Next Article in Journal
Effects of Pb Smelting on the Soil Bacterial Community near a Secondary Lead Plant
Next Article in Special Issue
Home as a Place of Noise Control for the Elderly? A Cross-Sectional Study on Potential Mediating Effects and Associations between Road Traffic Noise Exposure, Access to a Quiet Side, Dwelling-Related Green and Noise Annoyance
Previous Article in Journal
An Effectiveness Assessment of China’s WEEE Treatment Fund
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Development of a Multiple-Item Annoyance Scale (MIAS) for Transportation Noise Annoyance
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Different Spectral Shapes and Amplitude Modulation of Broadband Noise on Annoyance Reactions in a Controlled Listening Experiment

1
Empa, Laboratory for Acoustics/Noise Control, Swiss Federal Laboratories for Materials Science and Technology, 8600 Dübendorf, Switzerland
2
HSD Hochschule Döpfer—University of Applied Sciences, 50676 Köln, Germany
3
TU Kaiserslautern, 67663 Kaiserslautern, Germany
4
Federal Office for the Environment, 3003 Bern, Switzerland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15(5), 1029; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15051029
Submission received: 28 March 2018 / Revised: 15 May 2018 / Accepted: 16 May 2018 / Published: 19 May 2018
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Noise-Related Annoyance)

Abstract

:
Environmental noise from transportation or industrial infrastructure typically has a broad frequency range. Different sources may have disparate acoustical characteristics, which may in turn affect noise annoyance. However, knowledge of the relative contribution of the different acoustical characteristics of broadband noise to annoyance is still scarce. In this study, the subjectively perceived short-term (acute) annoyance reactions to different broadband sounds (namely, realistic outdoor wind turbine and artificial, generic sounds) at 40 dBA were investigated in a controlled laboratory listening experiment. Combined with the factorial design of the experiment, the sounds allowed for separation of the effects of three acoustical characteristics on annoyance, namely, spectral shape, depth of periodic amplitude modulation (AM), and occurrence (or absence) of random AM. Fifty-two participants rated their annoyance with the sounds. Annoyance increased with increasing energy content in the low-frequency range as well as with depth of periodic AM, and was higher in situations with random AM than without. Similar annoyance changes would be evoked by sound pressure level changes of up to 8 dB. The results suggest that besides standard sound pressure level metrics, other acoustical characteristics of (broadband) noise should also be considered in environmental impact assessments, e.g., in the context of wind turbine installations.

1. Introduction

Large portions of the population are exposed to hazardous (technical) environmental noise (e.g., [1,2]). While environmental noise is associated with various health impacts such as sleep disturbance or cardiovascular effects [2], noise annoyance is particularly widespread [3,4]. As the number of exposed people is likely to increase in the future, adequate environmental noise impact assessment becomes increasingly important.
Environmental noise from transportation or industrial infrastructure (e.g., wind farms) is typically broadband. Different sources may evoke annoyance reactions of different magnitudes [5,6], which is explainable by their differing acoustical characteristics. Such characteristics comprise (i) sound pressure level, (ii) spectral shape, and (iii) (very) short-term temporal level variations, referred to as amplitude modulation (AM).
Sound pressure level is crucial to annoyance (e.g., [5,6]) and, accordingly, the major variable in environmental noise impact assessment (e.g., [2,7]). Moreover, studies on the association of annoyance with spectral shape revealed that both low [8,9] and mid to high frequencies [10] may be important. However, studies comparing the effects of mid and high frequencies with those of low frequencies are scarce, and their findings are contradictory. One study found that annoyance increased with low-frequency content [11], while other studies stress the relative importance of high frequencies [12,13]. Furthermore, AM may be relevant. Modulation functions of AM may be quasi-periodic or random (or both). Periodic AM is sometimes observed for wind turbine (WT) noise and related to the blade passing frequency of WTs (e.g., [14]). It was found to be strongly associated with noise annoyance [15,16]. Also, random AM may play a role. For environmental noise, random AM can be caused by atmospheric turbulence, which affects sound emission [17] as well as propagation [18]. While omnipresent in the environment, we are not aware of any study investigating its effect on annoyance. Also, knowledge of the relative contribution of the above acoustical characteristics of broadband noise to annoyance is still scarce.
The objective of the present study was therefore to investigate short-term annoyance reactions to different situations of broadband sounds under controlled laboratory conditions (see also [19]). Parts of the situations consisted of realistic outdoor WT broadband sounds. WT was chosen as an environmental sound because, due to its strong effect on annoyance [5], it is a currently much discussed topic [20,21,22], at which several of our recent activities were also directed [23,24,25,26]. The WT sounds were complemented with artificial, generic broadband sounds. The study design allowed for separating the contributions of three acoustical characteristics to acute noise annoyance reactions, namely, spectral shape, depth of periodic AM, and occurrence (or absence) of random AM. While the outcomes generally apply to (environmental) broadband noise, practical implications may be specifically directed towards WT noise.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Listening Test Design

In this study, the effect of different acoustical characteristics of outdoor WT and other, generic broadband sounds on noise annoyance was studied under laboratory conditions. WT sounds were included because (i) WT noise effects are a much discussed topic, (ii) this study is a follow-up of a recent listening experiment by us, where annoyance with WT and road traffic noise was compared [26], and (iii) to put the annoyance assessment generally into environmental and specifically into WT noise context. The WT sounds were complemented with generic sounds instead of (further) realistic environmental sounds to have full control on the studied acoustical characteristics. The outcome corresponds to acute, “short-term” [27] or “psychoacoustic” [28] annoyance.
According to Swiss cantonal and federal law, this study was not subject to approval by an ethics committee. However, the Cantonal Ethics Committee KEK Zurich, after having checked the project, stated that from an ethical point of view there was no objection to carrying out the study (Waiver No. 40-2015 (KEK-ZH-No. 2014-0294) from 22 April 2015).
In the listening experiment, 18 acoustical stimuli were systematically varied with respect to three variables: (i) spectral shape; (ii) depth of periodic AM, expressed as standard deviation of the periodic level fluctuations (σpAM); and (iii) occurrence or absence of random AM (Table 1).
The spectral shape covers a wide range from pink to a realistic WT to low-frequency (LF) spectral shape. Pink spectral shape (i.e., 1/f power distribution) was included as a broadband sound with the same energy in all 1/3 octave bands (flat spectrum in logarithmic frequency bands). It is well-defined, closer to environmental sounds than white noise and has been used as a reference sound in previous laboratory studies [11,27]. A typical WT spectrum was chosen as a broadband environmental sound with prominent low-frequency components (20 Hz to ~200 Hz) [29] and thus a distinct “spectral slope” of the (unweighted) sound pressure level with increasing octave band [30]. Finally, an “exaggerated” LF spectrum with a double WT spectral slope, i.e., strong low frequency components but weak mid and high frequency components, was included (cf. Section 2.2). Pink and LF are generic spectra. The depth of periodic AM covers the occurrence of no (σpAM = 0 dB), medium (σpAM = 1.5 dB) and strong AM (σpAM = 3 dB). For WT, the latter two represent situations with high-frequency “swishing” and mid-frequency “thumping” sound [14]. The occurrence or absence of random AM was studied to assess its contribution to annoyance compared to periodic AM. The stimuli were reproduced at a LAeq of 40 dBA, which is a typical WT noise exposure for residents living close to wind farms and already associated with annoyance reactions (e.g., [31,32]).
In addition, one WT sound was used as a reference and additionally reproduced at a LAeq of 37 and 43 dBA, besides 40 dBA (Table 1). This reference (three stimuli) was used to express the contribution of the above three variables to annoyance as equivalents of a (WT) sound pressure level change.
From the resulting set of 20 stimuli, two subsets were separately analyzed regarding annoyance (cf. Section 2.7), with the reference stimulus at a LAeq of 40 dBA (Table 1) included in both subsets:
  • Subset I contained the ratings of the reference sound of Table 1 at a LAeq of 37, 40 and 43 dBA. It reveals the annoyance reactions to LAeq changes.
  • Subset II contained the ratings of the 18 stimuli of Table 1 without the two additional stimuli at a LAeq of 37 and 43 dBA. It reveals the annoyance reactions to the variables of Table 1 at a constant LAeq of 40 dBA.
While the full factorial design of the experiment (Table 1) required the inclusion of situations that do not occur in reality (namely, situations with LF and pink spectral shape and/or without random AM), it allowed for separation of the effects of the three variables on annoyance.

2.2. Acoustical Stimuli

The acoustical stimuli of Table 1 were artificially generated using the sound synthesis technique and the data described in [24,25].
Emission synthesis of the WT stimuli represents one single 2 MW Vestas V90 (three blades, hub height = 95 m, rotor diameter = 90 m) at strong wind and without tonal components. The depth of periodic AM was modelled by adjusting σpAM to 0.0, 1.5 or 3.0 dB (Table 1). The fluctuation frequency was set to 0.75 Hz, corresponding to a rotational speed of 15 rpm for a WT with three blades. Random AM of the emission was either simulated with a frequency dependent standard deviation (σrAM) amounting to ~1.5 dB at high frequencies [25] (stimuli “with random AM”) or switched off (“no random AM”). On the emission signals, propagation filtering [24] was performed for flat grassy terrain and a distance of 400 m, which corresponds to a LAeq of ~40 dBA. The receiver height was set to 2 m above ground. The filtering accounts for geometric spreading, air absorption and ground reflection from an extended source, as well as for random AM from propagation (turbulence in the case of WT) with a σrAM of 1.5 dB, but the latter only for the stimuli “with random AM” (otherwise switched off).
The stimuli with pink and LF spectral shapes were created by modifying the spectral shaping in the above described synthesis approach. Hereby, the LF spectrum was calculated based on the above resulting WT and the pink spectrum such as to obtain the same (absolute) sound level difference between LF and WT as between pink and WT in each 1/3 octave band.
The stimuli were normalized in amplitude to exactly match the desired LAeq of 40 dBA. In addition, the reference stimulus (cf. Table 1) was duplicated twice and scaled in amplitude to a LAeq of 37 and 43 dBA. For the experiments, a stimulus length of 20 s was chosen, which in [26] was found to be optimal. The 20 resulting synthesized sound pressure signals were saved as single-channel audio signals in the WAVE PCM format.
Figure 1 shows the resulting LF, WT and pink spectra for the situations without periodic AM and without random AM. The spectra of the other situations are identical. The WT spectrum is a typical one, lying within the bundle of WT spectra and having a spectral slope close to the −4 dB/oct measured in residential areas [30] (Figure 1a). LF has a double spectral slope of −9 dB/oct, and pink 0 dB/oct (Figure 1a). The differences LC-A between the C-weighted sound pressure level (LCeq) and LAeq of the stimuli amount to 2, 16 and 29 dB for pink, WT and LF spectra, respectively. This indicates that the WT and in particular the LF spectral shapes contain substantially more energy at low frequencies than the pink shape (Figure 1).
Figure 2 and Figure 3 show exemplary level-time histories of the A-weighted, FAST time-weighted sound pressure level (LAF). Both periodic and random AM strongly affect the level-time histories of the stimuli. Periodic AM increases the standard deviation of the LAF in situations with and without random AM (Figure 2), while random AM increases the standard deviation in situations without periodic AM (Figure 2b vs. Figure 2a). The effects of periodic and random AM are not additive, but in situations with periodic AM, random AM results in more irregular periodic level fluctuations than if no random AM is present (Figure 2b vs. Figure 2a).
Besides, the level-time histories also depend on spectral shape (Figure 3), with the standard deviation of the LAF of situations without periodic AM and without random AM increasing with increasing energy content in the low-frequency range in the order of pink < WT << LF. Within the set of 18 stimuli, the standard deviation of the LAF varies from 0.1–2.8 dB, depending on periodic AM, random AM, and spectral shape.

2.3. Annoyance Ratings and Questionnaire

Participants were asked to rate their annoyance with the stimuli with the ICBEN 11-point scale [33], with 0 representing the lowest and 10 the highest annoyance rating. To put the annoyance ratings into the context of environmental noise exposure (including WT noise), the participants were asked to answer the same question as in [26] (in German): “When you imagine that this is the sound situation in your garden, what number from 0 to 10 best shows how much you would be bothered, disturbed or annoyed by it?”
The listening tests were complemented with a questionnaire adapted from [26] (Supplementary Materials). The first part contained questions about hearing and well-being, and the second part questions about the participants’ gender, age, living environment, noise sensitivity, attitude towards wind farms, and some concluding questions about the listening test. Noise sensitivity was determined with the NoiSeQ-R [34], which is the Reduced Version of the NoiSeQ [35]. Attitude towards wind farms was measured with the questionnaire of [26].

2.4. Laboratory Setup

The experiments were carried out in the listening test facility AuraLab at Empa (Figure 4). The facility comprises a separate listening and control room, allowing for audio-visual supervision to comply with ethical requirements. The listening room contains a high-quality multichannel loudspeaker reproduction system including a bass management with two subwoofers (Neumann KH 805). It features controlled room acoustics with a reflective floor, low reverberation time (Tmid = 0.11 s) and low background noise level (7 dBA, GK0).
For the present tests, a one-channel setup together with bass management was chosen. The main loudspeaker (Neumann KH 120 A) was installed at a similar height as and at a distance of 2 m from the seated participant’s head, with a porous floor absorber between the loudspeaker and the participant (Figure 4). The frequency response of the laboratory setup lay within ±3.6 dB for the 1/3 octave bands from 16 Hz to 16 kHz. Prior to the tests, the playback chain was calibrated with a sound level meter located at the position of the seated participant’s head.

2.5. Listening Test Procedure

The listening tests were conducted in single sessions as focused tests. The stimuli were played only once, one by one, after complete playback and rating, with a break of 1 s between stimuli. The test procedure consisted of the following steps. First, a short introduction to the research topic and task (annoyance rating of WT and other sounds) was given. Second, the participants signed a consent form to participate. Third, they answered the first part of the questionnaire about hearing and well-being as criteria for study participation. Fourth, they were instructed about the listening test program. Fifth, they did the actual listening test. The test program guided the participants through the test by automatically choosing and playing the stimuli, and by recording the participants’ ratings entered via a graphical user interface (Figure 4). The listening test included (i) an orientation, where the participants listened to five 10 s long stimuli covering the range of situations to be rated, (ii) two exercise ratings, and (iii) the main experiment with annoyance rating of the 20 stimuli, which were reproduced in random order. Finally, the participants completed the second part of the questionnaire.
The whole listening test including the introduction and the questionnaire lasted about one hour. A compensation of 20 Swiss francs (approx. €18) was given for participation.

2.6. Participants

Fifty-two participants (24 males, 28 females), aged 18–62 years (median of 43 years), were recruited via online advertisement and word-of-mouth recommendation. The majority worked at Empa. None of the participants wore a hearing aid, and all of them declared that they have normal hearing and feel well (no colds). Sixty-five percent of the participants had heard WT noise before, but none of them lived close to a wind farm.

2.7. Statistical Analysis

The consistency of the annoyance ratings across participants was assessed with the inter-rater reliability [36]. To that aim, a two-way random, consistency, average-measures intraclass correlation (ICC) was calculated [37]. Large ICC values indicate a high degree of agreement between participants.
The annoyance ratings were analyzed by means of linear mixed-effects models (e.g., [38]), using the procedure MIXED of IBM SPSS Version 23. In Subset I, LAeq was treated as a continuous variable. In Subset II, depth of periodic AM was treated as a continuous variable, and spectral shape (three situations) and random AM (two situations) as categorical variables. In addition, interactions between the variables of Subset II were studied, as well as the sequence, i.e., the playback number of the stimuli, and the participants’ characteristics (Section 2.3). Further, different random effect models (random intercept; random coefficients describing the dependence on the variables of Table 1) were tested. From the set of potential models, the final model was chosen by considering completeness (include all relevant variables), performance (data representation, significance of effects) and parsimony (keep the model as simple as possible). The models were compared with the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) [39], where the model with the lowest BIC is preferred. Compliance with the model assumptions was visually checked with residual plots. The goodness-of-fit of the final model was assessed according to [40,41] with the marginal (R2m) and conditional coefficient of determination (R2c), where R2m represents the variance explained by the fixed factors and R2c the variance explained by the fixed plus random factors.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics (Raw Data)

As Figure 5 shows, the individual annoyance ratings of Subsets I and II cover a wide range of the 11-point scale. There is a clear trend of increasing ratings with LAeq (Figure 5a), as well as with spectral shape in the order pink < WT << LF, and with increasing depth of periodic AM (Figure 5b). Further, the annoyance ratings tended to be slightly higher in situations with random AM than in situations without, at least in the absence of periodic AM (Figure 5c). The ICC values for the annoyance ratings of 0.985 (Subset I) and 0.953 (Subset II) lie in the “excellent” range of ICC > 0.75 according to [42] and thus suggest a high degree of agreement between participants [36].
Further, individual ratings of Subset II tended to increase with sequence (playback number) of the stimuli (Pearson correlation coefficient r = 0.08, p = 0.02). In Subset I, in contrast, no such dependence was found. The ratings were not (strongly) related to the participants’ characteristics gender (p = 0.71) or noise sensitivity (p = 0.18), but tended to be higher with increasing age (r = 0.13, p < 0.001) and lower the more positive the attitude towards wind farms (r = −0.17, p < 0.001).
Since the annoyance ratings are bounded at a value of 10, the difference between the participants’ mean annoyance ratings of LF and pink spectral shapes was negatively correlated to their mean rating of pink shape (r = −0.68, p < 0.001), i.e., the ratings tended to strongly depend on spectral shape if annoyance to pink shape was low, and vice versa (Figure 6).

3.2. Effects of Acoustical Characteristics on Annoyance

The data of Subset I revealed that annoyance increases linearly with LAeq, by more than two units on the 11-point scale for an increase in LAeq from 37 to 43 dBA (Figure 7a). The linear mixed-effects model, which explains more than 80% of the variance (R2m = 0.19, R2c = 0.84), confirms the statistical significance of the LAeq (p < 0.001) with the following relationship:
Annoyance = 0.359 (±0.063) × LAeq − 7.885 (±2.577),
where the numbers in brackets indicate the 95% confidence intervals. Thus, a sound pressure level change of 2.8 dB is associated with a change of 1 unit on the 11-point scale, and vice versa.
The data of Subset II revealed that annoyance strongly increases with increasing energy content in the low-frequency range, in the order pink < WT << LF (Figure 7b). Furthermore, annoyance increases with increasing depth of periodic AM. This effect is very clear in situations without random AM, but less pronounced in situations with random AM (Figure 7c). Finally, annoyance with situations without random AM is lower than with random AM, but only at low depths of periodic AM (Figure 7c).
The observed effects can be described with the following mixed-effects model:
Annoyanceijk = μ + Speci + rAMj + βpAMijk + βrAM,jpAMijk + γSijk + ui,k + εijk.
In Equation (2), Annoyance is the dependent variable, μ is the overall mean, Spec and rAM are the categorical variables spectral shape (3 levels: i = 1, 2, 3) and random AM (2 levels: j = 1, 2), pAM and S are the continuous variables periodic AM and sequence, β and γ are regression coefficients, and βrAM represents the interaction between rAM and pAM. Further, ui,k are the participants’ random coefficient terms (k = 1, …, 52). They account for the dependence of the individual annoyance ratings on spectral shape (Figure 6), using an unstructured covariance matrix for that purpose. Finally, the error term ε is the random deviation between observed and predicted values of Annoyance. The index ijk represents the kth replicate observation of the ith spectral shape at the jth random AM.
In addition to the variables of Equation (2), the interaction between Spec and pAM was significant (p < 0.001), but seemed to be primarily caused by the outlying (low) rating to the stationary WT stimulus (lowest rating to WT in Figure 8b). Further, also the participants’ attitude towards wind farms was significant (p = 0.02). It was, however, not of focus here. The other tested variables (interaction terms Spec × rAM × pAM and Spec × rAM; participants’ gender, age and noise sensitivity) were not significant (p = 0.09–0.90). None of these variables were included in the model. The final model explains a large part of the variance (R2m = 0.18, R2c = 0.81), although only ~20% with the fixed effects. The model parameters are given in Table A1 of the Appendix.
Figure 7 further reveals the following variation in annoyance for the acoustical characteristics and equivalents of a (WT) sound pressure level change. On average, a change in spectral shape from pink to LF increases annoyance by almost two units on the 11-point scale (Figure 7b). The same effect would be evoked by a level increase of 5.3 dB. The effects of periodic and random AM are less pronounced. On average, an increase in depth of periodic AM from σpAM = 0 to 3 dB increases annoyance by more than 1 unit in situations without random AM, but only by 0.4 units with random AM. This would also be evoked by a level increase of 3.1 and 1.1 dB, respectively (Figure 7c). Similarly, in the absence of periodic AM, annoyance with situations with random AM on average is 0.7 units higher than without random AM. This corresponds to a level increase of 2.0 dB. No such effect, in contrast, is observable in situations with strong periodic AM. Finally, the mean annoyance with the individual stimuli of Subset II covers a wide range of three units on the 11-point scale (cf. Figure 8b). This corresponds to an equivalent level change of 8.4 dB.

3.3. Explorative Data Re-Analysis

While the acoustical variables of Table 1 affect short-term annoyance, they are usually not easy to determine, especially in the case of field recordings. Therefore, the stimuli of Subset II were characterized with two more accessible variables as substitutes for those of Table 1, to study their effects on annoyance:
  • LC-A ≡ sound level difference LCeqLAeq (cf. Section 2.2): indicator for the low-frequency content of the stimuli and substitute for the variable spectral shape.
  • σfluc ≡ standard deviation of the A-weighted, FAST time-weighted level-time histories of the high-pass filtered stimuli: indicator for the level variation due to periodic and random AM and substitute for the two variables. A high-pass filtered signal (here, with a cutoff frequency of 500 Hz, i.e., f > 500 Hz) was used to minimize the influence of spectral shape on level variation (Figure 3) and to obtain approximate independence of the two variables LC-A and σfluc.
Using these variables, Subset II was re-analyzed. Figure 8 shows the results. Both LC-A and σfluc are strongly associated with short-term annoyance.
The observed effects can be described with the following mixed-effects model:
Annoyancek = μ + δLC-A,k + ησfluc,k + γSk + u0k + u1k + εk,
where Annoyance, μ, γ, S, ε and the index k are defined in Equation (2), LC-A and σfluc are the continuous variables introduced above, δ and η are regression coefficients, and the terms u0k and u1k are the participants’ correlated random intercept and slope (unstructured covariance matrix) to account for the dependence of the individual ratings on LC-A, in analogy to spectral shape in Equation (2). The variables included in the model are all highly significant (LC-A and σfluc: p < 0.001; S: p < 0.002). The model parameters are given in Table A2 of the Appendix. An analogous model can also be established for the spectral slope variable (cf. Section 2.2) instead of LC-A.
Although the model of Equation (3) is considerably simpler than the one established for spectral shape, periodic and random AM (Equation (2)), using only four instead of seven degrees of freedom for the fixed effects, it represents the data equally accurately, with very similar coefficients of determination (R2m of 0.16, R2c of 0.74). Further, the model has the advantage that all variables are continuous and thus allow for interpolation and (to some degree) extrapolation of the results.

4. Discussion

In this study, a laboratory listening experiment was performed using stimuli representing different situations of WT and other, generic broadband sounds. The factorial design in combination with the sound synthesis tools used to generate the stimuli and the statistical methods allowed for separation of the relative contributions of the three acoustical characteristics spectral shape, depth of periodic AM and random AM to short-term annoyance. Further, with the study design the variation in annoyance reactions to the acoustical situations could be expressed as equivalent changes in (WT) sound pressure level, which is the most often used indicator in noise exposure assessments.

4.1. Acoustical Characteristics and Annoyance

Annoyance was found to increase with sound pressure level (here, LAeq). This was expected and reported in many previous laboratory (e.g., [15,26,27]) as well as field studies (e.g., [5,6]). Besides the LAeq, also the acoustical characteristics of Table 1 were found to be strongly linked to annoyance. Similar annoyance changes would be evoked by equivalent sound pressure level changes of more than 8 dB, corresponding to more than a 6-fold change in sound energy. Thus, different amplitude modulated broadband noises at a constant LAeq of 40 dBA were perceived very differently with respect to annoyance, even without presence of tonal or impulsive components, which are well known to be strongly linked to annoyance (e.g., [43,44]).
First, spectral shape was found to be important. Annoyance increased with increasing energy in the low-frequency range (Figure 7). This effect was also found in other studies [8,9]. It could be well predicted with the variable LC-A (Figure 8), which was also found in [11]. The latter study assessed annoyance as “pink noise equivalents” and found an increase of 0.46 dB per 1 dB LC-A, while our study yields “WT noise equivalents” with an increase of 0.20 dB per 1 dB LC-A. Interestingly, while WT was found here to be more annoying than pink spectral shape (~1.2 dB equivalent sound pressure level change: Figure 7), another laboratory study [27] found WT to be ~5 dB more annoying than pink noise if participants were residents of wind farms, but vice versa if they were not. Further, contrasting our results, other studies stress the relative importance of high compared to low frequencies [12,13]. Second, periodic AM was associated with annoyance. Also this observation is in line with the literature [15,16,26]. Third, as with periodic AM, annoyance increased with random AM. So far, this effect was not systematically studied in literature. Nevertheless, it is consonant with the results of [26] insofar as the latter revealed that annoyance with AM is not related to its periodicity but rather to the modulation frequency range. Here, the effects of periodic and random AM were not only of similar magnitude but also interrelated, i.e., annoyance increased less with increasing periodic AM in situations with random AM than without, and vice versa (Figure 7). Further corroborating the similarity of effects, periodic and random AM could be combined into a single variable for level fluctuations to predict annoyance without a notable loss of accuracy (Figure 8).

4.2. Sensory Perception and Annoyance

The participants’ annoyance ratings were found to be closely linked to their sensory perception, as answers to the concluding questions of the questionnaire (Supplementary Materials) revealed: Several participants mentioned discomfort due to a “pulsing” sensation in the ears by the “dull” or “low” LF sounds. The “pulsing” sensation may also have been caused by periodic AM. Besides, some participants found “hissing” or “high” sounds annoying, which might have referred to pink spectral shape, but probably also to the short-time level fluctuations of random AM. Thus, the sensory perception seems to be an important link between the acoustical characteristics of the stimuli and annoyance, which may help to better understand and predict annoyance reactions to noise. These aspects can be further explored with semantic differential tests (e.g., [45]). For that purpose, a corresponding test was carried out and is being analyzed with the stimuli of Subset II (work in progress).
Similarly, instead of sound pressure level-related quantities, psychoacoustic parameters [28] might serve as alternative, more precise predictors for aural perception and thus annoyance. Here, loudness seems particularly important. Short-term annoyance was reported to be strongly correlated with perceived or calculated loudness (e.g., [46,47,48]). Further, a listening experiment on road traffic noise revealed loudness to be a good predictor for annoyance with stimuli varying in LAeq and LC-A [11]. Nevertheless, short-term annoyance and loudness are not the same. In fact, in the present listening experiment, annoyance was found to increase with calculated Zwicker loudness [49] for the stimuli of Subset I where only the LAeq was varied, but to decrease with Zwicker loudness for the stimuli of Subset II comprising different acoustical characteristics (details see [19]). Apparently, Zwicker loudness was inappropriate to predict annoyance with the studied sounds. Analogous results were found in another laboratory study with low loudness levels [50]. Other loudness models may be more appropriate. Besides, further psychoacoustic parameters may be important, such as fluctuation strength in the case of the present data set. Fluctuation strength describes the hearing sensation of AM at fluctuation frequencies below 20 Hz [28]. It has been suggested that it reaches its maximum at 4 Hz [51]. Fluctuation strength might describe the (combined) perception of periodic and random AM and thus be an appropriate predictor for their associated annoyance. Re-analysis of the data sets with calculated psychoacoustic parameters might thus yield additional insights.
These are possible future research approaches to establish a closer link between acoustical characteristics, perception, and short-term annoyance.

4.3 Annoyance Responses in Laboratory vs. Field Studies

In interpreting the present findings, one needs to consider the inherent differences between short-term annoyance investigated in the laboratory in a focused test and annoyance with long-term exposure in the field [48]. In particular, annoyance in the laboratory is usually closely associated with the LAeq (Section 4.1) and also with loudness parameters (Section 4.2). It thus seems to be closely related to the sensory perception of sound. Supporting this interpretation, the participants’ ratings in the present study were highly consistent (ICC values > 0.9).
In field studies (socio-acoustic surveys), the association of annoyance with LAeq (or related noise metrics) is usually weaker than in laboratory studies, with values of R2 being in the range of 0.05–0.25 [52]. This might be related to various reasons. First, noise calculations usually reflect outdoor exposure (e.g., [5,6]), while study participants spend much of their time indoors. However, low frequencies are attenuated less than high frequencies during sound transmission (e.g., [29]), which changes spectral shape and increases LC-A. Second, noise calculations are afflicted with uncertainties ranging from ~1 dB (aircraft noise [53]) to ~4 dB (WT noise at distances <1 km [54]). Third, temporal patterns such as short-time level variations (AM) are usually neglected in calculations. Fourth, people commute between home, work and leisure and are thus exposed to a variety of sound situations (and not to specific situations as in the laboratory), which is never accounted for. Field studies thus carry a large exposure misclassification bias, which reduces the strength of statistically modelled relationships. Fifth, in the field not only the noise source in focus for annoyance, but also other sources are present as effect modifiers. This may affect annoyance, be it by an improved acoustic quality [55] or by masking [56,57]. Finally, personal and situational factors may strongly influence annoyance in the field [5,58], but probably less so in the laboratory (as, e.g., in this study or in [26]). These are all aspects that explain the higher correlations one sees in the laboratory.
In conclusion, while the present study design allowed disclosing the relative contributions of different acoustical characteristics to annoyance without potential effect modifiers, the high experimental control (including generic stimuli) was at the expense of ecological validity. Field studies, in contrast, have high ecological validity, but at the expense of control (sound exposure, effect modifiers). Thus, laboratory studies are best for reliable effect differentiations, which then might be validated in field studies. Consequently, laboratory studies should precede field studies—and not vice versa.

4.4. Practical Implications for WT Noise

Keeping the above discussed limited ecological validity of the present results in mind, practical implications may be tentatively directed towards WT noise. Given the important role of acoustical characteristics of (WT) broadband noise for annoyance, the design, operation and noise assessment of WTs and wind farms may positively or negatively affect residents in terms of annoyance. First, acoustically optimized designs of WT rotor blades may reduce sound emission and thus exposure [17]. Second, larger WTs may emit more low-frequent sound [29]. However, changes in spectral shape are much less pronounced than in the situations studied here, and a recent study suggested that the size of WTs might not be relevant for annoyance [59]. Third, annoyance might be reduced by reducing the occurrence of periodic AM by blade pitch control [60] and by prevention of stall on the WT blades [14]. On the other hand, periodic AM, if occurring, may be enhanced by interference between WTs of a wind farm [61]. Fourth, operational restrictions such as limits of angular blade velocity as a function of the wind direction may effectively reduce annoyance while still allowing for cost-effective energy production [16]. Fifth, the important role of the acoustical characteristics of WT noise asks for reliable assessed methods. A recent procedure allows measuring WT noise even in presence of masking sounds [62]. Finally, policy makers should account (more) for specific acoustical characteristics of WTs, e.g., by adding a penalty for periodic AM [63]. Current legislation distinctly varies between countries [64,65].

5. Conclusions

In the present laboratory listening experiment, the effects of three acoustical characteristics of broadband noise on short-term annoyance were studied, namely, spectral shape, depth of periodic AM, and random AM. To that aim, realistic WT as well as generic broadband sounds with pink and LW spectral shape were presented in a listening experiment, and participants’ annoyance reactions were recorded. The full factorial design of the listening experiment allowed for separation of the relative contributions of the acoustical characteristics to annoyance. It was found that besides sound pressure level, all three studied characteristics affect annoyance: Annoyance increased with increasing energy content in the low-frequency range as well as with depth of periodic AM, and was higher in situations with random AM than without. Similar annoyance changes would be evoked by sound pressure level changes of up to 8 dB. Thus, in essence, we could demonstrate that besides standard sound pressure level metrics spectral shape as well as short-term temporal level variations (i.e., AM) should be considered in environmental impact assessments. Our findings are particularly important in the wake of environmental impact assessments for WT noise. They cater to manufacturers and policy makers alike—people who want to forecast residents’ annoyance near such installations.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/15/5/1029/s1, Supplementary File: Authors’ questionnaire (in German). Note that page 6 of the questionnaire partly refers to the listening test mentioned in Section 4.2, which was carried out along with the experiment described here.

Author Contributions

Beat Schäffer (project leader), together with all coauthors (Reto Pieren, Sabine J. Schlittmeier and Mark Brink), conceived and designed the experiments. Reto Pieren generated the acoustical stimuli and, together with Beat Schäffer, did the experimental setup. Beat Schäffer performed the experiments, analyzed the data and wrote the paper, in discussion with and receiving contributions from all coauthors.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the 52 participants of the listening experiment. The project was funded by the Swiss Federal Office for the Environment. Portions of this work were presented in [19].

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Model coefficients with 95% confidence intervals (CI) and probabilities (p) of the linear mixed-effects model for short-term annoyance (Equation (2)).
Table A1. Model coefficients with 95% confidence intervals (CI) and probabilities (p) of the linear mixed-effects model for short-term annoyance (Equation (2)).
Linear Mixed-Effects Model
Variable 1Symbol 1Coefficient95% CIp
Interceptµ6.4028[5.8998; 6.9057]<0.001
Spectral shapeSpeci=LF1.4736[0.9491; 1.9981]<0.001
Speci=WT0 2
Speci=pink−0.4307[−0.7848; −0.0766]0.02
Random AMrAMj=no−0.7238[−0.9345; −0.5131]<0.001
rAMj=with0 2
Periodic AMβ0.1330[0.0563; 0.2098]<0.001
Random × Periodic AMβrAM,j=no0.2424[0.1335; 0.3513]<0.001
βrAM,j=with0 2
Sequenceγ0.0178[0.0055; 0.0301]<0.005
Random effect variance(spectral shape)u2i=LF,k3.1156[2.0670; 4.6961]<0.001
u2i=WT,k2.6436[1.7469; 4.0006]<0.001
u2i=pink,k3.6389[2.4219; 5.4674]<0.001
Residual varianceε2ijk1.0664[0.9654; 1.1780]<0.001
1 Variables and symbols: see Equation (2). 2 Redundant coefficients are set to zero.
Table A2. Model coefficients with 95% confidence intervals (CI) and probabilities (p) of the linear mixed-effects model for short-term annoyance (explorative data re-analysis, Equation (3)).
Table A2. Model coefficients with 95% confidence intervals (CI) and probabilities (p) of the linear mixed-effects model for short-term annoyance (explorative data re-analysis, Equation (3)).
Linear Mixed-Effects Model
Variable 1Symbol 1Coefficient95% CIp
Interceptµ4.9498[4.3186; 5.5811]<0.001
LC-Aδ0.0702[0.0455; 0.0949]<0.001
σflucη0.4436[0.3553; 0.5319]<0.001
Sequenceγ0.0215[0.0081; 0.0349]<0.002
Random effect variance (intercept and slope)u20k4.4574[2.9644; 6.7023]<0.001
u21k0.0073[0.0048; 0.0111]<0.001
Residual varianceε2ijk1.3669[1.2415; 1.5050]<0.001
1 Variables and symbols: see Equation (3).

References

  1. FOEN. Noise Pollution in Switzerland. Results of the SonBase National Noise Monitoring Programme. State of the Environment No. 0907; Federal Office for the Environment (FOEN): Bern, Switzerland, 2009; 61p, Available online: http://www.bafu.admin.ch/publikationen/ (accessed on 14 May 2018).
  2. WHO. Burden of Disease from Environmental Noise. Quantification of Healthy Life Years Lost in Europe; World Health Organization (WHO), Regional Office for Europe: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2011; 106p. Available online: http://www.euro.who.int/en/publications (accessed on 14 May 2018).
  3. Guski, R.; Schreckenberg, D.; Schuemer, R. WHO environmental noise guidelines for the European region: A systematic review on environmental noise and annoyance. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 1539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Babisch, W. The noise/stress concept, risk assessment and research needs. Noise Health 2002, 4, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  5. Janssen, S.A.; Vos, H.; Eisses, A.R.; Pedersen, E. A comparison between exposure-response relationships for wind turbine annoyance and annoyance due to other noise sources. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 130, 3746–3753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Miedema, H.M.E.; Oudshoorn, C.G.M. Annoyance from transportation noise: Relationships with exposure metrics DNL and DENL and their confidence intervals. Environ. Health Perspect. 2001, 109, 409–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. European Union. Directive 2002/49/EC of the European Parliament and of the Council of 25 June 2002 Relating to the Assessment and Management of Environmental Noise; European Union: Bruxelles, Belgium, 2002; 14p, Available online: https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/TXT/?uri=celex:32002L0049 (accessed on 14 May 2018).
  8. Berglund, B.; Hassmén, P.; Job, R.F.S. Sources and effects of low-frequency noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1996, 99, 2985–3002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Pawlaczyk-Łuszczyńska, M.; Dudarewicz, A.; Waszkowska, M.; Śliwińska-Kowalska, M. Assessment of annoyance from low frequency and broadband noises. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 2003, 16, 337–343. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  10. Ishiyama, T.; Hashimoto, T. The impact of sound quality on annoyance caused by road traffic noise: An influence of frequency spectra on annoyance. JSAE Rev. 2000, 21, 225–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Nilsson, M.E. A-weighted sound pressure level as an indicator of short-term loudness or annoyance of road-traffic sound. J. Sound Vib. 2007, 302, 197–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Torija, A.J.; Flindell, I.H. The subjective effect of low frequency content in road traffic noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2015, 137, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Veitch, J.A.; Bradley, J.S.; Legault, L.M.; Norcross, S.G.; Svec, J.M. Masking Speech in Open-Plan Offices with Simulated Ventilation Noise: Noise Level and Spectral Composition Effects on Acoustic Satisfaction. Report No. IRC-IR-846; National Research Council Canada: Ottawa, ON, Canada, 2002; 56p, Available online: https://nparc.nrc-cnrc.gc.ca/eng/home/ (accessed on 14 May 2018).
  14. Oerlemans, S. Effect of wind shear on amplitude modulation of wind turbine noise. Int. J. Aeroacoust. 2015, 14, 715–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lee, S.; Kim, K.; Choi, W.; Lee, S. Annoyance caused by amplitude modulation of wind turbine noise. Noise Control Eng. J. 2011, 59, 38–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bockstael, A.; Dekoninck, L.; Can, A.; Oldoni, D.; De Coensel, B.; Botteldooren, D. Reduction of wind turbine noise annoyance: An operational approach. Acta Acust. Acust. 2012, 98, 392–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Oerlemans, S.; Fisher, M.; Maeder, T.; Kögler, K. Reduction of wind turbine noise using optimized airfoils and trailing-edge serrations. AIAA J. 2009, 47, 1470–1481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gilbert, K.E.; Raspet, R.; Di, X. Calculation of turbulence effects in an upward-refracting atmosphere. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1990, 87, 2428–2437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Schäffer, B.; Pieren, R.; Schlittmeier, S.J.; Brink, M.; Heutschi, K. Annoyance to wind turbine Noise—Influence of different acoustical characteristics. Paper No. 458. In Proceedings of the Inter-Noise 2017, 46th International Congress and Exposition on Noise Control Engineering, Hong Kong, China, 27–30 August 2017; Institute of Noise Control Engineering: Indianapolis, IN, USA, 2017; pp. 2447–2454. [Google Scholar]
  20. McCunney, R.J.; Mundt, K.A.; Colby, W.D.; Dobie, R.; Kaliski, K.; Blais, M. Wind turbines and health: A critical review of the scientific literature. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2014, 56, e108–e130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Schmidt, J.H.; Klokker, M. Health effects related to wind turbine noise exposure: A systematic review. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e114183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Van Kamp, I.; van den Berg, F. Health effects related to wind turbine sound, including low-frequency sound and infrasound. Acoust. Aust. 2017, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Manyoky, M.; Wissen Hayek, U.; Heutschi, K.; Pieren, R.; Grêt-Regamey, A. Developing a GIS-based visual-acoustic 3D simulation for wind farm assessment. ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2014, 3, 29–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Heutschi, K.; Pieren, R.; Müller, M.; Manyoky, M.; Wissen Hayek, U.; Eggenschwiler, K. Auralization of wind turbine noise: Propagation filtering and vegetation noise synthesis. Acta Acust. Acust. 2014, 100, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Pieren, R.; Heutschi, K.; Müller, M.; Manyoky, M.; Eggenschwiler, K. Auralization of wind turbine noise: Emission synthesis. Acta Acust. Acust. 2014, 100, 25–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Schäffer, B.; Schlittmeier, S.J.; Pieren, R.; Heutschi, K.; Brink, M.; Graf, R.; Hellbrück, J. Short-term annoyance reactions to stationary and time-varying wind turbine and road traffic noise: A laboratory study. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2016, 139, 2949–2963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Bolin, K.; Bluhm, G.; Nilsson, M.E. Listening test comparing A-weighted and C-weighted sound pressure level as indicator of wind turbine noise annoyance. Acta Acust. Acust. 2014, 100, 842–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Fastl, H.; Zwicker, E. Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models, 3rd ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2007; p. 462. ISBN 3-540-23159-5. [Google Scholar]
  29. Møller, H.; Pedersen, C.S. Low-frequency noise from large wind turbines. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 129, 3727–3744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Tachibana, H.; Yano, H.; Fukushima, A.; Sueoka, S. Nationwide field measurements of wind turbine noise in Japan. Noise Control Eng. J. 2014, 62, 90–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Michaud, D.S.; Feder, K.; Keith, S.E.; Voicescu, S.A.; Marro, L.; Than, J.; Guay, M.; Denning, A.; McGuire, D.; Bower, T.; et al. Exposure to wind turbine noise: Perceptual responses and reported health effects. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2016, 139, 1443–1454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Pedersen, E.; van den Berg, F.; Bakker, R.; Bouma, J. Response to noise from modern wind farms in The Netherlands. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 126, 634–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Fields, J.M.; De Jong, R.G.; Gjestland, T.; Flindell, I.H.; Job, R.F.S.; Kurra, S.; Lercher, P.; Vallet, M.; Yano, T.; Guski, R.; et al. Standardized general-purpose noise reaction questions for community noise surveys: Research and a recommendation. J. Sound Vib. 2001, 242, 641–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Griefahn, B.; Marks, A.; Gjestland, T.; Preis, A. Annoyance and noise sensitivity in urban areas. In Proceedings of the 19th International Congress on Acoustics (ICA), Madrid, Spain, 2–7 September 2007; Sociedad Española de Acústica: Madrid, Spain, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  35. Schütte, M.; Marks, A.; Wenning, E.; Griefahn, B. The development of the noise sensitivity questionnaire. Noise Health 2007, 9, 15–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Hallgren, K.A. Computing inter-rater reliability for observational data: An overview and tutorial. Tutor. Quant. Methods Psychol. 2012, 8, 23–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. McGraw, K.O.; Wong, S.P. Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychol. Methods 1996, 1, 30–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Pinheiro, J.C.; Bates, D.M. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-Plus, 1st ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2000; p. 528. ISBN 0-387-98957-9. [Google Scholar]
  39. Schwarz, G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 1978, 6, 461–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Nakagawa, S.; Schielzeth, H. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2013, 4, 133–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Johnson, P.C.D. Extension of Nakagawa & Schielzeth’s R2GLMM to random slopes models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2014, 5, 944–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Cicchetti, D.V. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol. Assess. 1994, 6, 284–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Oliva, D.; Hongisto, V.; Haapakangas, A. Annoyance of low-level tonal sounds—Factors affecting the penalty. Build. Environ. 2017, 123, 404–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Schomer, P.D.; Wagner, L.R. Human and community response to military sounds—Part 2: Results from field-laboratory tests of sounds of small arms, 25-mm cannons, helicoperts, and blasts. Noise Control Eng. J. 1995, 43, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Jeon, J.Y.; Lee, P.J.; You, J.; Kang, J. Perceptual assessment of quality of urban soundscapes with combined noise sources and water sounds. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010, 127, 1357–1366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Lee, J.; Francis, J.M.; Wang, L.M. How tonality and loudness of noise relate to annoyance and task performance. Noise Control Eng. J. 2017, 65, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Skagerstrand, A.; Köbler, S.; Stenfelt, S. Loudness and annoyance of disturbing sounds—Perception by normal hearing subjects. Int. J. Audiol. 2017, 56, 775–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Guski, R.; Bosshardt, H.-G. Gibt es eine “unbeeinflußte” Lästigkeit? Z. Lärmbekämpf. 1992, 39, 67–74. [Google Scholar]
  49. ISO. ISO 532-1. Acoustics—Methods for Calculating Loudness—Part 1: Zwicker Method; International Standard; International Organisation for Standardization (ISO): Geneva, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  50. Berglund, B.; Preis, A.; Rankin, K. Relationship between loundess and annoyance for ten community sounds. Environ. Int. 1990, 16, 523–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Fastl, H. Fluctuation strength and temporal masking patterns of amplitude-modulated broadband noise. Hear. Res. 1982, 8, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Brink, M. A review of explained variance in exposure-annoyance relationships in noise annoyance survey. Paper ID 6_4. In Proceedings of the 11th Congress of the International Commission on the Biological Effects of Noise (ICBEN), Noise as a Public Health Problem, Nara, Japan, 1–5 June 2014. [Google Scholar]
  53. Schäffer, B.; Plüss, S.; Thomann, G. Estimating the model-specific uncertainty of aircraft noise calculations. Appl. Acoust. 2014, 84, 58–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Keith, S.E.; Feder, K.; Voicescu, S.A.; Soukhovtsev, V.; Denning, A.; Tsang, J.; Broner, N.; Leroux, T.; Richarz, W.; van den Berg, F. Wind turbine sound pressure level calculations at dwellings. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2016, 139, 1436–1442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Minichilli, F.; Gorini, F.; Ascari, E.; Bianchi, F.; Coi, A.; Fredianelli, L.; Licitra, G.; Manzoli, F.; Mezzasalma, L.; Cori, L. Annoyance judgment and measurements of environmental noise: A focus on Italian secondary schools. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Bolin, K.; Kedhammar, A.; Nilsson, M.E. The influence of background sounds on loudness and annoyance of wind turbine noise. Acta Acust. Acust. 2012, 98, 741–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bolin, K.; Nilsson, M.E.; Khan, S. The potential of natural sounds to mask wind turbine noise. Acta Acust. Acust. 2010, 96, 131–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Michaud, D.S.; Keith, S.E.; Feder, K.; Voicescu, S.A.; Marro, L.; Than, J.; Guay, M.; Bower, T.; Denning, A.; Lavigne, E.; et al. Personal and situational variables associated with wind turbine noise annoyance. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2016, 139, 1455–1466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Hongisto, V.; Oliva, D.; Keränen, J. Indoor noise annoyance due to 3–5 megawatt wind turbines—An exposure-response relationship. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2017, 142, 2185–2196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Makarewicz, R.; Gołębiewski, R. Influence of blade pitch on amplitude modulation of wind turbine noise. Noise Control Eng. J. 2015, 63, 195–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Larsson, C.; Öhlund, O. Amplitude modulation of sound from wind turbines under various meteorological conditions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2014, 135, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Fredianelli, L.; Gallo, P.; Licitra, G.; Carpita, S. Analytical assessment of wind turbine noise impact at receiver by means of residual noise determination without the wind farm shutdown. Noise Control Eng. J. 2017, 65, 417–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Eggenschwiler, K.; Heutschi, K.; Schäffer, B.; Pieren, R.; Bögli, H.; Bärlocher, M. Wirkung und Beurteilung des Lärms von Windenergieanlagen—Aktuelle Beiträge aus der Schweiz. Lärmbekämpf. 2016, 11, 159–167. [Google Scholar]
  64. Koppen, E.; Fowler, K. International legislation for wind turbine noise. In Proceedings of the Euronoise 2015, 10th European Congress and Exposition on Noise Control Engineering, Maastricht, The Netherlands, 1–3 June 2015; European Acoustical Society (EAA): Madrid, Spain; Nederlands Akoestisch Genootschap (NAG): Heerde, The Netherlands; Belgische Akoestische Vereniging (ABAV): Limelette, Belgium, 2015; pp. 321–326. [Google Scholar]
  65. Licitra, G.; Fredianelli, L. Which limits for wind turbine noise? A comparison with other types of sources using a common metric. In Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Wind Turbine Noise, Denver, CO, USA, 28–30 August 2013; Institute of Noise Control Engineering: Indianapolis, IN, USA, 2013; pp. 748–760. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. (a) Unweighted and (b) A-weighted 1/3 octave band spectra (in Leq, mean over the whole stimuli length) of the low-frequency (LF), wind turbine (WT) and pink spectral shapes for the situations without periodic and without random amplitude modulation, with a LAeq of 40 dBA. In (a), the mean spectral slopes (dashed lines: regressions of WT and LF Leq on 1/3 octave band) are shown.
Figure 1. (a) Unweighted and (b) A-weighted 1/3 octave band spectra (in Leq, mean over the whole stimuli length) of the low-frequency (LF), wind turbine (WT) and pink spectral shapes for the situations without periodic and without random amplitude modulation, with a LAeq of 40 dBA. In (a), the mean spectral slopes (dashed lines: regressions of WT and LF Leq on 1/3 octave band) are shown.
Ijerph 15 01029 g001
Figure 2. Level-time histories of the A-weighted, FAST time-weighted sound pressure level (LAF) of the stimuli with wind turbine spectral shape and a LAeq of 40 dBA, for depths of periodic amplitude modulation (AM) of 0 and 3 dB, in situations (a) without and (b) with random AM.
Figure 2. Level-time histories of the A-weighted, FAST time-weighted sound pressure level (LAF) of the stimuli with wind turbine spectral shape and a LAeq of 40 dBA, for depths of periodic amplitude modulation (AM) of 0 and 3 dB, in situations (a) without and (b) with random AM.
Ijerph 15 01029 g002
Figure 3. Level-time histories of the A-weighted, FAST time-weighted sound pressure level (LAF) of the stimuli without periodic and without random amplitude modulation, for (a) pink, (b) wind turbine and (c) low frequency spectral shapes.
Figure 3. Level-time histories of the A-weighted, FAST time-weighted sound pressure level (LAF) of the stimuli without periodic and without random amplitude modulation, for (a) pink, (b) wind turbine and (c) low frequency spectral shapes.
Ijerph 15 01029 g003
Figure 4. Laboratory setup in AuraLab at Empa, with the listening test program (graphical user interface) displayed on the screen and a porous floor absorber between loudspeaker and participant. One of the two subwoofers is visible on the right side.
Figure 4. Laboratory setup in AuraLab at Empa, with the listening test program (graphical user interface) displayed on the screen and a porous floor absorber between loudspeaker and participant. One of the two subwoofers is visible on the right side.
Ijerph 15 01029 g004
Figure 5. Boxplots of the short-term annoyance ratings as a function of (a) the LAeq (Subset I), (b) depth of periodic amplitude modulation (AM) and spectral shape (pink, wind turbine (WT), low frequency (LF); pooled data of situations with/without random AM), and (c) periodic AM and random AM (pooled data of situations with different spectral shapes) (Subset II). Boxes represent the interquartile range (25% and 75%) and the median (50%, horizontal line in boxes), whiskers the data within 1.5 times the interquartile range, and circles outliers outside the whiskers.
Figure 5. Boxplots of the short-term annoyance ratings as a function of (a) the LAeq (Subset I), (b) depth of periodic amplitude modulation (AM) and spectral shape (pink, wind turbine (WT), low frequency (LF); pooled data of situations with/without random AM), and (c) periodic AM and random AM (pooled data of situations with different spectral shapes) (Subset II). Boxes represent the interquartile range (25% and 75%) and the median (50%, horizontal line in boxes), whiskers the data within 1.5 times the interquartile range, and circles outliers outside the whiskers.
Ijerph 15 01029 g005
Figure 6. Examples of individual annoyance ratings (mean values per participant and spectral shape, pooled data of situations with/without random and/or periodic amplitude modulation) of 10 randomly selected participants as a function of spectral shape (pink, wind turbine (WT), low frequency (LF)). Different symbols connected by lines represent different participants, and the grey bold line shows the average of all 52 participants.
Figure 6. Examples of individual annoyance ratings (mean values per participant and spectral shape, pooled data of situations with/without random and/or periodic amplitude modulation) of 10 randomly selected participants as a function of spectral shape (pink, wind turbine (WT), low frequency (LF)). Different symbols connected by lines represent different participants, and the grey bold line shows the average of all 52 participants.
Ijerph 15 01029 g006
Figure 7. Mean short-term annoyance as a function of (a) the LAeq (Subset I), (b) spectral shape (pink, wind turbine (WT), low frequency (LF); pooled data of different situations of periodic and random amplitude modulation (AM)), (c) and periodic and random AM (pooled data of situations with different spectral shapes) (Subset II). Symbols represent observed values, and lines the corresponding mixed-effects model with 95% confidence intervals, in (b) as horizontal lines.
Figure 7. Mean short-term annoyance as a function of (a) the LAeq (Subset I), (b) spectral shape (pink, wind turbine (WT), low frequency (LF); pooled data of different situations of periodic and random amplitude modulation (AM)), (c) and periodic and random AM (pooled data of situations with different spectral shapes) (Subset II). Symbols represent observed values, and lines the corresponding mixed-effects model with 95% confidence intervals, in (b) as horizontal lines.
Ijerph 15 01029 g007
Figure 8. Mean short-term annoyance of Subset II as a function of (a) the sound level difference LC-A and (b) standard deviation of the A-weighted and FAST time-weighted high-pass filtered (f > 500 Hz) level-time histories of the signals (σfluc). Symbols represent observed values, in (b) as mean values per stimulus, and lines the corresponding mixed-effects model with 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 8. Mean short-term annoyance of Subset II as a function of (a) the sound level difference LC-A and (b) standard deviation of the A-weighted and FAST time-weighted high-pass filtered (f > 500 Hz) level-time histories of the signals (σfluc). Symbols represent observed values, in (b) as mean values per stimulus, and lines the corresponding mixed-effects model with 95% confidence intervals.
Ijerph 15 01029 g008
Table 1. Factorial design of the listening experiment with acoustical stimuli of different spectral shapes (low-frequency (LF), wind turbine (WT), pink), depth of periodic amplitude modulation (AM, expressed as standard deviation (σpAM) of the periodic level fluctuations), and occurrence or absence of random AM (with, no). The table shows the number of stimuli per variable combination. Except for two stimuli (cf. footnote 1), the stimuli were reproduced at a LAeq of 40 dBA.
Table 1. Factorial design of the listening experiment with acoustical stimuli of different spectral shapes (low-frequency (LF), wind turbine (WT), pink), depth of periodic amplitude modulation (AM, expressed as standard deviation (σpAM) of the periodic level fluctuations), and occurrence or absence of random AM (with, no). The table shows the number of stimuli per variable combination. Except for two stimuli (cf. footnote 1), the stimuli were reproduced at a LAeq of 40 dBA.
Depth of Periodic AM Spectral Shape
LF WT Pink
Random AM
withno withno withno
σpAM = 0.0 dB11 3 11 11 2
σpAM = 1.5 dB11 11 11
σpAM = 3.0 dB11 11 11
1 reference stimulus, additionally reproduced at a LAeq of 37 and 43 dBA, besides 40 dBA. 2 pink noise.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Schäffer, B.; Pieren, R.; Schlittmeier, S.J.; Brink, M. Effects of Different Spectral Shapes and Amplitude Modulation of Broadband Noise on Annoyance Reactions in a Controlled Listening Experiment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 1029. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15051029

AMA Style

Schäffer B, Pieren R, Schlittmeier SJ, Brink M. Effects of Different Spectral Shapes and Amplitude Modulation of Broadband Noise on Annoyance Reactions in a Controlled Listening Experiment. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018; 15(5):1029. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15051029

Chicago/Turabian Style

Schäffer, Beat, Reto Pieren, Sabine J. Schlittmeier, and Mark Brink. 2018. "Effects of Different Spectral Shapes and Amplitude Modulation of Broadband Noise on Annoyance Reactions in a Controlled Listening Experiment" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 15, no. 5: 1029. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15051029

APA Style

Schäffer, B., Pieren, R., Schlittmeier, S. J., & Brink, M. (2018). Effects of Different Spectral Shapes and Amplitude Modulation of Broadband Noise on Annoyance Reactions in a Controlled Listening Experiment. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(5), 1029. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15051029

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop