Next Article in Journal
The Impact of Socio-Economic Status on Self-Rated Health: Study of 29 Countries Using European Social Surveys (2002–2008)
Previous Article in Journal
Changes in Tobacco Use, Susceptibility to Future Smoking, and Quit Attempts among Canadian Youth over Time: A Comparison of Off-Reserve Aboriginal and Non-Aboriginal Youth
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Comment

Comments on: Chari, R.; Burke, T.A.; White, R.H.; Fox, M.A. Integrating Susceptibility into Environmental Policy: An Analysis of the National Ambient Air Quality Standard for Lead. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 1077–1096

Gradient, 20 University Road, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10(2), 742-746; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10020742
Submission received: 14 December 2012 / Revised: 6 February 2013 / Accepted: 14 February 2013 / Published: 21 February 2013

Abstract

:
A recent publication in International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health by Chari et al. [1] provides no basis for changing the National Ambient Air Quality Standard (NAAQS) for lead in the U.S. to protect children in low socio-economic (SES) populations. The studies selected by Chari et al. for analysis do not provide comparable information on regression coefficients for the blood lead level-IQ relationship. The coefficients differ from one another more on the basis of unequal blood lead metrics, ages of blood lead measurement, and differences in covariate adjustments and standardization, than the difference postulated by Chari et al. to correspond to low vs. high SES populations.

In [1], Chari et al. examine literature that summarizes the relationship between blood lead levels in young children and cognitive deficits (IQ) for high and low socio-economic status (SES) populations. Although it has long been observed that there is an inverse association between SES and blood lead levels, the authors go beyond this observation to hypothesize that differences in the dose-response relationship between blood lead and IQ exist depending on the SES status of the population. As a result, they conclude that EPA’s decision framework for development of the National Ambient Air Quality Standard (NAAQS) for lead should result in a lower NAAQS in order to protect these susceptible populations. However, Chari et al.’s analysis is not adequate for this purpose.
Chari et al. rely on four publications that examine the blood lead-IQ relationship as a function of SES status: Dietrich et al. [2]; McMichael et al. [3]; Tong et al. [4]; and Harvey et al. [5]. The ages of the children at the time of blood lead measurement, the blood lead metric, and the type of cognitive test, all differ in these four studies. Dietrich et al. and McMichael et al. employed the Mental Development Index (MDI) of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, administered at 6 months by Dietrich et al. and at 2 years by McMichael et al. Harvey employed the British Ability Scales, administered at 2 years, and Tong et al. employed the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-R), administered at 11–13 years. IQ testing at these various ages obviously takes different forms, and after control for confounders, there is little correlation between the MDI and IQ administered at later ages (Martin et al. [6]).
Children’s ages at the time of blood lead measurement range from 10 days (Dietrich et al.) to 11–13 years (Tong et al.), while the blood lead metric includes a measure of early childhood (Dietrich et al.), lifetime average (McMichael et al. and Tong et al.), and age 2.5 years, approximately peak blood lead (Harvey et al.). Further, the McMichael et al. and Tong et al. studies are of the same Port Pirie children, but at different ages. Blood lead levels are a function of age, peaking at approximately age 2–3 years, and declining into adolescence. Regression coefficients describing the relationship between blood lead level and IQ vary as a function of the age of blood lead measurement and the blood lead metric. This can be seen in Lanphear et al. [7] where four blood lead metrics at different ages are separately regressed against IQ measurements, with differing results (see Table 4 in Lanphear et al.). The difference in coefficients in the McMichael et al. and Tong et al. studies may result in part from the age difference. Comparisons of different SES groups must be based on regression coefficients for the same blood lead metric and approximately the same age, or adjusted in some way for the impact of variable blood lead metrics and ages at time of measurement. Although Chari et al. note that the variable blood lead metrics are a limitation of their analysis, they don’t indicate the direction of bias associated with this limitation and the subsequent impact on their conclusions. The difference in coefficients proposed by Chari et al. for high and low SES populations is similar to the difference seen between studies of children at different ages, suggesting if this bias were considered that there might be no difference in dose-response relationships at different SES levels.
Blood lead levels reported in the Dietrich et al. study are hematocrit-adjusted. This adjustment would also have an impact on the magnitude of the regression coefficient, and complicate the comparison of this result to those of other studies.
Additional uncertainty in the Chari et al. analysis comes from the authors’ extrapolation of the log-linear regression coefficients to a blood lead level of 2 µg/dL, well below the average blood lead levels in each of the studies. Although extrapolation of linear coefficients to lower blood lead levels may produce a bias towards underestimating effects, extrapolation of log-linear coefficients may produce a bias in either direction. As an example of the impact on the dose-response slope resulting from this extrapolation, Chari et al. extrapolate and linearize the regression coefficient from Dietrich et al. at 2 µg/dL to be −2.35. However, a later publication on the same data set by Dietrich et al. [8] reports a linear decrease of 0.73 MDI points per 1 µg/dL increase in (hematocrit-corrected) blood lead level across the blood lead range in the study. The difference between coefficients of −0.73 and −2.35, which are derived in different ways for the same data set, is more than the difference postulated by Chari et al. for high vs. low SES children, suggesting again that there might be no difference in dose-response relationships at different SES levels.
Complicating the comparison of coefficients among these four studies further, some of the coefficients are adjusted for covariates while others are not, and some are standardized while others are not. The regression coefficients from the McMichael study are unadjusted, while those from the other three studies are adjusted. This difference matters: Bellinger [9] notes that “…adjusting for SES and related covariates can result in reductions of 50% or more in the magnitude of lead’s regression coefficient”. Again, Chari et al. should have indicated the direction of this bias. Further, Harvey et al. reported a standardized regression coefficient. The value of a standardized coefficient, which can vary from negative to positive one, is not comparable to an unstandardized coefficient. Although Chari et al. acknowledge that some coefficients are adjusted and others are not, they neither note the direction of bias associated with this limitation, nor note the lack of comparability with the Harvey et al. standardized regression coefficient.
Although the comparison across studies is complicated by the various types of regression coefficients, and the mixed ages, blood lead metrics and cognitive tests, it remains worth reviewing the results found within a study between the high and low SES categories. Chari et al. cite two studies that give this comparison: McMichael et al. and Tong et al. As noted above, these two studies are of the same children, and it is perhaps not surprising that a similar result is found. However, in neither study did the authors find a statistically significant impact of SES on the blood lead-IQ relationship.
The study by Harvey et al. also did not find statistical significance. Before adjustment for covariates, Harvey et al. reported a correlation between blood lead and IQ that was larger and negative for the manual labor class. Harvey et al. [5] present results only for the group of children at age 2.5 years. In a later publication, Harvey et al. [10] report the results of a similar analysis done for a group of children at age 5.5 years. These are not the same children, but rather two groups of children at different ages that were selected and studied at approximately the same time. However, in examining the older children, Harvey et al. [10] found the opposite result in that the children of fathers engaged in non-manual labor had the higher correlation between blood lead and IQ, with a more negative coefficient, although again unadjusted for covariates. In both studies, Harvey and co-authors found that adjustment of the regression for other predictors of IQ resulted in no statistically significant relationship between blood lead and IQ within the studied social class. Chari et al. included the younger children from the first study where the father is engaged in manual labor as low SES in their analysis, but did not include the older children from the second study where the result was the opposite.
Only the study by Dietrich et al. found statistical significance, in an analysis of the Cincinnati cohort based on blood lead levels at age of ten days. However Dietrich et al. [8] also examined the blood lead levels at age 3 months for this same subset of children with SES less than the median, and found that the relationship between blood lead and MDI was not statistically significant. Chari et al. also note that an SES effect was not seen in the Cincinnati cohort in later years.
In summary, the differences in regression coefficients describing the blood lead-IQ relationship between low and high SES populations as put forward by Chari et al. are similar to or less than the differences between regression coefficients expected as a result of mixing blood lead metrics, children’s ages, adjusting or not adjusting for covariates, and comparing standardized and non-standardized regression coefficients. Further, the studies selected by Chari et al. show little or no statistical significance of an SES effect on the relationship between blood lead and IQ. As a result, the Chari et al. analysis does not support a need to modify the Pb NAAQS for protection of low-SES populations. At this time, the methods by which to consider non-chemical stressors in risk assessment are an active area of investigation and debate within the scientific community and among policy makers. The Chari et al. analysis does not inform this debate for the impact of SES on the relationship between blood lead and cognitive impacts. An appropriate analysis would need to focus, at a minimum, on a data set for the same blood lead metric, in children with blood lead measures and cognitive testing at approximately the same age.

Acknowledgments

This work was funded in part by the Association of Battery Recyclers.

Conflict of Interest

This work was funded in part by the Association of Battery Recyclers, who recycle lead-acid automotive batteries in the U.S.

References

  1. Chari, R.; Burke, T.A.; White, R.H.; Fox, M.A. Integrating susceptibility into environmental policy: An analysis of the National Ambient Air Quality Standard for lead. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 1077–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Dietrich, K.N.; Krafft, K.M.; Bier, M.; Succop, P.A.; Berger, O.; Bornschein, R.L. Early effects of fetal lead exposure: Neurobehavioral findings at 6 months. Int. J. Biosoc. Res. 1986, 8, 151–168. [Google Scholar]
  3. McMichael, A.J.; Baghurst, P.A.; Vimpani, G.V.; Robertson, E.F.; Wigg, N.R.; Tong, S.L. Sociodemographic factors modifying the effect of environmental lead on neuropsychological development in early childhood. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 1992, 14, 321–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Tong, S.; McMichael, A.J.; Baghurst, P.A. Interactions between environmental lead exposure and sociodemographic factors on cognitive development. Arch. Environ. Health 2000, 55, 330–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Harvey, P.G.; Hamlin, M.W.; Kumar, R. Blood lead, behavior and intelligence test performance in preschool children. Sci. Total Environ. 1984, 40, 45–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Martin, A.; Volkmar, F.R. Lewis’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook, 3rd ed; Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2007; p. 1071. [Google Scholar]
  7. Lanphear, B.P.; Hornung, R.; Khoury, J.; Yolton, K.; Baghurst, P.; Bellinger, D.C.; Canfield, R.L.; Dietrich, K.N.; Bornschein, R.; Greene, T.; Rothenberg, S.J.; Needleman, H.L.; Schnaas, L.; Wasserman, G.; Graziano, J.; Roberts, R. Low-level environmental lead exposure and children’s intellectual function: An international pooled analysis. Environ. Health Perspect. 2005, 113, 894–899. [Google Scholar]
  8. Dietrich, K.N.; Krafft, K.M.; Bornschein, R.L.; Hammond, P.B.; Berger, O.; Succop, P.A.; Bier, M. Low-level fetal lead exposure effect on neurobehavioral development in early infancy. Pediatrics 1987, 80, 721–730. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bellinger, D.C. Lead neurotoxicity and socioeconomic status: Conceptual and analytical issues. Neurotoxicology 2008, 29, 828–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Harvey, P.G.; Hamlin, M.W.; Kumar, R.; Morgan, J.; Spurgeon, A.; Delves, H.T. Relationships between blood lead, behavior, psychometric and neuropsychological test performance in young children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 1988, 6, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bowers, T.S. Comments on: Chari, R.; Burke, T.A.; White, R.H.; Fox, M.A. Integrating Susceptibility into Environmental Policy: An Analysis of the National Ambient Air Quality Standard for Lead. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 1077–1096. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 742-746. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10020742

AMA Style

Bowers TS. Comments on: Chari, R.; Burke, T.A.; White, R.H.; Fox, M.A. Integrating Susceptibility into Environmental Policy: An Analysis of the National Ambient Air Quality Standard for Lead. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 1077–1096. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2013; 10(2):742-746. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10020742

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bowers, Teresa S. 2013. "Comments on: Chari, R.; Burke, T.A.; White, R.H.; Fox, M.A. Integrating Susceptibility into Environmental Policy: An Analysis of the National Ambient Air Quality Standard for Lead. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 1077–1096" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 10, no. 2: 742-746. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10020742

APA Style

Bowers, T. S. (2013). Comments on: Chari, R.; Burke, T.A.; White, R.H.; Fox, M.A. Integrating Susceptibility into Environmental Policy: An Analysis of the National Ambient Air Quality Standard for Lead. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 1077–1096. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 10(2), 742-746. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10020742

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop