (HCL) and a low (LCL) cognitive load condition. EEG activity was recorded during wakefulness in the 5 min resting state immediately before and after, as well as during the 16 min of the TloadDback task practice. In the high cognitive load under a sleep-fragmentation (HCL/SF) condition, high beta power increased during the TloadDback, indicating heightened cognitive effort, and the beta and alpha power increased in the post- vs. pre-task resting state, suggesting a relaxation rebound. In the low cognitive load/undisturbed sleep (LCL/UdS) condition, low beta activity increased, suggesting a relaxed focus, as well as mid beta activity associated with active thinking. These findings highlight the dynamic impact of SF on the neurophysiological correlates of CF and underscore the importance of sleep quality and continuity to maintain optimal cognitive functioning.

1. Introduction

Continuous and high-quality sleep during the night is crucial for optimal brain and body functioning [1,2,3]. Conversely, sleep deprivation (SD) and/or poor sleep quality can have detrimental effects on both physical health and cognitive performance [4,5]. While there is considerable variation in individual sleep requirements [6], it is generally recommended that adults get an average of 7 h of sleep per night to avoid negative health consequences [7]. However, even those with adequate sleep duration may not experience efficient sleep [8], potentially due to various factors impacting sleep quality and continuity [9]. One factor that may affect sleep continuity is sleep fragmentation (SF), characterized by repeated interruptions during sleep that, even if not resulting in complete awakenings, prevent the brain from entering consolidated, slow-wave [10], and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. SF of these stages was shown to impair the restorative effects of sleep [11,12,13] and exert a detrimental influence on daytime cognitive functions [14,15]. Amongst others, SF is a prominent feature in sleep-related breathing disorders, particularly obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), characterized by recurrent episodes of airflow obstruction leading to brief arousals, intermittent hypoxemia, snoring, and SF [16]. Although studies demonstrated a specific impact of SF on cognitive performance in individuals with OSA [17], there is still an ongoing debate regarding the respective effects of hypoxemia and SF on the ensuing daytime neurocognitive deficits [17,18,19]. Nonetheless, SF is certainly a contributing factor to OSA-related disturbances, with neurocognitive deficits being commonly reported in individuals with OSA in whom the restorative effects of sleep are impaired [20,21]. Neurocognitive deficits predominantly impact vigilance [22], but also the performance of tasks that require continuous cognitive engagement, such as sustained attention and executive functions, and can also exert adverse effects on various aspects of memory, productivity, and social interactions [23,24,25,26,27]. In healthy individuals, SF induced by noise was shown to increase upper airway collapsibility and heart rate, which are characteristic features of OSA [28,29]. Since the independent contributions of SF and hypoxemia to cognitive deficits are hard to disentangle in pathological conditions such as OSA [17], studying the consequences of experimentally induced SF in healthy individuals may help to better understand the impact of disrupted restorative sleep mechanisms on cognitive processes and their underlying neurophysiological basis. In other terms, systematically examining the effects of experimentally induced and reversible SF in a healthy population may provide a valuable model to understand how and to what extent SF contributes to the cognitive deficits associated with OSA.
One cognitive symptom that is associated with both SF and sleep-related breathing disorders is cognitive fatigue (CF), subjectively experienced as a decrease in cognitive efficiency in situations involving sustained cognitive demands and constrained processing time, independently of sleepiness [30]. CF poses a significant concern, as it can lead to errors, impaired performance in daily activities, decreased efficiency, motivation loss, and an increased risk of accidents [31]. The terminology and definitions used to describe CF widely vary throughout the literature [32]. One of the most notable definitions comes from Grandjean [33], who describes cognitive fatigue as a state of reduced alertness that approaches, but does not reach, sleepiness. This state is induced by prolonged task execution and/or highly demanding tasks and can be mitigated by taking breaks. In this study, we adhere to Grandjean’s [33] definition, which characterizes CF as acute and non-pathological, induced by demanding or prolonged tasks or when processing time is limited in mentally demanding activities, leading to subjective feelings of fatigue, tiredness, reduced energy, and decreased performance [34]. Symptoms of cognitive fatigue include difficulties concentrating [35], memory issues [36], and a decline in executive functions [37]. Thus, CF is distinguished from sleepiness or drowsiness, which is characterized by a strong urge to sleep or a tendency to fall asleep, usually due to insufficient sleep, disrupted sleep patterns, or certain medical conditions [38]. Physical fatigue, on the other hand, involves a sensation of physical exhaustion and decreased physical performance caused by prolonged physical activity, lack of rest, or medical conditions affecting physical stamina and energy levels [39]. While cognitive fatigue, sleepiness, and physical fatigue are interrelated, they can occur independently or together, depending on factors such as lifestyle, mental or physical health status, and external stressors. Different countermeasures are typically needed to alleviate each condition, including active or passive rest for cognitive and physical fatigue, and sleep for sleepiness. However, sleep might also alleviate cognitive fatigue levels, although it may not always be effective, such as in cases of insomnia or chronic fatigue conditions [40].
Previous studies on CF mainly used tasks performed for extended periods, even up to several hours, with a constant level of cognitive demands, such as e.g., mental arithmetic calculations [41,42,43,44,45,46,47]. Hence, these studies examined the impact of high cognitive task demands on CF, with the underlying assumption that sustained cognitive demands will eventually deplete cognitive resources and increase CF levels [48,49]. Other studies investigated neural compensation within a fatigue framework, wherein participants engage in a taxing cognitive task for an extended duration [50], consistently triggering cognitive fatigue. Among young adults, as cognitive fatigue intensifies, behavioral performance decreases, with observable signs of neural compensation mitigating fatigue-induced cognitive deficits [51]. Recently, Borragán and collaborators [52] proposed that CF can also be understood within the framework of the time-based resource-sharing (TBRS) model [53]. According to this model, it is not the complexity of the task itself, but the time allotted for processing incoming stimuli that determines cognitive load and, subsequently, fatigue. In this framework, attention is viewed as a finite resource that varies among individuals, and an individual’s maximal cognitive load corresponds to the fastest pace at which he or she can still accurately meet task demands. Working continuously at maximal cognitive load will eventually result in increased CF. Modulation of CF by cognitive load level [52], as well as by sleep deprivation [54], duration [55], and fragmentation [56], have already been investigated using the TloadDback task [52], a cognitive load task designed to account for interindividual variability in working memory processing capabilities.
However, the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying CF and its interactions with prior sleep quality and/or continuity, as well as their combined impact on performance across several cognitive domains remain poorly investigated. Most cognitive fatigue research in neuroscience has concentrated on examining changes in EEG patterns [57,58], which are considered a promising biomarker of cognitive fatigue [59]. Evidence supports the notion that cognitive fatigue induces discernible alterations in EEG signals, as estimated through power spectral density (PSD) [60]. Within PSD, individual alpha frequency emerges as a significant metric. Situated within the alpha frequency range (7.5–12.5 Hz), it is indicative of arousal, attention, and cognitive performance. It serves as a stable neurophysiological marker and aids in detecting physical fatigue [31]. CF has also been found to be associated with changes in frontal theta activity, a reliable correlate of cognitive control [61]. Alterations in resting-state EEG power induced by cognitive fatigue may offer valuable insights into identifying its neural mechanisms [62]. Given the evidence that EEG changes serve as reliable indicators for detecting cognitive fatigue, EEG has emerged as one of the foremost technical methods to investigate neurobiological correlates and detect cognitive fatigue [57,58]. Previous studies have consistently reported specific EEG changes associated with cognitive effort and fatigue. Increased frontal theta power, for instance, is often linked to heightened cognitive control during demanding tasks [63], while beta oscillations are associated with sustained cognitive engagement and attention regulation [64]. Alpha desynchronization, observed during tasks requiring focused attention, is another marker of cognitive load, with greater desynchronization correlating with increased mental effort [65]. Delta power, although typically linked to sleep, has been observed to increase during intense cognitive fatigue, reflecting compensatory mechanisms in the brain [66].
In this context, we investigated, using electroencephalography (EEG) in healthy young adults, how experimentally induced SF exerts an impact on the neurophysiological correlates of CF at rest before, after, and during practice on the TloadDback working memory task, which allows for tailoring the cognitive load to each individual’s maximal capabilities. The participants spend three consecutive nights in the laboratory two times, once in an SF condition induced by non-awakening auditory stimulations, and once in a restorative, undisrupted sleep (UdS) condition, counterbalanced. Based on the previous literature, we hypothesize increased frontal theta power during the TloadDback task, reflecting the cognitive demands of the task. Similarly, we anticipate increased beta activity during the task, indicating sustained cognitive engagement despite fatigue. Alpha desynchronization is expected to signal cognitive load, and delta power may rise, particularly at rest, as a marker of residual fatigue from sleep fragmentation. These markers may contribute to our understanding of how sleep fragmentation modulates the neurophysiological correlates of cognitive fatigue.

2. Results

2.1. Resting-States EEG

The 5 min EEG data before (first resting state [RS1]) and after (second resting state [RS2]) the TloadDback task were analyzed within each condition (high (HCL)/low (LCL) cognitive load, and fragmented (SF)/undisturbed sleep (UdS). The results are separated by frequency band.

2.1.1. Theta Band (~4.75–6 Hz)

Significant differences emerged in the theta frequency range between pre- and post-task resting states in the high cognitive load and sleep-fragmentation condition (HCL/SF). Specifically, post-task resting EEG power increased in this frequency range. Topographical analyses, including all channels, indicated that these changes were widespread across the scalp (see Figure 1). No significant differences were observed in theta power in the undisturbed sleep condition (UdS), nor in the low cognitive load conditions after either SF or UdS.

2.1.2. Alpha Band (~9–12.25 Hz)

In the alpha frequency range, post-task resting EEG power also increased compared to pre-task activity under the high cognitive load and SF condition. This pattern was not observed under the UdS condition, nor in the low cognitive load conditions (see Figure 1). Changes in alpha power were widespread across channels and were statistically significant, with topographical analyses indicating scalp-wide involvement.

2.1.3. Beta Band (~13–19 Hz)

Significant changes in beta activity were observed in both the SF and UdS conditions but with distinct patterns. After SF and high cognitive load (HCL), the beta power (~18–19 Hz) significantly increased from pre- to post-task resting states. In the UdS condition, a similar increase was noted in the posterior low beta range (~13–14 Hz) after high cognitive load tasks (HCL), but no significant changes occurred under low cognitive load conditions for either SF or UdS. Topographical analyses highlighted the widespread distribution of these changes across the scalp (see Figure 1).
A comparison between the first resting state (RS1) following SF vs. UdS conditions, with the RS1 averaged across high and low cognitive load conditions, did not disclose any baseline difference, suggesting that identified differences at the second resting state (RS2) are linked to developing cognitive fatigue (CF) during the intermediate TloadDback task practice.

2.2. TloadDback EEG

The 16 min EEG data collected in each condition (HCL/LCL and SF/UdS) were segmented into four 4 min sections. To assess the evolution of fatigue markers throughout the task practice, the first and the last segments of the tasks were contrasted for each condition (HCL/LCL and SF/UdS).

Beta Band (~12.5–24 Hz)

As shown in Figure 2, we observed a significant difference in beta power (~14–24 Hz) between the first and the last segment of the task in the high cognitive load and sleep-fragmentation (HCL/SF) condition. More specifically, beta power exhibited a relative increase in the last segment of the task compared to the first segment. A similar increase in beta power, starting from the low beta range (~12.5–24 Hz) was found in the last compared to the first segment of the task in the low cognitive load and undisturbed sleep (LCL/UdS) condition. No significant differences emerged in the other conditions (see Figure 2).

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Participants

Sixteen young healthy participants (8 females; average 28.5 ± 4.48 years, range 24–38 years) were recruited through social media advertisements and flyers and provided written consent to take part in this study approved by the faculty and by the ULB-Erasme Hospital ethics committees (CE 001/2019). To avoid desirability bias, they were not informed of the exact purpose of the experiment and received a complete debriefing at the end of the study. Sleep or breathing disorders, irregular sleep patterns, extreme morning or evening chronotypes, habitual sleep duration of less than 6.5 h, neurological or psychiatric conditions, or a history of opioid treatment or current use of benzodiazepines were exclusion criteria. They were also instructed to refrain from consuming stimulating or alcoholic beverages on the day prior to the experimental days and during the experimental days.

3.2. General Experimental Procedure

The behavioral component of CF in this experiment is reported elsewhere [56], to which the reader is referred for a detailed presentation. In this study, a neuropsychological battery was administered after the first experimental night in each condition (sleep fragmentation and undisturbed sleep). It included verbal and visuospatial working memory assessments using the digit span (WAIS) and block-tapping tests. These tasks were administered to evaluate participants’ maximum memory capacities, with the digit span assessing both forward and reverse spans, and the block tapping measuring visuospatial memory. Other assessments included episodic memory (RLS-15), vigilance (PVT), inhibition (Stroop task), and verbal fluency. Detailed results are reported [56]. Only the essential information is reported here. The study spanned a duration of 17 days, as depicted in Figure 3. To confirm the absence of sleep or breathing disorders to facilitate habituation to the sleep lab conditions, participants were asked to spend the first night (Day 1) in the sleep laboratory under polysomnographic recordings (PSG) one week before the first experimental session, using SD LTM Express Morpheus system (Micromed, Mogliano Veneto, Italy) digital recorder operated with BrainRT software Suite Version 3.1 (Patch pack 5, build 4201) (OSG, Rumst, Belgium). The regularity of the sleep–wake pattern was controlled by using both a daily sleep diary and visual inspection of wrist-worn actigraphy (wGT3XBT Monitor, ActiGraph, Pensacola, FL, USA). On day 8, the participants went to the sleep laboratory for three consecutive nights under PSG recording, either in undisturbed sleep (UdS) or a sleep-fragmentation (SF) condition. After three nights of sleep at home, participants returned for three consecutive PSG nights in the other condition (SF or UdS). The order of UdS and SF conditions was counterbalanced across the participants. On the SF and UdS experimental nights, the participants arrived at the laboratory between 21:30 and 22:00 for PSG prepping. The lights were turned off between 23:00 and 24:00 depending upon individual sleep habits, as well as their habitual sleep duration (mean 531.43 ± 58.82 min). In the SF condition, auditory stimuli were presented during the three consecutive nights to induce sleep fragmentation (see below SF procedure), while in the UdS condition, the participants were allowed to sleep undisturbed for three consecutive nights. During the morning following each UdS and SF night (days 9 to 11 and 15 to 17), they completed the St Mary’s Sleep Questionnaire [67], which included 14 items to subjectively assess the quality of their previous night of sleep. In addition, throughout the experimental period, the participants filled out daily sleep diaries, allowing us to monitor subjective sleep onset latency, wake after sleep onset, sleep efficiency, total sleep time, and overall sleep and wake quality. The results of subjective sleep quality assessments are detailed in [56]. Following the initial nights of undisturbed sleep (UdS) and sleep fragmentation (SF) on days 9 and 15, the participants underwent a neuropsychological assessment battery and the calibration segment of the TloadDBack task, aimed at determining their peak working memory processing capacity (see below) [52]. Subsequently, during days 10–11 and 16–17, after the second and third UdS and SF nights, the participants completed each day the TloadDBack cognitive fatigue (CF) induction protocol under either high cognitive load (HCL) or low cognitive load (LCL) conditions in a within-subject, counterbalanced design (see Figure 4). Prior to and immediately following the TloadDBack practice (TTask), the participants’ EEGs were recorded during a quiet resting period. Those pre-task (resting-state 1 [RS1]), and post-task (resting-state 2 [RS2]) periods spent under EEG had a duration of five minutes, in which the participants were seated with their eyes open and instructed to fixate on a white cross on a black background. The 16 min TloadDback task (TTask) practice was also performed under EEG. Cognitive fatigue was subjectively evaluated using visual analog scales for fatigue severity (VASf [68]) and sleepiness (VASs) at each experimental phase (RS1, TTask, RS2), alongside visual analog scales for stress (VASst) and motivation (VASm) to account for potential confounds. To mitigate interindividual circadian variability, each participant was assessed at the same time of day across all UdS and SF, LCL, and HCL conditions.

3.3. Sleep-Fragmentation Procedure

Sleep fragmentation was induced using auditory stimulations at a frequency intended to simulate the sleep fragmentation experienced by patients with obstructive sleep apnea (for details, see [56]). The timing of the stimulations was personalized for each participant, considering their typical sleep duration. During each SF night, the initial sleep cycle remained undisturbed, after which arousing auditory tones were introduced through loudspeakers at random intervals ranging from 60 to 120 s to induce SF. The auditory tones randomly alternated between beep tones and firecracker sounds to prevent habituation to the sounds or their repetition. The auditory stimulation started at a low-intensity level and was gradually increased until microarousals were observed. If any signs of awakening were observed, as defined by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) criteria, the generation of auditory tones was manually paused until the participant resumed a deeper stage of sleep (N2, N3, or REM) for at least 2 min, after which the SF protocol was resumed. In both the SF and UdS nights, the participants were informed that auditory tones might be presented without the intention of waking them up but were not informed on which nights, or how many nights, it would occur. No information regarding the sleep condition was provided the following morning.

3.4. Cognitive Fatigue-Inducing TloadDback Task

The TloadDback task is a dual task that requires participants to perform an N-back working memory updating task and an odd/even number decision task simultaneously [52]. A detailed description of the task can be found elsewhere [56]. In a nutshell, 30 digits and 30 letters are displayed on a screen in alternating order (e.g., N–2–X-7–X–1–L…), and participants must press the space key with their left hand every time the displayed letter is the same as the previous letter (1-back task; e.g., …X-7–X…) and indicate whether the displayed digit is odd or even by pressing the “2” or “3” keys with their right hand. The cognitive resources required for the task were individually adjusted during separate calibration sessions that took place after the first SF or UdS night (Days 9 and 15). The participants completed a maximum of 20 blocks, with an initially comfortable interstimulus interval (ISI) of 1500 ms for the first block. If the participant achieved an accuracy of at least 85% at a block, then the ISI for the next block was decreased by 100 ms, making the task more challenging by allowing less processing time. This staircase procedure was repeated until the participant’s performance dropped below 85% over three consecutive blocks, indicating that they had reached their cognitive load limit. During the experimental conditions of SF or UdS (Days 10–11 and 16–17), the participants completed the TloadDback task at a constant ISI for 16 min either in an HCL or an LCL condition, counterbalanced. In the HCL condition, the ISI was set to the last successful ISI at the calibration session plus 100 ms. In the LCL condition, the ISI was one-third longer than in the HCL condition (ISI (LCL) = ISI (HCL) + 1/2 ISI (HCL)).

3.5. Electroencephalography

For the EEG recordings, the EEG signals on the second and third day of each sleep condition (SF and RS) were recorded, amplified, and digitized at a 256 Hz sampling rate with a 24-bit resolution with the 32-channel SD LTM Express Morpheus system (Micromed, Mogliano Veneto, Italy) digital recorder operated with BrainRT software Suite Version 3.1 (Patch pack 5, build 4201) (OSG, Rumst, Belgium). The PSG recordings included nine channels at scalp locations F4, Fz, F3, C4, Cz, C3, P3, Pz, and P4, each referenced to the ipsilateral mastoid and placed according to the international 10–20 system [69] and bipolar EOG and EMG channels. The impedance of the electrodes was kept below 5 kΩ. The participants’ skins were prepared according to standard procedures. All EEG recordings were exported as EDF+ files and preprocessed using the Fieldtrip toolbox Version 20221223 [70] on MATLAB (Version R2021b; The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). EEG data collection spanned 26 min, encompassing the 5 min prior to (RS1), the 16 min during (TTask), and the 5 min following (RS2) the completion of the TloadDback task.
For EEG processing, The raw EEG data were bandpass-filtered between 1 Hz and 30 Hz (with Butterworth, zero phase forward and reverse digital filter) to exclude low-frequency drifts and high-frequency noise. An independent component analysis (ICA) of the 5 min long recordings of the pre- and post-task resting states, as well as of the data collected during the 16 min long TloadDback task, was performed to identify eye-movement and muscular artifacts using Fieldtrip routines [70]. ICA was performed on the concatenated pre-and post-task resting states, and on the recordings during the TloadDback task, separately. Independent components (mostly one, maximum two), representing components linked to eye movements and muscular artifacts, were detected semi-automatically and were identified by inspecting the waveforms, as well as by their topographical distribution [71]. The 16 min TloadDback task was cut into 4 successive segments of 4 min each in order to investigate the neurophysiological differences between the beginning and the end of the task, linked to the gradual development of CF during task practice. Recordings were further segmented into 4 sec long trials and visually inspected to exclude the trials with remaining artifacts. Fast Fourier transformation was performed on Hanning tapered, non-overlapping 4 sec long trials with 0.25 Hz resolution to extract the averaged power spectral density of EEG activity in the pre- and post-task resting states, as well as in the TloadDback task. Absolute power values were computed between 1.5 Hz and 25 Hz.

3.6. Statistics

Multiple comparisons were performed to contrast bin-wise absolute power between RS1 (pre-task) and RS2 (post-task) within the same sleep condition (SF and UdS) and within the same cognitive load condition (HCL and LCL) to test the fatigue effect linked to cognitive load and sleep fragmentation. The first and last 4 min segments of each TloadDback condition were also compared across sleep (SF and UdS) and cognitive load (HCL and LCL) conditions. We expected that the HCL and SF conditions would induce changes in power spectral density (PSD). A sensitivity analysis was conducted to ensure that the sample size was adequate for detecting meaningful effects. Using a two-tailed t-test for dependent means, with an α error probability of 0.05, a power (1-β) of 0.80, and a total sample size of 16 participants, the critical t-value was determined to be 2.131. The required effect size (dz) was 0.75, confirming that the sample size was sufficient to detect moderate effects. Differences between conditions were evaluated by non-parametric bootstrapping (Monte Carlo simulation with 1000 shuffled samples) [72], as implemented in Fieldtrip [70]. Such an approach is suitable for the analyses of EEG signals, as it does not require assumptions of data distribution [72]. Two-tailed, paired t-tests were performed on all pairs of bin-wise absolute spectra (between 1.5–25 Hz) across two conditions. Spectral power values were averaged over the nine channels. To address the issue of multiple comparisons, spectral power differences along frequency ranges were considered to be significant if they spanned along a minimum of three consecutive bins (with a threshold p < 0.05), since there is a low probability of obtaining significant differences in adjacent bins by chance [73]. Significant bins were visualized but were not considered significant if they did not reach the threshold. To explore and highlight the topographical aspects of spectral power differences, we also visualized significant differences in the frequency x channel space.
In order to ensure the integrity of the data analysis, participants with excessively noisy data were excluded from the study. Therefore, when analyzing the TloadDback task, data from 15 participants were kept in the HCL and UdS conditions and in the LCL and SF conditions. Data from 14 participants were kept in the HCL and SF conditions and in the LCL and UdS conditions. When analyzing resting-state data, the 16 participants were kept in the HCL and SF conditions and in the LCL and SF conditions. Data from 15 participants were kept in the HCL and UdS and LCL and UdS conditions.

4. Discussion

In the present study, young healthy adults experienced three consecutive nights of auditory-induced sleep fragmentation (SF) and three nights of undisturbed sleep (UdS) in a counterbalanced design. We previously showed an effect of SF on sleep architecture, subjective appraisal of fatigue, and performance, even though the total sleep time was equivalent between the two conditions. Noticeably, our young adult participants proved able to compensate for the consequences of altered sleep continuity at the behavioral level in several cognitive domains [56]. Here, we analyzed CF- and SF-related changes in EEG during the resting states before (RS1) and after (RS2), and during the fatigue-inducing TloadDback task in high (HCL) and low (LCL) cognitive load conditions following either SF or UdS.
Beta activity in the resting state increased from before (RS1) to after (RS2) the TloadDback task in the high cognitive load (HCL) condition after sleep fragmentation (SF). Additionally, there was a notable rise in the high beta frequency band between the first and last segments of the TloadDback task in the HCL/SF and the LCL/UdS conditions. These findings suggest that, under conditions of high cognitive load combined with sleep fragmentation, the brain exhibits increased beta activity. Sleep deprivation consistently increases EEG beta activity, which correlates with impaired cognitive performance, emotional dysregulation, and changes in brain network organization [74,75]. Specifically, sleep deprivation was shown to lead to increased beta power during wakefulness and task performance, with the relative beta power being higher after sleep deprivation compared to normal sleep [76]. This increase is associated with slower reaction times and decreased vigilance, indicating that heightened beta activity reflects cognitive fatigue rather than improved performance [77]. In this context, a compensatory mechanism refers to the brain’s potential adaptation to cognitive fatigue, wherein increased beta activity may serve to maintain performance despite sleep deprivation. Furthermore, sleep deprivation has been shown to alter brain network organization, impacting cognitive function during wakefulness [78]. These findings reinforce the significance of monitoring EEG beta activity as a marker for cognitive and emotional impairments linked to sleep deprivation. To confirm whether increased beta activity is a compensatory response, future studies could measure the correlation between beta power and task performance metrics. Analyzing beta activity during varying cognitive load conditions could further elucidate its role, potentially revealing whether it increases as an adaptive response to maintain cognitive function.
Beta-band oscillations have been suggested to promote the maintenance of the current cognitive set, signaling the ‘status quo’ of the system, which resonates with our findings regarding increased beta activity under cognitive load (status quo theory). However, the dynamic nature of the beta activity may also encompass changes due to cognitive fatigue, as evidenced by previous studies highlighting alterations in the beta dynamics related to fatigue accumulation [79]. Beta rhythms have been often associated with active concentration, alertness, and top-down control mechanisms [80,81]. They are also linked to attention to impending processes and their execution [82]. Furthermore, beta rhythms have been found to encode information about task rules in the frontal cortex and are crucial for the activation of task sets in response to task demands [83]. This suggests that beta rhythm-related processes are essential to consider when examining the effects of fatigue during cognitive tasks, particularly those involving the initiation of motor processes following the presentation of target stimuli. Previous research has demonstrated a positive correlation between high-beta activity and the number of microsleep-associated errors, along with a decrease in the hit ratio. This has been interpreted as an effort to stay awake rather than an indication of increased alertness, which interferes with performance and leads to an increase in task-related errors [84]. Moreover, psychosocial stress induced by cognitive tasks has been shown to increase relative beta power during correct trials of an attentional task, correlating positively with anxiety and heart rate increase and inversely with attentional accuracy [85]. In conditions of low cognitive load and undisturbed sleep conditions, the increase in both low and high beta activity over the task duration suggests that the brain still engages in heightened cognitive processing, possibly to maintain performance levels [86]. These convergent results indicate a consistent pattern of beta band modulation in response to cognitive demands. Engagement in tasks of varying difficulty leads to an increase in beta oscillatory activity, highlighting the dynamic nature of beta rhythms in response to cognitive workload variations and sleep quality [87]. Given that cognitive fatigue builds up over time, conducting the same TloadDback task over different durations (e.g., 16 min vs. 30 min) could allow us to determine whether the observed beta increase reflects an adaptive effort to maintain performance or a compensatory mechanism due to fatigue. Although the 16 min version was proven to be efficient at inducing CF [52], a longer task duration may help to reveal more pronounced fatigue-related effects on performance and EEG markers.
Additionally, we found a trend towards positive changes in power frequencies between the first and second 4 min segments in the delta power band in the LCL/SF conditions. Delta oscillations, typically ranging from 0.5 to 4 Hz, are associated with deep sleep, and cognitive processing during sleep [88]. While the precise role of delta activity in cognitive fatigue is still under investigation, augmented delta power has been reported in conjunction with cognitive fatigue [89]. One possible interpretation of the trend towards positive changes in delta power between the first and second 4 min segments is that the brain may allow for some sleep-like activity in a less demanding task after SF. Durmer and Dinges [11] showed that cognitive fatigue, often induced by sleep deprivation, can lead to microsleep episodes. These brief lapses in consciousness, lasting from a fraction of a second to several seconds, are characterized by increased delta activity and reduced responsiveness to external stimuli. However, recent research highlights that delta power can also reflect cognitive effort. Mander and collaborators [90] found increased delta power during recovery sleep following sleep deprivation, suggesting a link between delta activity and cognitive recovery. Additionally, Vogel and collaborators [66] observed increased delta activity during intense cognitive effort in EEG studies, indicating that heightened delta power may reflect the brain’s attempt to compensate for fatigue by increasing cognitive effort. Furthermore, delta activity has been observed in memory-related tasks, potentially playing a role in cognitive processing and memory retrieval [91]. Studies by Zhang and collaborators [92], and Dimpfel and collaborators [93] similarly reported that delta power changes are associated with cognitive load and active engagement in cognitive tasks. It must be recognized that, while increased delta power may signal cognitive effort, it could also indicate cognitive overload or fatigue when sustained over time. For instance, Rosenblum and collaborators [94] found increased delta activity during concentration tasks, but also that excessive delta power may signify reduced cognitive efficiency. This dual interpretation of delta power, as both a marker of cognitive engagement and a sign of the brain’s struggle to maintain performance under demanding conditions, aligns with the subjective reports of higher baseline fatigue after SF and the larger increase following UdS [56]. Further studies introducing rest periods between tasks could help to distinguish whether delta power increases are primarily driven by cognitive fatigue or the demands of ongoing task engagement.
It is only during the resting state following the more demanding cognitive task after sleep fragmentation (HCL/SF conditions) that alpha waves increase. This may indicate a rebound effect where the brain, after being taxed by high cognitive load and fragmented sleep, shifts toward a more relaxed state once the task is completed. This aligns with the theory that alpha oscillations are markers of relaxation and reduced sensory processing [95]. Moreover, the psychosocial stress linked to demanding cognitive tasks has been shown to induce an increase in alpha power synchronization [85]. Previous studies suggest that higher beta frequencies, along with alpha activity during eyes-open tasks, increase as a result of sleep-resistance mechanisms [84]. In the EEG signal, cognitive fatigue typically leads to an increase in parietal alpha power [41], indicating a shift from active attention to the default mode, and a suppression of external stimulus processing [95,96]. This rise in alpha activity is often paired with an elevation in frontal theta power, which indicates cognitive control, task execution, memory function, and error processing [97]. Increasing theta activity is believed to indicate greater exertion to manage heightened cognitive demands during extended tasks. Furthermore, heightened power in slow frequency bands is linked to diminished performance, underscoring that prolonged cognitive fatigue can degrade task performance over time [32]. The trend observed in our study toward increased theta activity between the first and last segments of the TloadDback task in the HCL/SF condition may reflect a compensatory mechanism, as theta waves are not only indicative of working memory and cognitive load processing [95], but also suggest adaptive brain functioning. This increase in theta oscillations could signify an effort to maintain cognitive performance despite the challenges posed by sleep fragmentation and high cognitive load, potentially highlighting the brain’s adaptive response to mitigate the effects of cognitive fatigue.
Previous publications reported augmented delta and theta activity accompanying fatigue, alongside diminished beta activity concurrent with declining performance, vigilance, and sustained attention [89,95,98,99,100]. It suggests that increased delta and theta oscillations may reflect the neurophysiological manifestations of cognitive fatigue. Changes in delta power also align with behavioral outcomes, indicating a decline in performance over practice time, emphasizing the complex relationship between neural markers and cognitive fatigue manifestations [101]. In studies examining EEG patterns, the consistent findings include increases in delta and theta rhythms as drowsiness and fatigue build up, especially in the frontal and central areas. Increased theta activity, in particular, has been correlated with decrements in cognitive task performance [32]. EEG activity has been shown to be highly sensitive to cognitive fatigue over extended periods, characterized by substantial increases in frontal theta and parietal alpha power, even preceding noticeable performance decline. Our CF-inducing task, although relatively short (16 min), has been carefully designed to induce cognitive fatigue effectively. This design was informed by pretest calibrations tailored to each participant’s specific cognitive abilities, ensuring that the task reliably induces fatigue within the given timeframe.
In line with other studies [102,103,104], our previous research demonstrated that sleep fragmentation, while maintaining total sleep duration, minimally impacts behavioral performance across various cognitive domains [56]. However, neurophysiological measures reveal heightened sensitivity to even moderate sleep disruptions [104]. The current findings underscore the significant impact of cognitive load and sleep quality on neural activity patterns, highlighting the intricate interplay between cognitive demands, brain oscillations, and cognitive fatigue [105]. Specifically, alpha and theta rhythms appear pivotal in the context of cognitive fatigue and task performance, suggesting their potential as neural markers for monitoring cognitive workload and fatigue levels [106]. Moreover, increases in parietal beta power have been associated with emerging cognitive fatigue [107,108]. Tanaka and collaborators [62] propose that the observed enhancements in beta and alpha power densities during cognitive fatigue may signify a decline in multi-modal, high-level information processing within the central nervous system. Consequently, integrating real-time cognitive fatigue-monitoring methods, such as continuous EEG analysis, could enable researchers and practitioners to gain deeper insights into the effects of sleep quality on cognitive performance and fatigue dynamics. Continuous monitoring could also facilitate the development of individualized cognitive interventions, leading to more effective strategies to mitigate cognitive fatigue in both clinical and non-clinical populations. The implications of this research are substantial, particularly for fields such as occupational health and educational settings, where cognitive fatigue can impact performance and productivity.
In contrast to the significant changes observed under the high cognitive load (HCL) and sleep-fragmentation (SF) conditions, we did not observe significant changes in alpha, beta, or theta power under the low cognitive load (LCL) conditions following sleep fragmentation (SF) and high cognitive load (HCL) conditions following undisturbed sleep (UdS), suggesting that the interplay between cognitive load and sleep quality may differentially impact neural activity patterns. The lack of observable changes in these conditions may indicate that compensatory brain mechanisms are more effective when either cognitive load is lower or sleep quality is higher, allowing for the maintenance of more stable EEG activity. This stability may reflect an adaptive response, where the brain is better able to cope with cognitive demands under less stressful conditions, or when undisturbed sleep has mitigated the adverse effects of sleep fragmentation. It highlights the importance of considering both cognitive load and sleep quality together, as their interaction appears to play a crucial role in influencing neural activity and cognitive performance.

5. Limitations

While our study offers valuable insights into the impact of sleep fragmentation on cognitive fatigue and its neurophysiological correlates, several limitations warrant consideration. Our sample, composed of young and healthy adults, may restrict the generalizability of our findings to broader populations, especially in pathological contexts. Future research could build upon these findings by examining clinical populations, such as individuals with long-COVID cognitive fatigue, multiple sclerosis, or chronic fatigue syndrome, to further explore the impact of SF in pathological contexts.
Additionally, the experimental induction of sleep fragmentation may not fully replicate real-world sleep disturbances that usually span over much longer periods of time, potentially limiting the ecological validity of our results. Finally, the cognitive load task design and EEG measurements, while carefully constructed, may not fully capture the complexity of everyday cognitive demands and neural responses. Despite these limitations, our study provides a foundation for future research to explore these phenomena in more diverse and ecologically valid contexts.

6. Conclusions

Our EEG study examined the effects of sleep quality and cognitive load on the neurophysiological markers of cognitive fatigue in young adults. Significant alterations in EEG activity were observed under high cognitive load (HCL) and sleep-fragmentation (SF) conditions, with increased beta and alpha activities indicating heightened cognitive effort and relaxation rebound, respectively. A trend for increased theta activity suggested compensatory mechanisms to maintain performance. In the low cognitive load (LCL) and undisturbed sleep (UdS) conditions, increases in low beta power density suggest that the brain engages in heightened cognitive processing, even under less demanding circumstances, to maintain performance levels. These findings underscore the roles of alpha and theta rhythms in cognitive fatigue and task performance, suggesting their potential as neural markers for monitoring cognitive workload. Overall, the study highlights the interplay between sleep quality and cognitive load, emphasizing their combined impact on cognitive performance and neural activity. These insights may contribute to understanding cognitive fatigue mechanisms and offer implications for managing fatigue in conditions like sleep apnea.

Author Contributions

All of the authors conceived and contributed to the design of the experiments and the writing of the manuscript and approved it in its final form. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

O.B. was supported by the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique (F.R.S.-F.N.R.S., Aspirant Research Fellowship). P.S. received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Marie Sklodowska–Curie Actions COFUND IF@ULB Grant 801505, and by the (Hungarian) National Research, Development, and Innovation Office Grant NKFI FK 128100. The study was also partially supported by the F.N.R.S. and the Fonds Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek—Vlaanderen (F.W.O.) under the Excellence of Science (EOS) Project (MEMODYN, No. 30446199 to P.P.).

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Faculty and ULB-Erasme Hospital Ethics Committees (CE 001/2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data are accessible from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Xie, L.; Kang, H.; Xu, Q.; Chen, M.J.; Liao, Y.; Thiyagarajan, M.; O’Donnell, J.M.; Christensen, D.J.; Nicholson, C.; Iliff, J.J.; et al. Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science 2013, 342, 373–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Jiang, Y.; Chai, Y.; Yang, F.; Xu, S.; Basner, M.; Detre, J.A.; Dinges, D.F.; Rao, H. Effects of Sleep Deprivation and Recovery Sleep on Human Brain Network Organization. Sleep 2018, 41 (Suppl. S1), A85–A86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Laharnar, N.; Fatek, J.; Zemann, M.; Glos, M.; Lederer, K.; Suvorov, A.V.; Demin, A.V.; Penzel, T.; Fietze, I. A sleep intervention study comparing effects of sleep restriction and fragmentation on sleep and vigilance and the need for recovery. Physiol. Behav. 2020, 215, 112794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Killgore WD, S. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog. Brain Res. 2010, 185, 105–129. [Google Scholar]
  5. Sharma, S.; Kavuru, M. Sleep and Metabolism: An Overview. Int. J. Endocrinol. 2010, 2010, 270832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Hirshkowitz, M.; Whiton, K.; Albert, S.M.; Alessi, C.; Bruni, O.; DonCarlos, L.; Hazen, N.; Herman, J.; Katz, E.S.; Kheirandish-Gozal, L.; et al. National Sleep Foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: Methodology and results summary. Sleep Health 2015, 1, 40–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Watson, N.F.; Badr, M.S.; Belenky, G.; Bliwise, D.L.; Buxton, O.M.; Buysse, D.; Dinges, D.F.; Gangwisch, J.; Grandner, M.A.; Kushida, C.; et al. Recommended amount of sleep for a healthy adult: A joint consensus statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society. Sleep 2015, 38, 843–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Touzet, C. Cognitive fatigue: An impaired cortical inhibitory replenishment. Brain Inj. 2017, 31, 1625–1631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ohayon, M.M. Prévalence et comorbidité des troubles du sommeil dans la population générale. Rev. Prat. 2007, 57, 1521–1528. [Google Scholar]
  10. Philip, P.; Stoohs, R.; Guilleminault, C. Sleep fragmentation in normals: A model for sleepiness associated with upper airway resistance syndrome. Sleep 1994, 17, 242–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Durmer, J.S.; Dinges, D.F. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin. Neurol. 2005, 25, 117–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Riemann, D.; Spiegelhalder, K.; Nissen, C.; Hirscher, V.; Baglioni, C.; Feige, B. REM sleep instability–a new pathway for insomnia? Pharmacopsychiatry 2012, 45, 167–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Ren, R.; Zhang, Y.; Yang, L.; Shi, Y.; Covassin, N.; Tang, X. Sleep fragmentation during rapid eye movement sleep and hypertension in obstructive sleep apnea. J. Hypertens. 2023, 41, 310–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Stepanski, E.J. The Effect of Sleep Fragmentation on Daytime Function. Sleep 2002, 25, 268–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Nair, D.; Zhang, S.X.; Ramesh, V.; Hakim, F.; Kaushal, N.; Wang, Y.; Gozal, D. Sleep fragmentation induces cognitive deficits via nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phosphate oxidase–dependent pathways in mouse. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2011, 184, 1305–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Mannarino, M.R.; Di Filippo, F.; Pirro, M. Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Eur. J. Intern. Med. 2012, 23, 586–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Alomri, R.M.; Kennedy, G.A.; Wali, S.O.; Ahejaili, F.; Robinson, S.R. Differential associations of hypoxia, sleep fragmentation, and depressive symptoms with cognitive dysfunction in obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep 2021, 44, zsaa213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Colt, H.G.; Haas, H.; Rich, G.B. Hypoxemia vs sleep fragmentation as cause of excessive daytime sleepiness in obstructive sleep apnea. Chest 1991, 100, 1542–1548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Valencia-Flores, M.; Mokhlesi, B.; Santiago-Ayala, V.; Reséndiz-García, M.; Castaño-Meneses, A.; Meza-Vargas, M.S.; Mendoza, A.; Orea-Tejeda, A.; García-Ramos, G.; Aguilar-Salinas, C.; et al. Intermittent hypoxemia and sleep fragmentation: Associations with daytime alertness in obese sleep apnea patients living at moderate altitude. Sleep Med. 2016, 20, 103–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Verstraeten, E. Neurocognitive effects of obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 2007, 7, 161–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Krysta, K.; Bratek, A.; Zawada, K.; Stepańczak, R. Cognitive deficits in adults with obstructive sleep apnea compared to children and adolescents. J. Neural Transm. 2017, 124 (Suppl. S1), 187–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Daurat, A.; Sarhane, M.; Tiberge, M. Syndrome d’apnées obstructives du sommeil et cognition: Une revue. Neurophysiol. Clin. 2016, 46, 201–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Csábi, E.; Benedek, P.; Janacsek, K.; Katona, G.; Nemeth, D. Sleep disorder in childhood impairs declarative but not nondeclarative forms of learning. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2013, 35, 677–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Daurat, A.; Foret, J.; Bret-Dibat, J.L.; Fureix, C.; Tiberge, M. Spatial and temporal memories are affected by sleep fragmentation in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2008, 30, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Djonlagic, I.; Saboisky, J.; Carusona, A.; Stickgold, R.; Malhotra, A. Increased sleep fragmentation leads to impaired off-line consolidation of motor memories in humans. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e34106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Olaithe, M.; Bucks, R.S. Executive Dysfunction in OSA before and after Treatment: A Meta-Analysis. Sleep 2013, 36, 1297–1305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Weaver, T.E.; George, C.F.P. Cognition Performance in Patients with Obstructive Sleep Apnea. In Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 5th ed.; Kryger, M., Roth, T., Dement, W., Eds.; Elsevier Saunders: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2011; pp. 1194–1205. [Google Scholar]
  28. Sériès, F.; Roy, N.; Marc, I. Effects of sleep deprivation and sleep fragmentation on upper airway collapsibility in normal subjects. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 1994, 150, 481–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Griefahn, B.; Bröde, P.; Marks, A.; Basner, M. Autonomic arousals related to traffic noise during sleep. Sleep 2008, 31, 569–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mairesse, O.; Damen, V.; Newell, J.; Kornreich, C.; Verbanck, P.; Neu, D. The Brugmann Fatigue Scale: An Analogue to the Epworth Sleepiness Scale to Measure Behavioral Rest Propensity. Behav. Sleep Med. 2019, 24, 437–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Aziezah, F.; Pratama, S.H.; Yulianti; Wahidah, E.; Haryanto, F. Suprijadi Characterization of Individual Alpha Frequency of EEG Signals as an Indicator of Cognitive Fatigue. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 2020, 1505, 012068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hamann, A.; Carstengerdes, N. Assessing the development of mental fatigue during simulated flights with concurrent EEG-fNIRS measurement. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 4738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Grandjean, E. Fatigue in industry. Br. J. Ind. Med. 1979, 36, 175–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Marcora, S.M.; Staiano, W.; Manning, V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 2009, 106, 857–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Holtzer, R.; Shuman, M.; Mahoney, J.R.; Lipton, R.; Verghese, J. Cognitive fatigue defined in the context of attention networks. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Sect. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2011, 18, 108–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Massar, S.A.A.; Wester, A.E.; Volkerts, E.R.; Kenemans, J.L. Manipulation specific effects of mental fatigue: Evidence from novelty processing and simulated driving. Psychophysiology 2010, 47, 1119–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kato, Y.; Endo, H.; Kizuka, T. Mental fatigue and impaired response processes: Eventrelated brain potentials in a Go/NoGo task. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2009, 72, 204–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Neu, D.; Mairesse, O.; Hoffmann, G.; Valsamis, J.-B.; Verbanck, P.; Linkowski, P.; Le Bon, O. Do “sleepy” and “tired” go together? Rasch analysis of the relationships between sleepiness, fatigue and nonrestorative sleep complaints in a nonclinical population sample. Neuroepidemiology 2010, 35, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Stone, M.H.; Pierce, K.; Wilson, G.D.; Rozenek, R. Heart rate and lactate levels during weight-training exercise in trained and untrained men. Physician Sportsmed. 1987, 15, 97–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Neu, D.; Kajosch, H.; Peigneux, P.; Verbanck, P.; Linkowski, P.; Le Bon, O. Cognitive impairment in fatigue and sleepiness associated conditions. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 189, 128–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Trejo, L.; Kubitz, K.; Rosipal, R.; Kochavi, R.; Montgomery, L. EEG-Based Estimation and Classification of Mental Fatigue. Psychology 2015, 6, 572–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lorist, M.M.; Klein, M.; Nieuwenhuis, S. Mental fatigue and task control: Planning and preparation. Psychophysiology 2000, 37, 614–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Lorist, M.M.; Boksem, M.S.; Ridderinkhof, K.R. Impaired cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Brain Research. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005, 24, 199–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Mizuno, K.; Tanaka, M.; Fukuda, S.; Imai-Matsumura, K.; Watanabe, Y. Relationship between cognitive functions and prevalence of fatigue in elementary and junior high school students. Brain Dev. 2011, 33, 470–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Van der Linden, D.; Frese, M.; Sonnentag, S. The impact of mental fatigue on exploration in a complex computer task: Rigidity and loss of systematic strategies. Hum. Factors 2003, 45, 483–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Ackerman, P.L.; Kanfer, R. Test Length and Cognitive Fatigue: An Empirical Examination of Effects on Performance and Test-Taker Reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2009, 15, 163–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Lim, J.; Wu, W.; Wang, J.; Detre, J.A.; Dinges, D.F. Perfusion Study of the Time-on-Task Effect. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 3426–3435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Cook, D.B.; O’Connor, P.J.; Lange, G.; Steffener, J. Functional neuroimaging correlates of mental fatigue induced by cognition among chronic fatigue syndrome patients and controls. NeuroImage 2007, 36, 108–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Shigihara, Y.; Tanaka, M.; Ishii, A.; Kanai, E.; Funakura, M.; Watanabe, Y. Two types of mental fatigue affect spontaneous oscillatory brain activities in different ways. Behav. Brain Funct. 2013, 9, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Wang, C.; Ding, M.; Kluger, B.M. Change in intraindividual variability over time as a key metric for defining performance-based cognitive fatigability. Brain Cogn. 2014, 85, 251–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Wang, C.; Trongnetrpunya, A.; Samuel, I.B.H.; Ding, M.; Kluger, B.M. Compensatory neural activity in response to cognitive fatigue. J. Neurosci. 2016, 36, 3919–3924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Borragán, G.; Slama, H.; Bartolomei, M.; Peigneux, P. Cognitive fatigue: A Time-based Resource-sharing account. Cortex 2017, 89, 71–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Barrouillet, P.; Camos, V. As Time Goes By: Temporal Constraints in Working Memory. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 413–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Borragán, G.; Guerrero-Mosquera, C.; Guillaume, C.; Slama, H.; Peigneux, P. Decreased prefrontal connectivity parallels cognitive fatigue-related performance decline after sleep deprivation. Opt. Imaging Study. Biol. Psychol. 2019, 144, 115–124. [Google Scholar]
  55. Borragán, G.; Gilson, M.; Atas, A.; Slama, H.; Lysandropoulos, A.; De Schepper, M.; Peigneux, P. Cognitive Fatigue, Sleep and Cortical Activity in Multiple Sclerosis Disease. A Behavioral, Polysomnographic and Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Investigation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Benkirane, O.; Delwiche, B.; Mairesse, O.; Peigneux, P. Impact of Sleep Fragmentation on Cognition and Fatigue. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 15485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Li, G.; Huang, S.; Xu, W.; Jiao, W.; Jiang, Y.; Gao, Z.; Zhang, J. The impact of mental fatigue on brain activity: A comparative study both in resting state and task state using EEG. BMC Neurosci. 2020, 21, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Li, J.; Zhang, Z.; Wang, Y.; Zhou, G.; Wang, L. EEG beta power and corticomuscular coherence as a mechanism for effective brain-computer interface control. Front. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 22. [Google Scholar]
  59. Tran, Y.; Craig, A.; Craig, R.; Chai, R.; Nguyen, H. The influence of mental fatigue on brain activity: Evidence from a systematic review with meta-analyses. Psychophysiology 2020, 57, e13554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Tian, S.; Wang, Y.; Dong, G.; Pei, W.; Chen, H. Mental fatigue estimation using EEG in a vigilance task and resting states. In Proceedings of the Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Honolulu, HI, USA, 18–21 July 2018; pp. 1980–1983. [Google Scholar]
  61. Wascher, E.; Rasch, B.; Sänger, J.; Hoffmann, S.; Schneider, D.; Rinkenauer, G.; Heuer, H.; Gutberlet, I. Frontal theta activity reflects distinct aspects of mental fatigue. Biol. Psychol. 2014, 96, 57–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Tanaka, M.; Shigihara, Y.; Funakura, M.; Kanai, E.; Watanabe, Y. Fatigue-Associated Alterations of Cognitive Function and Electroencephalographic Power Densities. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e34774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Arnau, S.; Liegel, N.; Wascher, E. Frontal midline theta power during the cue-target-interval reflects increased cognitive effort in rewarded task-switching. Cortex 2024, 180, 94–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Balconi, M.; Acconito, C.; Allegretta, R.A.; Crivelli, D. What is the relationship between metacognition and mental effort in executive functions? the contribution of neurophysiology. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. So, W.K.Y.; Wong, S.S.; Mak, J.N.; Chan, R.H.M. An evaluation of mental workload with frontal eeg. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0174949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Vogel, W.; Broverman, D.M.; Klaiber, E.L. EEG and mental abilities. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1968, 24, 166–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Ellis, B.W.; Johns, M.W.; Lancaster, R.; Raptopoulos, P.; Angelopoulos, N.; Priest, R.G. The St. Mary’s Hospital sleep questionnaire: A study of reliability. Sleep 1981, 4, 93–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Lee, K.A.; Hicks, G.; Nino-Murcia, G. Validity and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue. Psychiatry Res. 1991, 36, 291–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Keil, A.; Debener, S.; Gratton, G.; Junghöfer, M.; Kappenman, E.S.; Luck, S.J.; Yee, C.M. Committee report: Publication guidelines and recommendations for studies using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography. Psychophysiology 2014, 51, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Oostenveld, R.; Fries, P.; Maris, E.; Schoffelen, J.-M. FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011, 2011, 156869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Campos Viola, F.; Thorne, J.; Edmonds, B.; Schneider, T.; Eichele, T.; Debener, S. Semi-automatic identification of independent components representing EEG artifact. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009, 120, 868–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Maris, E.; Oostenveld, R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 2007, 164, 177–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Tarokh, L.; Carskadon, M.A.; Achermann, P. Developmental changes in brain connectivity assessed using the sleep EEG. Neuroscience 2010, 171, 622–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Corsi-Cabrera, M.; Ramos, J.; Arce, C.; Guevara, M.A.; Ponce de León, M.; Lorenzo, I. Changes in the waking EEG as a consequence of sleep and sleep deprivation. Sleep 1992, 15, 550–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Zhang, J.; Lau, E.Y.Y.; Hsiao, J.H. Sleep deprivation compromises resting-state emotional regulatory processes: An EEG study. J. Sleep Res. 2019, 28, e12671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Lorenzo, I.; Ramos, J.; Arce, C.; Guevara, M.A.; Corsi-Cabrera, M. Effect of total sleep deprivation on reaction time and waking EEG activity in man. Sleep 1995, 18, 346–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Corsi-Cabrera, M.; Arce, C.; Ramos, J.; Lorenzo, I.; Guevara, M.A. Time course of reaction time and EEG while performing a vigilance task during total sleep deprivation. Sleep 1996, 19, 563–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Miraglia, F.; Tomino, C.; Vecchio, F.; Gorgoni, M.; De Gennaro, L.; Rossini, P.M. The brain network organization during sleep onset after deprivation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2020, 131, 2841–2851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Matta, P.-M.; Baurès, R.; Duclay, J.; Alamia, A. Modulation of beta oscillatory dynamics in motor and frontal areas during physical fatigue. bioRxiv 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Egner, T.; Gruzelier, J.H. EEG Biofeedback of low beta band components: Frequency-specific effects on variables of attention and event-related brain potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004, 115, 131–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Stoll, F.M.; Wilson, C.R.E.; Faraut, M.C.M.; Vezoli, J.; Knoblauch, K.; Procyk, E. The effects of cognitive control and time on frontal beta oscillations. Cereb. Cortex 2016, 26, 1715–1732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Khanna, P.; Carmena, J.M. Neural oscillations: Beta band activity across motor networks. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2015, 32, 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Spitzer, B.; Haegens, S. Beyond the status quo: A role for beta oscillations in endogenous content (re)activation. eNeuro 2017, 4, ENEURO.0170-17.2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Mairesse, O.; Neu, D.; Rosseel, Y.; Van Acker, F.; Cluydts, R.; Theuns, P. Comparative sensitivity of outcome variables of a software-based behavioral sleep resistance task. Ind. Health 2009, 47, 80–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Palacios-García, I.; Silva, J.; Villena-González, M.; Campos-Arteaga, G.; Artigas-Vergara, C.; Luarte, N.; Rodríguez, E.; Bosman, C.A. Increase in beta power reflects attentional top-down modulation after psychosocial stress induction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 630813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Engel, A.K.; Fries, P. Beta-band oscillations—Signalling the status quo? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2010, 20, 156–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Bonnet, M.; Arand, D. Clinical effects of sleep fragmentation versus sleep deprivation. Sleep Med. Rev. 2003, 7, 297–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Steriade, M.; Nunez, A.; Amzica, F. A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: Depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J. Neurosci. 1993, 13, 3252–3265. [Google Scholar]
  89. Belyavin, A.; Wright, N.A. Changes in electrical activity of the brain with vigilance. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1987, 66, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Mander, B.A.; Reid, K.J.; Baron, K.G.; Tjoa, T.; Parrish, T.B.; Paller, K.A.; Gitelman, D.R.; Zee, P.C. Eeg measures index neural and cognitive recovery from sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 2686–2693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Michalopoulos, K.; Sakkalis, V.; Iordanidou, V.; Zervakis, M. Activity detection and causal interaction analysis among independent eeg components from memory related tasks. In Proceedings of the 2009 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, Minneapolis, MN, USA, 3–6 September 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Zhang, Y.; Ku, Y.; Sun, J.; Daskalakis, Z.J.; Yuan, T. Intermittent theta burst stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex improves working memory of subjects with methamphetamine use disorder. Psychol. Med. 2021, 53, 2427–2436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Dimpfel, W.; Tausend, S.; Suliman, S.; Dipah, G.N.C. Psychophysiological effectiveness of calmvalera hevert tablets as measured by enkephalovision in anxious subjects during audio-visual cognitive and emotional challenges: A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, 2-armed, phase iv study in parallel design. J. Behav. Brain Sci. 2016, 6, 404–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Rosenblum, Y.; Maidan, I.; Goldstein, O.; Gana-Weisz, M.; Orr-Urtreger, A.; Bregman, N.; Giladi, N.; Mirelman, A.; Shiner, T. Event-related oscillations in dementia with lewy bodies with a mutation in the gba gene. Res. Sq. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Klimesch, W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 1999, 29, 169–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Hanslmayr, S.; Gross, J.; Klimesch, W.; Shapiro, K.L. The role of α oscillations in temporal attention. Brain Res. Rev. 2011, 67, 331–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Sauseng, P.; Griesmayr, B.; Freunberger, R.; Klimesch, W. Control mechanisms in working memory: A possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 34, 1015–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Makeig, S.; Jung, T.P.; Sejnowski, T.J. Awareness during drowsiness: Dynamics and electrophysiological correlates. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 2000, 54, 266–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Oken, B.S.; Salinsky, M.C.; Elsas, S.M. Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: Physiological basis and measurement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006, 117, 1885–1901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Lal, S.K.L.; Craig, A. Driver fatigue: Electroencephalography and psychological assessment. Psychophysiology 2002, 39, 313–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Gorgoni, M.; D’Atri, A.; Scarpelli, S.; Ferrara, M.; Gennaro, L. The electroencephalographic features of the sleep onset process and their experimental manipulation with sleep deprivation and transcranial electrical stimulation protocols. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020, 114, 25–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Cote, K.A.; Milner, C.E.; Osip, S.L.; Ray, L.B.; Baxter, K.D. Waking quantitative electroencephalogram and auditory event-related potentials following experimentally induced sleep fragmentation. Sleep 2003, 26, 687–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Kingshott, R.N.; Cosway, R.J.; Deary, I.J.; Douglas, N.J. The effect of sleep fragmentation on cognitive processing using computerized topographic brain mapping. J. Sleep Res. 2000, 9, 353–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Ko, C.-H.; Fang, Y.-W.; Tsai, L.-L.; Hsieh, S. The effect of experimental sleep fragmentation on error monitoring. Biol. Psychol. 2015, 104, 163–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Cano, G.; Mochizuki, T.; Saper, C. Neural circuitry of stress-induced insomnia in rats. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 10167–10184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Yu, S.; Mückschel, M.; Beste, C. Event-related synchronization/desynchronization and functional neuroanatomical regions associated with fatigue effects on cognitive flexibility. J. Neurophysiol. 2021, 126, 383–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Dasari, D.; Crowe, C.; Ling, C.; Zhu, M.; Ding, L. EEG pattern analysis for physiological indicators of mental fatigue in simulated air traffic control tasks. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 2010, 54, 205–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Nguyen, T.; Ahn, S.; Jang, H.; Jun, S.C.; Kim, J.G. Utilization of a combined EEG/NIRS system to predict driver drowsiness. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 43933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Visualization of absolute power spectra during resting states across contrasted conditions. Panels highlight the comparison between resting-state EEG power before the TloadDback task (RS1) and after the TloadDback task (RS2) under high (HCL) and low (LCL) cognitive load conditions, under sleep fragmentation (SF) and undisturbed sleep (UdS) conditions. Inset heatmaps show channel-wise changes in the power spectra across frequencies for the specified conditions. Each heatmap displays the EEG channels (F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, and Cz) on the y-axis and frequency on the x-axis. Greyscale intensity indicates statistical significance of power change, with scale on the right-side axis (0 to 0.05). Significant differences at each frequency bin are marked with * (p < 0.05). To address multiple comparisons, spectral power differences along frequency ranges were considered significant only if they spanned along at least three adjacent bins.
Figure 1. Visualization of absolute power spectra during resting states across contrasted conditions. Panels highlight the comparison between resting-state EEG power before the TloadDback task (RS1) and after the TloadDback task (RS2) under high (HCL) and low (LCL) cognitive load conditions, under sleep fragmentation (SF) and undisturbed sleep (UdS) conditions. Inset heatmaps show channel-wise changes in the power spectra across frequencies for the specified conditions. Each heatmap displays the EEG channels (F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, and Cz) on the y-axis and frequency on the x-axis. Greyscale intensity indicates statistical significance of power change, with scale on the right-side axis (0 to 0.05). Significant differences at each frequency bin are marked with * (p < 0.05). To address multiple comparisons, spectral power differences along frequency ranges were considered significant only if they spanned along at least three adjacent bins.
Clockssleep 06 00041 g001
Figure 2. Visualization of absolute power spectra during the first and last segment of the TloadDback task across conditions. Figure panels illustrate power spectra in the first 4 min segment (Tload1) and the last 4 min segment (Tload4) of the TloadDback task under different cognitive load (HCL and LCL) and sleep (SF and UdS) conditions. Inset heatmaps show channel-wise changes in the power spectra across frequencies for the specified conditions. Each heatmap displays the EEG channels (F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, and Cz) on the y-axis and frequency on the x-axis. Greyscale intensity indicates statistical significance of power change, with a legend on the right showing the scale (0 to 0.05). Significant differences per frequency bin are marked with * (p < 0.05). However, to address for multiple comparisons, spectral power differences along frequency ranges were considered significant if they spanned along a minimum of three adjacent bins.
Figure 2. Visualization of absolute power spectra during the first and last segment of the TloadDback task across conditions. Figure panels illustrate power spectra in the first 4 min segment (Tload1) and the last 4 min segment (Tload4) of the TloadDback task under different cognitive load (HCL and LCL) and sleep (SF and UdS) conditions. Inset heatmaps show channel-wise changes in the power spectra across frequencies for the specified conditions. Each heatmap displays the EEG channels (F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, and Cz) on the y-axis and frequency on the x-axis. Greyscale intensity indicates statistical significance of power change, with a legend on the right showing the scale (0 to 0.05). Significant differences per frequency bin are marked with * (p < 0.05). However, to address for multiple comparisons, spectral power differences along frequency ranges were considered significant if they spanned along a minimum of three adjacent bins.
Clockssleep 06 00041 g002
Figure 3. Experimental protocol (see general experimental procedure). Note: PSG = polysomnography. VAS = visual analog scales (fatigue, sleepiness, stress, and motivation). LCL = low cognitive load condition. HCL = high cognitive load condition. Participants spent three consecutive nights both in a sleep fragmentation (SF) and in a sleep undisturbed (UdS) condition at a one-week interval, counterbalanced. Cognitive fatigue (CF) calibration (pretest) was performed after the first SF or UdS night, and CF assessment was conducted in HCL and LCL conditions after the second and third nights, counterbalanced.
Figure 3. Experimental protocol (see general experimental procedure). Note: PSG = polysomnography. VAS = visual analog scales (fatigue, sleepiness, stress, and motivation). LCL = low cognitive load condition. HCL = high cognitive load condition. Participants spent three consecutive nights both in a sleep fragmentation (SF) and in a sleep undisturbed (UdS) condition at a one-week interval, counterbalanced. Cognitive fatigue (CF) calibration (pretest) was performed after the first SF or UdS night, and CF assessment was conducted in HCL and LCL conditions after the second and third nights, counterbalanced.
Clockssleep 06 00041 g003
Figure 4. Timeline of resting states before and after the TloadDback task, performed under EEG. The TloadDback task was administered in both cognitive load conditions, counterbalanced, after SF/UdS nights 2 and 3.
Figure 4. Timeline of resting states before and after the TloadDback task, performed under EEG. The TloadDback task was administered in both cognitive load conditions, counterbalanced, after SF/UdS nights 2 and 3.
Clockssleep 06 00041 g004
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Benkirane, O.; Simor, P.; Mairesse, O.; Peigneux, P. Sleep Fragmentation Modulates the Neurophysiological Correlates of Cognitive Fatigue. Clocks & Sleep 2024, 6, 602-618. https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep6040041

AMA Style

Benkirane O, Simor P, Mairesse O, Peigneux P. Sleep Fragmentation Modulates the Neurophysiological Correlates of Cognitive Fatigue. Clocks & Sleep. 2024; 6(4):602-618. https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep6040041

Chicago/Turabian Style

Benkirane, Oumaïma, Peter Simor, Olivier Mairesse, and Philippe Peigneux. 2024. "Sleep Fragmentation Modulates the Neurophysiological Correlates of Cognitive Fatigue" Clocks & Sleep 6, no. 4: 602-618. https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep6040041

APA Style

Benkirane, O., Simor, P., Mairesse, O., & Peigneux, P. (2024). Sleep Fragmentation Modulates the Neurophysiological Correlates of Cognitive Fatigue. Clocks & Sleep, 6(4), 602-618. https://doi.org/10.3390/clockssleep6040041

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop