Causal Reasoning and Event Cognition as Evolutionary Determinants of Language Structure
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. What Is Special about Human Causal Cognition?
2.1. Non-Human Primate Reasoning about Causes
2.2. Human Reasoning about Forces
2.3. Tool Manufacture and Use Were Selective Factors for Reasoning about Forces
3. Event Cognition
3.1. A Cognitive Model of Events
3.2. Event Cognition, Planning and Episodic Memory
4. How Event Cognition Improves Communication
4.1. Levels of Communication
4.2. Referring to Absent Objects
4.3. Communication for Future Cooperation
Analogous to the way that manual tool use has been shown to enlarge the peripersonal space by extending the bodily action potential of arm and hand in space …, linguistic symbols liberate human interactions from the temporal and spatial immediacy of face-to-face and bodily coordination and thus radically expand the interaction space.
- (1)
- Simplifies otherwise difficult coordination problems, especially those involving many agents and planning for future events;
- (2)
- Reduces the cost of enforcing adherence to collectively beneficial norms;
- (3)
- Enhances the efficiency of signals, including those which provide collective goods;
- (4)
- Facilitates the positive assortment of individuals who adhere to similar norms and conventions.
4.4. Who Did What to Whom? The Adaptive Benefits of Gossip
5. From Events to Sentences
5.1. Sentences Are Needed for the Coordination of Common Knowledge
5.2. Noun Phrases and Verb Phrases
5.3. Other Word Classes
6. Conclusions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Hauser, M.D.; Chomsky, N.; Fitch, W.T. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 2002, 298, 1569–1579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chomsky, N. Derivation by Phase; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff, R. Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999, 3, 272–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heine, B.; Kuteva, T. The Genesis of Grammar: A Reconstruction; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S.; Lewin, R. Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Köhler, W. The Mentality of Apes; Penguin Books: Traverse City, MI, USA, 1917. [Google Scholar]
- Cacchione, T.; Call, J.; Zingg, R. Gravity and solidity in four great ape species (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus): Vertical and horizontal variations of the table task. J. Comp. Psychol. 2009, 123, 168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tomonaga, M.; Imura, T.; Mizuno, Y.; Tanaka, M. Gravity bias in young and adult chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Tests with a modified opaque-tubes task. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 411–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gärdenfors, P. Cued and detached representations in animal cognition. Behav. Process. 1996, 36, 263–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hockett, C.F. The origin of speech. Sci. Am. 1960, 203, 88–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanz, C.; Morgan, D.; Gulick, S. New insights into chimpanzees, tools, and termites from the Congo Basin. Am. Nat. 2004, 164, 567–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Decety, J.; Grèzes, J. The power of simulation: Imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Res. 2006, 1079, 4–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gärdenfors, P. How Homo Became Sapiens: On the Evolution of Thinking; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Grush, R. The architecture of representation. Philos. Psychol. 1997, 10, 5–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spreng, R.N.; Grady, C.L. Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 1112–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheney, D.; Seyfarth, R. Attending to behaviour versus attending to knowledge: Examining monkeys’ attribution of mental states. Anim. Behav. 1990, 40, 742–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Povinelli, D.J. Folk Physics for Apes: The Chimpanzee’s Theory of how the World Works; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Calvin, W.H.; Bickerton, D. Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the Human Brain; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Shaw-Williams, K. The social trackways theory of the evolution of human cognition. Biol. Theory 2014, 9, 16–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stuart-Fox, M. The origins of causal cognition in early hominins. Biol. Philos. 2014, 30, 247–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Völter, C.J.; Call, J. Great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii) follow visual trails to locate hidden food. J. Comp. Psychol. 2014, 128, 199–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Civelek, Z.; Call, J.; Seed, A.M. Inferring unseen causes: Developmental and evolutionary origins. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Call, J. Trapping the minds of apes: Causal knowledge and inferential reasoning about object-object interactions. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives; Lonsdorf, E.V., Ross, S.R., Matsuzawa, T., Goodall, J., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2010; pp. 75–86. [Google Scholar]
- Hanus, D.; Call, J. Chimpanzees infer the location of a reward on the basis of the effect of its weight. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, R370–R372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Martin-Ordas, G.; Call, J.; Colmenares, F. Tubes, tables and traps: Great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Anim. Cogn. 2008, 11, 423–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Penn, D.C.; Povinelli, D.J. On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2007, 362, 731–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Seed, A.M.; Call, J.; Emery, N.J.; Clayton, N.S. Chimpanzees solve the trap problem when the confound of tool-use is removed. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2009, 35, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Seed, A.M.; Hanus, D.; Call, J. Causal knowledge in corvids, primates and children: More than meets the eye? In Tool Use and Causal Cognition; McCormack, T., Hoerl, C., Butterfill, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 89–110. [Google Scholar]
- Povinelli, D.; Penn, D. Through a floppy tool darkly. In Tool Use and Causal Cognition; McCormack, T., Hoerl, C., Butterfill, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 69–97. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson-Frey, S.H. What’s so special about human tool use? Neuron 2003, 39, 201–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Boesch, C.; Bombjaková, D.; Boyette, A.; Meier, A. Technical intelligence and culture: Nut cracking in humans and chimpanzees. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2017, 163, 339–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hannah, A.C.; McGrew, W.C. Chimpanzees using stones to crack open oil palm nuts in Liberia. Primates 1987, 28, 31–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morgan, B.J.; Abwe, E.E. Chimpanzees use stone hammers in Cameroon. Curr. Biol. 2006, 16, R632–R633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Michotte, A. The Perception of Causality; Methuen: New York, NY, USA, 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Leslie, A.M.; Keeble, S. Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition 1987, 25, 265–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leslie, A.M. A theory of agency. In Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary Debate; Sperber, D., Premack, D., James Premack, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 121–141. [Google Scholar]
- Wolff, P. Representing causation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2007, 136, 82–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wolff, P. Dynamics and the perception of causal events. In Understanding Events: How Humans See, Represent, and Act on Events; Thomas, S., Zacks, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 555–587. [Google Scholar]
- Wolff, P. Representing verbs with force vectors. Theor. Linguist. 2012, 38, 237–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wolff, P.; Shepard, J. Causation, touch, and the perception of force. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Ross, B.H., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013; Volume 58, pp. 167–202. [Google Scholar]
- Wolff, P.; Thorstad, R. Force dynamics. In The Oxford Handbook of Causal Reasoning; Waldmann, M.R., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 147–167. [Google Scholar]
- Runeson, S. Perception of biological motion: The KSD-principle and the implications of a distal versus proximal approach. In Perceiving Events and Objects; Jansson, G., Bergström, S.S., Epstein, W., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1994; pp. 383–405. [Google Scholar]
- Göksun, T.; George, N.R.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R.M. Forces and motion: How young children understand causal events. Child Dev. 2013, 84, 1285–1295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- George, N.R.; Göksun, T.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R.M. Any way the wind blows: Children’s inferences about force and motion events. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 177, 119–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gärdenfors, P. Evolutionary and developmental aspects of intersubjectivity. In Consciousness Transitions: Phylogenetic, Ontogenetic and Physiological Aspects; Liljenström, H., Århem, P., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; pp. 281–305. [Google Scholar]
- Premack, D.; Woodruff, G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1978, 1, 515–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomasello, M. The human adaptation for culture. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 1999, 28, 509–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M.; Call, J.; Behne, T.; Moll, H. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005, 28, 675–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lombard, M.; Gärdenfors, P. Causal cognition and theory of mind in evolutionary cognitive archaeology. Biol. Theory 2021, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Lombard, M. Technology led to more abstract causal reasoning. Biol. Philos. 2020, 35, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cléry, J.; Guipponi, O.; Wardak, C.; Hamed, S.B. Neuronal bases of peripersonal and extrapersonal spaces, their plasticity and their dynamics: Knowns and unknowns. Neuropsychologia 2015, 70, 313–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maravita, A.; Iriki, A. Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004, 8, 79–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Calvin, W.H. The unitary hypothesis: A common neural circuitry for novel manipulations, language, plan-ahead, and throwing. In Tools, Language, and Cognition in Human Evolution; Gibson, K.R., Ingold, T., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1993; pp. 230–250. [Google Scholar]
- Preutz, J.D.; Bertolani, P. Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 412–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Roach, N.T.; Venkadesan, M.; Rainbow, M.J.; Lieberman, D.E. Elastic energy storage in the shoulder and the evolution of high-speed throwing in Homo. Nature 2013, 498, 483–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Lombard, M. Causal cognition, force dynamics and early hunting technologies. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Conard, N.J.; Serangeli, J.; Bigga, G.; Rots, V. A 300,000-year-old throwing stick from Schöningen, northern Germany, documents the evolution of human hunting. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2020, 4, 690–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hardy, B.L.; Moncel, M.; Daujeard, C.; Fernandes, P.; Béarez, P.; Desclaux, E.; Navarro, M.G.C.; Puaud, S.; Gallotti, R. Impossible Neanderthals? Making string, throwing projectiles and catching small game during Marine Isotope Stage 4 (Abri du Maras, France). Quat. Sci. 2013, 82, 23–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thieme, H. Lower Palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature 1997, 385, 807–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Milks, A.; Parker, D.; Pope, M. External ballistics of Pleistocene hand-thrown spears: Experimental performance data and implications for human evolution. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wilkins, J.; Schoville, B.J.; Brown, K.S.; Chazan, M. Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science 2012, 338, 942–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bradfield, J.; Wadley, L.; Lombard, M. Southern African arrow poison recipes, their ingredients and implications for Stone Age archaeology. S. Afr. Humanit. 2015, 27, 29–64. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, P. The Geometry of Meaning: Semantics Based on Conceptual Spaces; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Jost, J.; Warglien, M. From actions to events: Three constraints on event mappings. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Warglien, M. Using conceptual spaces to model actions and events. J. Semant. 2012, 29, 487–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radvansky, G.A.; Zacks, J.M. Event Cognition; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Warglien, M.; Gärdenfors, P.; Westera, M. Event structure, conceptual spaces and the semantics of verbs. Theor. Linguist. 2012, 38, 159–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dowty, D. Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language 1991, 67, 547–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levin, B.; Rappaport Hovav, M. Argument Realization; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Casati, R.; Varzi, A. Event concepts. In Understanding Events: From Perception to Action New; Shipley, T.F., Zacks, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: York, UK, 2008; pp. 31–54. [Google Scholar]
- Zacks, J.M.; Tversky, B. Event structure in perception and conception. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Osta Vélez, M. Generics as expectations. 2021, submitted. [Google Scholar]
- Granger, C.W. Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econom. J. Econom. Soc. 1969, 37, 424–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salmon, W.C. Scientific Explanation and the Causal Structure of the World; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Talmy, L. Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cogn. Sci. 1988, 12, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liebenberg, L. The Art of Tracking; David Philip Publishers: Cape Town, South Africa, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Lombard, M.; Gärdenfors, P. Tracking the evolution of causal cognition in humans. J. Anthropol. Sci. 2017, 95, 219–234. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Gulz, A. The Planning of Action as a Cognitive and Biological Phenomenon; Lund University Cognitive Studies 2: Lund, Sweden, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Osvath, M.; Gärdenfors, P. Oldowan Culture and the Evolution of Anticipatory Cognition; Lund University Cognitive Studies 122: Lund, Sweden, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Premack, D. Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 13861–13867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Suddendorf, T.; Corballis, M.C. The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behav. Brain Sci. 2007, 30, 299–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tulving, E. Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2002, 53, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mulcahy, N.J.; Call, J. How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task. Anim. Cogn. 2006, 9, 193–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Osvath, M.; Osvath, H. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: Self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Anim. Cogn. 2008, 11, 661–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osvath, M. Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing by a male chimpanzee. Curr. Biol. 2009, 19, R190–R191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Osvath, M. Great ape foresight is looking great. Anim. Cogn. 2010, 13, 777–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Raby, C.; Alexis, D.; Dickinson, A.; Clayton, N.S. Planning for the future by western scrub-jays. Nature 2007, 445, 919–921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Correia, S.P.; Dickinson, A.; Clayton, N.S. Western scrub-jays anticipate future needs independently of their current motivational state. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 856–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schacter, D.L.; Addis, D.R. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2007, 362, 773–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tulving, E. Memory and consciousness. Can. Psychol. 1985, 26, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, V.C.; Schacter, D.L.; Corballis, M.C.; Addis, D.R. A role for the hippocampus in encoding simulations of future events. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 13858–13863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bellmund, J.L.; Gärdenfors, P.; Moser, E.I.; Doeller, C.F. Navigating cognition: Spatial codes for human thinking. Science 2018, 362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kuhrt, D.; John, N.R.S.; Bellmund, J.L.; Kaplan, R.; Doeller, C.F. An immersive first-person navigation task for abstract knowledge acquisition. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coolidge, F.L.; Haidle, M.N.; Lombard, M.; Wynn, T. Bridging theory and bow hunting: Human cognitive evolution and archaeology. Antiquity 2016, 90, 219–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rafetseder, E.; Schwitalla, M.; Perner, J. Counterfactual reasoning: From childhood to adulthood. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 114, 389–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Markovits, H. On the road toward formal reasoning: Reasoning with factual causal and contrary-to-fact causal premises during early adolescence. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 128, 37–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baird, A.A.; Fugelsang, J.A. The emergence of consequential thought: Evidence from neuroscience. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2004, 359, 1797–1804. [Google Scholar]
- Wolff, P.; Barbey, A.K.; Hausknecht, M. For want of a nail: How absences cause events. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2010, 139, 191–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clark, H.H. Using Language; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Brinck, B.; Osvath, M. Coevolution of cooperation, cognition and communication. In New Perspectives of the Symbolic Species; Stjernfelt, F., Deacon, T., Schilhab, T., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2012; pp. 193–222. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Osvath, M. Prospection as a cognitive precursor to symbolic communication. In Evolution of Language: Biolinguistic Approaches; Larson, R.K., Déprez, V., Yamakido, H., Eds.; Cambridge University: Cambridge, UK, 2010; pp. 103–114. [Google Scholar]
- Morrison, D.M. Disambiguated indexical pointing as a tipping point for the explosive emergence of language among human ancestors. Biol. Theory 2020, 15, 196–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M.; Liszkowski, U. A new look at infant pointing. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 705–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lyn, H.; Russell, J.L.; Leavens, D.A.; Bard, K.A.; Boysen, S.T.; Schaeffer, J.A.; Hopkins, W.D. Apes communicate about absent and displaced objects: Methodology matters. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 85–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bates, E.; Camaioni, L.; Volterra, V. The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill Palmer Q. Behav. Dev. 1975, 21, 205–224. [Google Scholar]
- Bühler, K. Sprachtheorie: Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache; Fischer: Stuttgart, Germany, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Haviland, J.B. Pointing, gesture space and mental maps. In Language and Gesture; McNeill, D., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000; pp. 13–46. [Google Scholar]
- McNeill, D. Gesture and Thought; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Warglien, M. The development of semantic space for pointing and verbal communication. In Conceptual Spaces and the Construal of Spatial Meaning: Empirical Evidence from Human Communication; Hudson, J., Magnusson, U., Paradis, C., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013; pp. 29–42. [Google Scholar]
- Bickerton, D. Language and Species; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Dessalles, J.-L. Why We Talk; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Everett, D. How Language Began: The Story of Humanity’s Greatest Invention; Norton: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, P. The role of cooperation in the evolution of protolanguage and language. In Evolution of Mind, Brain, and Culture; Hatfield, G., Pittman, H., Eds.; University of Pennsylvania Museum of Archaeology and Anthropology: Philadephia, PA, USA, 2013; pp. 193–216. [Google Scholar]
- Gärdenfors, P. The cognitive and communicative demands of cooperation. In Games, Actions and Social Software; van Eijck, J., Verbrugge, R., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2012; Volume 7010, pp. 164–183. [Google Scholar]
- Zlatev, J.; Persson, T.; Gärdenfors, P. Bodily mimesis as ‘the missing link’ in human cognitive evolution. Lund Univ. Cogn. Stud. 2005, 121, 1–45. [Google Scholar]
- Brinck, I.; Gärdenfors, P. Cooperation and communication in apes and humans. Mind Lang. 2003, 18, 484–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gärdenfors, P. Cooperation and the evolution of symbolic communication. In The Evolution of Communication Systems; Oller, K., Griebel, U., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2004; pp. 237–256. [Google Scholar]
- Tylén, K.; Weed, E.; Wallentin, M.; Roepstorff, A.; Frith, C.D. Language as a tool for interacting minds. Mind Lang. 2010, 25, 3–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meldrum, J.; Hilton, C.E. From Biped to Strider: The Emergence of Modern Human Walking, Running, and Resource Transport; Meldrum, J., Hilton, C.E., Eds.; Springer Science & Business Media: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Isaac, L.G. The earliest archaeological traces. In Cambridge History of Africa, Volume 1; Clark, J.D., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1982; pp. 157–247. [Google Scholar]
- Toth, N. The Oldowan reassessed: A close look at early stone artefacts. J. Archeol. Sci. 1985, 12, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donald, M. Key cognitive preconditions for the evolution of language. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2016, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gärdenfors, P.; Högberg, A. The archaeology of teaching and the evolution of Homo docens. Curr. Anthropol. 2017, 58, 188–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kline, M.A. How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behav. Brain Sci. 2015, 38, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boesch, C. Cooperative hunting roles among Tai chimpanzees. Hum. Nat. 2002, 13, 27–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sterelny, K. The Evolved Apprentice; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Számadó, S. Pre-hunt communication provides context for the evolution of early human language. Biol. Theory 2010, 5, 366–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, E.A. Communication and collective action: Language and the evolution of human cooperation. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2010, 31, 231–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nowak, M.A.; Sigmund, K. Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature 2005, 437, 1291–1298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leimar, O.; Hammerstein, P. Evolution of cooperation through indirect reciprocity. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2001, 268, 745–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunbar, R. Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language; Faber and Faber: London, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Slingerland, I.; Mulder, M.; van der Vaart, E.; Verbrugge, R. A multi-agent systems approach to gossip and the evolution of language. In Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci’09), Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 29 July–1 August 2009; 1 August 2009; pp. 1609–1614. [Google Scholar]
- Semmann, D.; Krambeck, H.J.; Milinski, M. Reputation is valuable within and outside one’s own social group. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2005, 57, 611–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gärdenfors, P. From pantomime to protolanguage. Paradigmi 2020, 38, 251–268. [Google Scholar]
- Croft, W. Syntactic Categories and Grammatical Relations: The Cognitive Organization of Information; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, A.E. Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Sstructure; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Langacker, R.W. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar: Theoretical Prerequisites; Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA, USA, 1987; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
- Talmy, L. Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Vol 2: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Croft, W.; Wood, E.J. Construal operations in linguistics and artificial intelligence. Mean. Cogn. Multidiscip. Approach 2000, 2, 51. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff, R. Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution; Oxford University Press: Oxford, MS, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Rosen, S.T. The syntactic representation of linguistic events. In The Second Glot International State-of-the-Article Book; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 2014; pp. 323–366. [Google Scholar]
- DeLancey, S. Event construal and case role assignment. In Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General Session and Parasession on the Grammar of Event Structure; Linguistic Society of America: Washington, DC, USA, 1991; Volume 17, pp. 338–353. [Google Scholar]
- Rappaport Hovav, M.; Levin, B. Reflections on manner/result complementarity. In Lexical Semantics, Syntax, and Event Structure; Rappaport Hovav, M., Doron, D., Sichel, I., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 21–38. [Google Scholar]
- Rappaport Hovav, M.; Levin, B. Building verb meanings. In The Projection of Arguments: Lexical and Compositional Factors; CSLI Publications: Stanford, CA, USA, 1998; pp. 97–134. [Google Scholar]
- Goddard, C.; Wierzbicka, A. Introducing lexical primitives. In Semantic and Lexical Universals: Theory and Empirical Findings; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1994; pp. 31–54. [Google Scholar]
- Dixon, R.M.W. Adjective classes in typological perspective. In Adjective Classes: A Cross-Linguistic Typology; Dixon, R.M.V., Aikhenvald, A.Y., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2004; pp. 1–49. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, C. Ontologies and construals in lexical semantics. Axiomathes 2005, 15, 541–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zwarts, J.; Gärdenfors, P. Locative and directional prepositions in conceptual spaces: The role of polar convexity. J. Logic Lang. Inf. 2016, 25, 109–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Gärdenfors, P. Causal Reasoning and Event Cognition as Evolutionary Determinants of Language Structure. Entropy 2021, 23, 843. https://doi.org/10.3390/e23070843
Gärdenfors P. Causal Reasoning and Event Cognition as Evolutionary Determinants of Language Structure. Entropy. 2021; 23(7):843. https://doi.org/10.3390/e23070843
Chicago/Turabian StyleGärdenfors, Peter. 2021. "Causal Reasoning and Event Cognition as Evolutionary Determinants of Language Structure" Entropy 23, no. 7: 843. https://doi.org/10.3390/e23070843
APA StyleGärdenfors, P. (2021). Causal Reasoning and Event Cognition as Evolutionary Determinants of Language Structure. Entropy, 23(7), 843. https://doi.org/10.3390/e23070843