1. Background
Past research, starting from Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model, and more recent studies, including research by Fiese [
1], have all highlighted the importance of repetitive, predictable proximal processes for children’s development. Activities that occur at the same time each night, following a similar, familiar pattern, actively promote a safe and reassuring environment for children. Predictability and safety are vital components for family routines and increase compliance with a given task. Bedtime routines can be described as a recurrent, dynamic and common set of activities that take place roughly the hour before children go to bed [
1]. Previous studies on bedtime routines have shown the importance of bedtime routines in terms of quality of sleep [
2,
3], dental health [
4,
5], school performance [
6] and school readiness [
7,
8], psycho-social and emotional development [
9], overall family functioning and parental stress and confidence [
1,
8]. Moreover, intervention studies have shown that it is possible to intervene and alter these routines with subsequent benefits for children and parents alike [
10].
Bedtime routine activities can encompass a diverse range of interactive, non-interactive and hygiene activities [
10]. These activities include: dietary habits such as bottle feeding/drinks, snacks before bed; bath and other hygiene, including oral hygiene, behaviours; proactive, prosocial and positive parent–child interactions, such as being rocked, singing songs, listening to music, massage and playing; reading or sharing books and storytelling before bed, and finally, use of electronic devices including watching TV, playing or interacting with a gaming console, tablet, mobile phone or computer before bed [
10]. These activities can be further categorised into adaptive (i.e., hygiene behaviours, reading books) and maladaptive (i.e., snacks/drinks, watching TV/using electronic devices) [
11]. These activities around bedtime routines are not consistent throughout childhood, with necessary adaptations as children grow older, for example, rocking will eventually stop as the child moves from infancy to toddlerhood and beyond, while other activities, such as watching TV/using electronic devices, may become more prevalent as a child ages [
10].
An optimal bedtime routine for young children, ages 2–8 years old, should: (a) be consistent throughout the week following the recommended sleep hours for each age group, (b) include tooth brushing and other positive, beneficial hygiene behaviours such as a bath before bed, but not necessarily every night, (c) avoid drinks (such as bottle feeding) and snacks before bed, excluding water and unflavoured milk that can actually form part of the routine itself, (d) minimise the use of electronic devices, including television, around and during bedtime, and finally, (e) include book reading and book sharing activities including storytelling before bed [
2,
4,
8,
10,
12]. Despite differences in developmental processes, expert agreement has showcased the great overlap in bedtime routine characteristics for children between ages of 2 and 8 resulting in important opportunities for exploring these age groups together [
11].
Despite growing evidence for the importance of bedtime routines, there is a limited understanding of the characteristics and prevalence of bedtime routines across different countries and across populations of varying socio-economic and demographic composition [
10]. In their systematic review, Mindell and Williamson [
10] highlighted the issue of limited studies around bedtime routine characteristics. From available research, it is known that most families have some sort of routine around bedtime, but little is known about what takes place during that routine [
10]. Research in the United States has shown differences in the implementation and characteristics of bedtime routines between families of different ethnic and socioeconomic backgrounds [
12]. However, similar studies are lacking on a global scale, including in the United Kingdom (UK), and in particular, England [
13]. The same review identified the additional need for longitudinal studies that examine bedtime routines in more detail rather than through the utilisation of one-off retrospective measures. Most studies within bedtime routines deployed retrospective, paper-based questionnaires to capture data. Retrospective data collection can increase the likelihood of recall bias, while paper-based approaches allow very little room for dynamic data capturing, especially for a dynamic and complex issue such as bedtime routines. Finally, a lack of studies examining the possibility of a weekend effect on the quality of bedtime routines further limits our understanding on how to best support families to achieve optimal routines. With potentially important differences in the quality of routines throughout the week, exploring these possible changes is important.
Aims
Overall, the aim of this study is to examine bedtime routine characteristics and activities in families with young children in the North of England in order to offer some preliminary yet vital information of what unfolds during bedtime. Additionally, this study aims to utilise a real-time, dynamic approach for data collection to potentially overcome limitations found in retrospective, paper-based data collection approaches. The study aims to explore possible differences in the quality of bedtime routines between weekdays and weekends, and finally, examine the role of demographic and socio-economic characteristics on the quality of bedtime routines.
2. Methods
A cross-sectional study exploring bedtime routines characteristics in families with young children using a real-time, dynamic data collection approach was completed between February and July 2018. The study in its entirety, including consent forms and all study materials, was approved by the Health Research Authority (Integrated Research Application System (IRAS) ID: 238552).
2.1. Sample and Recruitment
In total, 200 parents were recruited with children between the ages of 3 to 7 years of age. There was an overall consent rate of 65%, with 308 people approached in total. Inclusion criteria included: (a) ability to speak and comprehend English, (b) owning a working mobile phone, not necessarily a smartphone, and (c) having children between the ages of 3 and 7 years old. Eligible participants were firstly identified by relevant staff at health and dental centres in the North of England, and initial contact was then made by a researcher who provided information about the study. Compensation (£10 shopping vouchers) was provided at the end of the study in the form of online shopping vouchers.
2.2. Data Collection
Data collection was completed by one parent, with demographic information reflecting data related to the parent who completed the questionnaire. Data collection took place over a 7-night period in order to capture a wider range of data points per participant, including fluctuations in bedtime routines occurring between weekdays and weekends. In line with a previous study on bedtime routines [
8], an automated text-survey was utilised to capture data on bedtime routines. In a previous study [
8], the automated text-survey was successfully deployed to capture data on bedtime routines in families with young children, with parents providing positive feedback on the use of the survey. The automated text-survey, identical to the one used in a previous study [
8], included both open- and close-ended questions (a breakdown of questions asked on a nightly basis is presented in
Appendix A Table A1). Questions covered all areas associated with an optimal bedtime routine, such as: time consistency (what time child went to bed), dietary habits before bed (including drinks and snacks before bed), oral hygiene behaviours (i.e., toothbrushing), use of electronic devices (including watching TV) before bed and book reading/sharing (or storytelling) before bed. During recruitment, parents agreed on a predetermined time for receiving the survey, aiming for data collection just after their routine was completed. Each night, parents could delay, defer or decide not to complete the survey. The survey was delivered to their mobile phone. Secure servers and secure online platforms were used to develop and deliver the survey to parents’ phones. All information sent back by parents was anonymised and securely kept, minimising risk to their personal information.
Information on socioeconomic and demographic characteristics were collected during recruitment through the completion of a brief demographics form. Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) scores, a commonly used metric of deprivation in England, were calculated based on the information provided by participants. Higher scores reflect higher levels of deprivation.
2.3. Data Analysis
Each night’s routine was assessed based on available guidelines on what constitutes an optimal bedtime routine. As with a previous study [
8], a score of 1 was assigned to each of the 5 selected activities (1 point for undertaking the activity and 0 points for not undertaking the activity, with reverse scoring for dietary habits before bed and use of electronic devices). Average scores for the 7 nights were calculated and used for further analyses. High scores indicate better bedtime routines. Data were descriptively analysed in order to examine frequency and prevalence of bedtime routines and activities. Since not all families responded to every night of the assessment, percentage scores were used instead of raw scores to reflect frequencies. All data were analysed using SPSS v.25 (IBM Corp. 2017, Armonk, NY, USA). Multiple comparisons were used to examine within- and between-subject variance regarding a participant’s bedtime routine scores over the study period and the possibility of a ‘weekend effect’ impacting the quality of bedtime routines. Demographic and socioeconomic information were used to examine possible effects on overall quality of bedtime routines using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Finally, in terms of missing data, two analyses were calculated: one by excluding missing data and another one with median imputation replacing missing data. No significant differences were found, and therefore, it was determined that missing data did not significantly affect the results. All data presented are based on the original dataset, with missing data excluded from analyses.
3. Results
3.1. Sample Characteristics
Out of 200 parents, 185 parents completed data collection (92% retention rate). Parents had a mean age of 34.6 (SD = 5.01), with the youngest participant being 25 years of age and the oldest 46 years of age. Most participants were female, with only 13% (
n = 24) being male.
Table 1 summarises key demographic information. Due to the areas where recruitment was undertaken, there was a significant proportion of Asian/British-Asian participants, reflecting the ethnic composition of recruitment sites.
3.2. Response Rates
All but one participant replied to at least 3 out of 7 nights. The participant who replied for only 2 nights was removed from subsequent data analyses due to a low number of replies. From the remaining 184 participants, most of them (n = 100) replied to every night of the assessment while 56 replied to 6/7 nights, 16 replied to 5/7 nights, 9 replied to 4/7 nights and finally, only 3 participants replied to 3/7 nights.
3.3. Overview of Bedtime Routine Characteristics and Activities
In total, five different bedtime routine activities were measured as part of this study. These are presented as percentage of participants who completed the activity based on their replies on a nightly basis.
Table 2 summarises the overview of bedtime routine characteristics and activities. In general, a small majority of participants reported brushing their children’s teeth every night (53%). Only 4.4% of participants reported completely avoiding snacks or drinks the hour before bed, while 36 (19.7%) reported having snacks or drinks every single night before bed. From those allowing food and/or drinks the hour before bed, 44.3% gave their children water or unflavoured milk, 16.9% allowed consumption of fruit or vegetables, while 14.3% allowed sugary or savoury snacks, including chocolate, crisps or soft drinks. Finally, a total of 25.1% of participants read to their children every night of the week, 9.3% never read or shared a book with their children during the course of the study, only 8.2% (16 out of 184) did not allow use of electronic devices at all the hour before bed, while around 30% of parents achieved time consistency (i.e., within the hour of the time that the child went to bed the night before) in getting their children to bed every night.
A repeated measurements ANOVA was completed to examine if there was an exposure effect based on how many days a participant completed the text-survey. This looked to examine any differences between the first night of the assessment vs. the remaining nights to determine changes in bedtime routine scores overtime. Due to the nature of recruiting participants, there were differences regarding the start of data collection, for example, some participants might have started their data collection on a Monday (night 1), while someone else might have started on a Friday (night 1). A significant difference was observed for night 1 vs. night 2 (F(1, 92) = 5.194, p < 0.001), night 1 vs. night 4 (F(1, 92) = 6.186, p < 0.001) and night 1 vs. night 5 (F(1, 92) = 7.886, p < 0.001). On the contrary, there was no significant change between night 1 vs. night 3, night 1 vs. night 6 and night 1 vs. night 7. The results of the analysis showed no clear exposure effect, even though there were significant differences between the first night of completing the survey and other nights later in the week. Effects of demographics and socioeconomic characteristics (education level, IMD score, ethnicity, employment status and household type) were included in this analysis, with no significant modifications regarding observed differences.
3.4. Examining Weekend Effects on Quality of Bedtime Routines
Examining possible effects of weekend nights compared to weekday nights was conducted using repeated measures ANOVAs. Possible changes in overall bedtime routines’ quality were examined by comparing Saturday and Sunday scores with scores on different nights of the weeks. The results of this analysis showed that across all other nights (Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday, Friday), there were significant differences in the overall bedtime routine scores when compared to Saturday and Sunday scores.
Table 3 presents the results of this analysis comparing Saturday and Sunday results to the rest of the week. Effects of demographics and socioeconomic characteristics were included in this analysis, with no significant modifications regarding observed differences.
3.5. Examining Effect of Demographic and Economic Characteristics on Quality of Bedtime Routines
A series of one-way ANOVAs was calculated to examine possible effects of demographic and socioeconomic characteristics affecting the overall quality of bedtime routines. Employment status, education, age of parent, age of children, number of children, ethnicity, household type and IMD scores were included in this analysis. The results of the analysis showed that only employment had a significant effect on overall quality of bedtime routines (F(1, 175) = 7.151, p < 0.05). Post-hoc comparisons between the different types of employment showed that there were significant differences in mean bedtime routine scores between full-time vs. part-time employed parents and full-time vs. stay at home parents. Parents employed full-time were shown to have, on average, a lower (and therefore less optimal) bedtime routine score (mean = 2.7, SD = 0.64) compared to parents working part-time (mean = 3.23, SD = 0.89) and stay at home parents (mean = 3.23, SD = 0.74). Based on these results, it appears that both stay at home parents and parents working part-time achieved better overall bedtime routine scores when compared to those parents who work full-time.
4. Discussion
So far, little is known about bedtime routines in families with young children living in the North of England, the possibility of a weekend effect on the quality of bedtime routines and the impact of socioeconomic and demographic factors that can affect bedtime routines. Similar to other studies [
10], key results from this study show that all families implement one form of routine around bedtime each night, and while some families manage to achieve optimal routines, other struggle. Additionally, this study offered some preliminary evidence of a difference in the quality of bedtime routines between weekdays and the weekend, with routines being significantly worse in the latter period. Finally, employment status appears to affect the quality of bedtime routines.
4.1. Importance of Bedtime Routine Activities in Overall Child Wellbeing and Development
The present study simply examined prevalence, frequency and characteristics of bedtime routines in families with young children. No measurements on the impact of those routines on areas such as sleep, parental mood and family functioning were included. Therefore, there is no basis for causal or associative connections between the results of this study and the impact of optimal vs. suboptimal bedtime routines. However, and based on available data regarding bedtime routines, successful and consistent implementation of an optimal bedtime routine can have important, long-term benefits for children and their families alike. Good oral hygiene behaviours, including frequent tooth brushing before bed as well as avoidance of food and/or drinks (excluding water and unflavoured milk) before bed, can lead to overall better dental health [
12,
14]. Lack of dental problems early in life can then have positive implications for a child’s overall development, leading to avoidance of dental extractions, dental pain, loss of sleep due to dental pain and missing days in school [
15,
16,
17]. Apart from dental health, good dietary habits the hour before bed have shown important associations with obesity rates [
18,
19]. In this study, it was found that just over half (53%) of parents managed to consistently brush their children’s teeth every night, with around 1.1% never reporting brushing their children’s teeth over the course of the data collection process. Moreover, most parents (175 out of 184) allowed either snacks or drinks other than water or unflavoured milk the hour before bed, resulting in problematic dietary habits for their young children.
Book reading, sharing a book with children as part of the bedtime routine or simply storytelling can promote child literacy, improve school performance and enhance school readiness in young children, with subsequent possible implications in later achievement and attainment [
20,
21]. Based on the results of this study, around 10% of parents never read to their children before bed, while another 36.1% read or shared a book with their children for less than half of the nights observed. Book reading or sharing a book with a young child as part of his/her bedtime routine is a crucial element for successful later development, and therefore it is a behaviour that needs proactive and consistent participation from parents.
Having a consistent, appropriate time that children go to bed could aid in achieving adequate hours of sleep. In addition, maintaining the routine around a stable and protected time each night can further reinforce the formation of habits and rituals for the family and the child [
22]. Finally, optimal routines can lead to better family functioning with less behavioural issues (i.e., tantrums, bedtime resistance), while enhancing parent–child relationships and interactions and improving parental socio-emotional wellbeing [
23,
24,
25]. In terms of time consistency, this is the only area where a larger majority of parents either achieved it every night or tried and achieved it for at least half of the nights they replied to the text-surveys. Overall, 67.2% of parents managed to achieve consistency in getting children to bed, with only 5.5% or 10 out of 184 people failing to do so every single night of the data collection period.
The only activity where results presented a more mixed picture between those with overall optimal and those with overall sub-optimal bedtime routines was use of electronic devices before bed. Use of electronic devices at some point during the week was reported from 92% of the sample. Based on available data collected from the automated text-survey, it is not possible to distinguish what type of engagement with electronic devices those 92% had. Use of electronic devices is a broad subject, ranging from watching TV to reading an e-book on a portable reader. The recent rise in access rates to electronic devices from a younger age and time spent in front of a screen has prompted a more robust look at possible effects and implications. Recent studies have shown that prolonged exposure to electronic devices can lead to overall issues around development, with particularly important implications around sleep as well as cognitive, educational and behavioural development [
26]. With a possible important link between the use of electronic devices and overall child wellbeing and development, it is important to further explore the types of interactions and uses of those devices in the context of bedtime routines.
Finally, results showed evidence of a “weekend” effect, where the quality of bedtime routines deteriorates closer to and around the weekend when compared to weekdays or school-nights. This is an interesting and previously non-reported finding. Anecdotal evidence could suggest that parents and families at large might alter their behaviours between weekday and weekend nights, with Friday and Saturday nights commonly referred to as “weekend” nights. The mere labelling of some nights as school-nights and other nights as weekend nights can provide sufficient justification for the creation of cognitive, at first, and later, practical and behavioural modifications, and altered expectations on what children are supposed to do and not do. There is no specific evidence base for this hypothesis, but the conscious and sub-conscious power that simple labels might have on people’s behaviours and expectations has been previously observed [
27]. It is also possible that Saturday (and the weekend at large) activities are distinct, with each family engaging in specific activities with a specific meaning for them, but this needs to be further explored in subsequent studies.
4.2. Demographic, Socioeconomic Factors and Implications for Bedtime Routines
Previous studies have reported differences in prevalence and frequency of bedtime routines between white and non-white samples, as well as samples from predominately Asian countries and regions [
28,
29]. Despite prior evidence of significant differences in the quality and frequency of bedtime routines amongst different ethnic groups [
10], this study failed to replicate this difference. Additionally, with a long-established misrepresentation of minority ethnic groups in health research [
30], it is crucial for studies to be able to include and report on communities that are routinely overlooked. Examination of diverse populations can allow for better and more inclusive findings for a recurrent family activity that transcends sociodemographic and ethnic boundaries. This study utilised a diverse sample, reflecting the composition of local communities where recruitment took place.
4.3. Limitations
Risk of bias, especially desirability bias, is probably the most important possible limitation of this study. As with every self-reported measure, it is difficult to control for the effects of this type of bias since it is the responsibility of the participant to reply with accuracy and honesty, with no means of guaranteeing the accuracy of their responses. The development and deployment of the text-survey assessment of bedtime routines aimed at reducing effects of desirability bias by introducing a more intrinsic approach that utilised a rapid and more automated response pattern from participants. Additionally, missing data is another limitation, despite attempts to manage the implication of missing data on subsequent analyses. Lack of complementary measurements, especially around children’s sleep, further limits our overall understanding on the quality of bedtimes and how they can affect an important area of child wellbeing and development. The bedtime routines survey used in this study was also not validated, despite being used successfully in previously published research with a similar population. Finally, the lack of information on both parents and the focus on one parent specifically hinders our analyses, since there is no clear, holistic picture of family life and family dynamics. More detailed and in-depth demographic questions would have allowed for a better exploration of family structures and how they might have affected bedtime routines.