Next Article in Journal
Efficacy of Combining an Extraoral High-Volume Evacuator with Preprocedural Mouth Rinsing in Reducing Aerosol Contamination Produced by Ultrasonic Scaling
Next Article in Special Issue
Psychometric Analysis and Contribution to the Evaluation of the Exams-Related Emotions Scale in Primary and Secondary School Students
Previous Article in Journal
Early Return to Daily Life through Immediate Weight-Bearing after Lateral Malleolar Fracture Surgery
Previous Article in Special Issue
Variables Influencing Professors’ Adaptation to Digital Learning Environments during the COVID-19 Pandemic
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Validation of an Academic Self-Attribution Questionnaire for Primary and Secondary School Students: Implications of Gender and Grade

by
Ana I. Obregón-Cuesta
1,
Paula Rodríguez-Fernández
2,*,
Benito León-del-Barco
3,
Santiago Mendo-Lázaro
3,
Luis A. Mínguez-Mínguez
4,
Josefa González-Santos
5 and
Jerónimo J. González-Bernal
5
1
Department of Mathematics and Computing, University of Burgos, 09001 Burgos, Spain
2
Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Universidad Isabel I, 09003 Burgos, Spain
3
Department of Psychology and Anthropology, University of Extremadura, 10071 Cáceres, Spain
4
Department of Education Sciences, University of Burgos, 09001 Burgos, Spain
5
Department of Health Sciences, University of Burgos, 09001 Burgos, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(10), 6045; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19106045
Submission received: 30 March 2022 / Revised: 9 May 2022 / Accepted: 10 May 2022 / Published: 16 May 2022

Abstract

:
The way in which students attribute causes to their successes and failures in school has important implications for their development. The objectives of our research were to validate the Academic Success and Failure Attribution Questionnaire (ASFAQ) and to analyze the gender and grade differences in the ASFAQ data for primary and secondary school students in Spain. For the construction and analysis of the psychometric characteristics of the scale, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) were performed. To compare the ASFAQ scores based on gender and school year, a parametric t-test for independent samples and a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used. A total of 562 students in the fifth (n = 228) and sixth year (n = 186) of primary studies and the first (n = 134) and second year (n = 94) of secondary studies participated in the research. The results showed the adequate factorial structure, internal consistency, and validity of the ASFAQ, in addition to statistically significant differences by gender and school year. This research provides scientific evidence about the psychometric properties of the ASFAQ to assess and understand attributional style in the educational context, as well as current and consistent empirical evidence related to gender and grade differences in the attributional patterns of academic success and failure for primary and secondary school students.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Attribution can be defined as the process of assigning causes to one’s own behavior or that of other people. This attribution can be internal, called personal or dispositional attribution, or external, which is known as situational attribution. The development of attributional styles theory is the result of scientific research on the search and assignment of human behavior causes [1].
Weiner’s Attribution Theory [2] is one of the most used in the study of attributional styles. The theory defines academic attribution as the explanation that individuals give for their academic performance in school, whether successful or not [3]. When people attribute the causes of their behavior to factors that do not depend on themselves, they act using an external or situational style of attribution. In contrast, the internal, personal, or dispositional, style is characterized by a causal explanation based on the relationship between the action itself and the result [4,5]. Weiner et al. [2] found in their research that students used the skill, effort, luck and difficulty of the task as the main reasons to explain their academic successes and failures [6].
The attributional style chosen can favor or hinder learning and determines the motivation with which students engage and cope with academic demands, even influencing their perception of themselves and their performance [6]. Its practical implications are increasingly evident [7,8], and school failure can be largely explained by the use of ineffective and maladaptive attributional styles [9]. Since having been identified as important determinants for student learning and self-esteem [10,11,12,13], attributions have been associated with variables such as anxiety and depression or academic performance [14,15,16] and self-efficacy [17].
The academic attributional style acquires special relevance in primary and secondary school students due to their current stage of development [18], and the way in which they attribute causes to their successes and failures in school has important implications for their development [18]. When students attribute their results only to external causes, they make little effort toward their learning and evolution, while if they explain their results with the belief they are lacking in intellectual capacity, their confidence and school performance will be seriously affected [9]. Along the same lines, if you associate success with your own effort or ability, you will probably feel pride and the motivation to continue obtaining good results [17]. Normandeau y Gobeil [19] stated in their study that changes in causal attributions are the result of metacognitive developments in children about themselves as problem solvers and that these determine their emotional reaction and orientation towards the task.
Gender has been shown to play an important role in attributional patterns in the school context. Most of the studies in which gender differences in student attributions have been analyzed report greater self-concept and self-esteem in boys, who frequently attribute their failures to external and unstable causes and their successes to internal causes, as opposed to girls [20,21,22,23]. However, a consistent attributional trend based on gender cannot be affirmed, since it can vary depending on other variables such as the academic content area or the school year [23].
In order to promote adaptive attributional styles, valid and reliable assessment instruments aimed at specific population groups are required. Taking into account the relevance of this aspect to the school context, the main we objective proposed was to validate the Academic Success and Failure Attribution Questionnaire (ASFAQ) with a sample of primary and secondary school students in Spain (Supplementary Materials), hypothesizing that the ASFAQ would be a rigorous scientific instrument with the adequate validity and reliability to evaluate the attributions of academic success and failure in primary and secondary school students. In addition, we intended to study gender and school year differences in order to provide current and consistent empirical evidence. Along these lines, and considering the findings of previous studies, we expected to find attributional differences based on gender and school year.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample consisted of 562 students of Compulsory Primary Education (CPE) and Compulsory Secondary Education (CSE), aged between 10 and 14 years (mean (M) = 11.66; standard deviation (SD) = 1.21). A total of 284 subjects belonged to the male gender and 278 to the female. The CPE students (n = 334) were in their fifth (n = 228) and sixth school years (n = 186), and those in CSE (n = 148) belonged to the first (n = 134) and second school years (n = 94). The sample was collected from 5 different schools, both public (n = 4) and private (n = 1), in the Autonomous Community of Castilla y León.
The selection of the sample was performed by conglomerates.

2.2. Instruments

The Academic Success and Failure Attribution Scales were designed with the objective of evaluating the attributions of academic success and failure. The scales consist of twelve items in Likert format with five intervals in numerical form from 1 (Not at all agree) to 5 (Totally agree). The items aim to adequately show the most relevant contents of the construct for evaluation. In this sense, students attribute their successes and failures mainly to four elements: ability, effort, task difficulty and luck; classified according to three dimensions: locus of causality, stability and controllability [24]. The locus of causality can be internal (skill, effort) or external (chance, difficulty of the task) and the causes can be stable (ability) or unstable (effort, luck) [25].

2.3. Procedure

Following the ethical guidelines of the American Psychological Association regarding consent, confidentiality and anonymity in the answers, a member of the research group contacted school principals and explained to them the objectives of the research.
Not all the schools contacted decided to collaborate in the research. Those who did not participate indicated a lack of time in the classroom and the difficulty of obtaining parental consent as causes.
Once the collaboration was accepted, the participants were contacted in the classrooms, and after obtaining their parents’ or legal guardians’ informed consent, they proceeded to fill in the scales. Its completion was anonymous, guaranteeing the confidentiality of the data obtained and its exclusive use for research purposes. The administration of the scales was performed during school hours, with explanations as to how they should complete it and the answering of any questions that arose during the process. We insisted the nature of the investigation be kept anonymous. The questionnaires were completed individually in a suitable environment and without distractions. The process of completing the questionnaires lasted approximately 15 min.
No questionnaire was rejected.
All the students in the grades selected by the principals were included in the research. No boy or girl was excluded based on their culture, language, religion, race, disability, sexual orientation, ethnicity, gender or age.
The Bioethics Committee of the University of Burgos approved the research (Reference UBU 032/2021), respecting all the requirements established in the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975.

2.4. Data Analysis

Initially, for the construction and analysis of the psychometric characteristics of the scales, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was performed. Once the EFA was performed, the factorial structure found was confirmed by a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The reliability of the scale factors was calculated using Cronbach’s alpha, the Composite Reliability coefficients, McDonald’s Omega and the Extracted Mean Variance. The EFA was performed using the SPSS-21 program; for the CFA, the AMOS-21 program was used.
To compare the scores obtained in the ASFAQ on the basis of gender, a parametric t-test for independent samples was used, while the analysis on the basis of school year was performed using the one-way ANOVA test. A statistical significance value of p < 0.05 was established, using SPSS version 25 software (IBM-Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

The original sample (n = 562) was divided into two randomly drawn subsamples (n1 = 276 and n2 = 276). The first (n1) was used to perform the EFA and the second (n2) as a validation sample for the CFA. Both subsamples were equivalent based on gender, χ2(1) = 0.359, p = 0.549, and age, t (560) = 0.285, p = 0.776

3.1. Exploratory Factor Analysis

Two exploratory analyses were performed for the Attribution Scales of success and failure, respectively.

3.1.1. Exploratory Factor Analysis of the Attributions of Academic Success

The sample adequacy measure (Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test = 0.828) and Bartlett’s sphericity test (χ2 = 1826.282 (66), p < 0.001) justified the factor analysis. Using Kaiser’s rule [26] eigenvalues greater than unity and the unweighted least squares extraction method with Varimax rotation, a solution of three factors was obtained (Table 1) that together explained 55.9% of the variance. The first factor, “Controllable internal attributions” (four items), explained 21.8% of the variance and the collected information on the attributions with an internal location, in which the students were able to exert some influence or control over the cause to which the problem was attributed. The second factor, “Uncontrollable internal attributions” (four items), explained 21% of the variance and the collected information on the attributions with an internal location in which people could not exert influence or control over the cause to which the problem was attributed. The third factor, “Uncontrollable external attributions” (four items), explained 13.1% of the variance and the collected information related to the attributions with an external location in which people could not exert influence or control over the cause to which the problem was attributed.
Cronbach’s alpha of factors 1 (α = 0.744), 2 (α = 0.781) and 3 (α = 0.734) demonstrated an acceptable level of internal consistency.

3.1.2. Exploratory Factor Analysis of the Attributions of Academic Failure

The sample adequacy measure (KMO = 0.902) and Bartlett’s test of sphericity (χ2 = 3566.131 (66), p < 0.001) justified the factor analysis. Using Kaiser’s rule eigenvalues greater than unity and the unweighted least squares extraction method with Varimax rotation, a solution of three factors was obtained (Table 2) that together explained 70.2% of the variance. The first factor, “Controllable internal attributions” (four items), explained 24.5% of the variance and the collected information on the attributions with an internal location in which people could exert some influence or control over the cause to which the problem was attributed. The second factor, “Uncontrollable internal attributions” (four items), explained 23.5% of the variance and the collected information on the attributions with an internal location in which people could not exert influence or control over the cause to which the problem is attributed. The third factor, “Uncontrollable external attributions” (four items), explained 22.2% of the variance and the collected information about attributions with an external location in which people could not exert influence or control over the cause to which it was attributed.
Cronbach’s alpha of factors 1 (α = 0.876), 2 (α = 0.848) and 3 (α = 0.827) indicated an adequate level of internal consistency.

3.2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The CFA for each attribution scale (success and failure) was performed with the second subsample (n2 = 276) with the aim of confirming the structures of three factors found in the AFE and whether they were related or independent.
Taking into account some of the most-used fit indices (χ2, χ2/gl, Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and Standarized Root Mean-Square (SRMR)) using the maximum likelihood method, four models of attributions for success and four of attributions for failure. They are as follows: M1: Three related factors; M2: three first order factors and one second order factor that brings together the first two factors; M3 three independent factors; M4: a single factor. The χ2 should acquire non-significant values (p > 0.05), the χ2/gl was considered acceptable when it was less than 5; values greater than 0.90 of the incremental indices (CFI and TLI) and ≥0.08 of the RMSEA [27] and SRMR [28] were considered acceptable [29].

3.2.1. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Success Attributions

All the successful attribution models (Table 3) presented a significant chi2 value (p < 0.05). Models 3 and 4 were discarded, since the significant chi2 square values and the CFI, TLI, RMSEA and SRMR indices did not approach the ideal values. The CFI, TLI indices of models 1 and 2 presented values above 0.92; model 1 presented the best fit, with a lower value of χ2/gl, higher CFI and TLI fit indices and lower values of the RMSEA and SRMR indicators (Table 3).
The t-values (ranging from 8.38 to 16.76) of the non-standardized regression coefficients of model 1 were statistically significant. The ranges of the standardized coefficients for factor one (0.650–0.834), two (0.604–0.803) and three (0.601–0.781) demonstrated the consistency of the indicators for the measurement of the constructs, these being clearly related (Figure 1).
In addition, the coefficients of the Extracted Mean Variance (EMV and Composite Reliability (CR), with values greater than 0.50 of EMV and 0.80 of CR, showed evidence of reliability in the model for three related factors: (F1 [four items]: EMV = 0.519, CR = 0.811; F2 [four items]: EMV = 0.524, CR = 0.813; F3 [four items]: EMV = 0.509, EMV = 0.804.

3.2.2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Failure Attributions

All failure attribution models (Table 3) present a significant chi2 value (p < 0.05). Again, models 3 and 4 were discarded, since the CFI, TLI, RMSEA and SRMR indices did not approach ideal values. The CFI, TLI indices of models 1 and 2 presented values above 0.95; Model 1 presented a better fit with a lower value of χ2/gl, higher CFI and TLI fit indices, and lower values of the RMSEA and SRMR indicators (Table 4).
The t-values (ranging from 15.31 to 20.40) of the non-standardized regression coefficients of model 1 were statistically significant. The ranges of the standardized coefficients for factor one (0.725–0.836), two (0.694–0.865) and three (0.720–0.870) were related in a statistically significant way, demonstrating the consistency of the indicators for the measure of the constructs (Figure 2).

3.3. Attributional Differences Based on Gender and School Year

3.3.1. Analysis of the ASFAQ According to Gender

The results of the inferential analysis showed statistically significant differences in the attributions of academic success according to gender, but not in those of academic failure (Table 5). More specifically, the male participants demonstrated a greater attribution of their academic success to uncontrollable internal causes (p <.001) such as intelligence, good memory, talent or calm character; as well as external causes (p <.001) such as easy exams, good luck, low demands or good explanations from teachers. No significant differences were found in the controllable internal attributions of academic success according to gender (p = 0.057).

3.3.2. Analysis of the ASFAQ According to the School Year

Statistically significant differences were found depending on the academic year in all the factors (Table 6). In general, the attributions of academic success decreased as the academic year increased. The fifth grade primary students reported the highest average scores in the controllable and uncontrollable internal and external attributions, followed by the sixth grade primary students, first grade secondary students, and finally, second grade secondary students.
Regarding the attributions of academic failure, statistically significant differences were found in the internal attributions between the students in fifth grade primary and second grade of secondary school (p = 0.001); The latter demonstrating a greater attribution of academic failure to aspects such as spending little time preparing for exams, making little effort in class, not paying attention in class, or not using appropriate strategies to prepare for exams. Statistically significant differences were also obtained in the external attributions of academic failure, where the students in the first cycle of secondary school tended to attribute their failures to aspects such as bad luck or the difficulty of the exams.

4. Discussion

The main objective of this study was to validate a questionnaire designed to evaluate the attributions of academic success and failure in primary and secondary school students. The relevance of this study comes from the need to develop an instrument tailored to this specific population that considers the school context, since the way in which students attribute causes to their successes and failures during this stage has been shown to have important implications in their development [18], as well as in academic and social results [14,15,18,19,30].
The results of the exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis performed revealed the adequate factorial structure as well the internal consistency and validity of the instrument. In addition, the ASFAQ is invariant by gender, so it is an appropriate instrument with which to evaluate the attributions of academic success and failure in primary and secondary school students.
To date, no specific instrument has been validated that analyzes the attributional styles of primary and secondary school children and adolescents in a Spanish academic context, though this would entail great benefits in a fundamental stage of their evolutionary development. Most refer to the general population or university students [31]. Rotter [32] designed the Locus of Control Scale (LOC), one of the most widely used instruments created to assess the attributional style in groups of students, composed of 29 items which measure individual differences in generalized expectations for internal and external control. There are other scales aimed at the university academic field, such as the Multidimensional-Multiattributional Causality Scale [33], Attributional Style Questionnaire [34], Multidimensional Attribution Scale [35], Academic Attributional Style Questionnaire (AASQ) [30], Sydney Attribution Scale [36] and the General Achievement Attributional Motivation Scale [37]. Similarly, for adolescent students, there are scales such as an adaptation of the Strategy and Attribution Questionnaire [31].
In this sense, the main contribution of this project is that it enables the valid and reliable evaluation of the attributions of academic success and failure in the educational context of primary and secondary school, providing useful information for the improvement of variables such as motivation, self-efficacy or academic performance, and even the improvement of learning approaches and students’ regulation [1]. Along these lines, if educational failure is explained in terms of ineffective learning or attributional strategies, the student will be able to select or develop more effective ones [9].
Previous research with university students found a statistically significant positive relationship between adaptive attributional styles and variables such as academic performance, orientation to the learning goal, and academic self-concept [38,39]. Furthermore, psycho-emotional variables related to anxiety and depression have been associated with external attributional patterns in positive situations and with internal and stable attributional patterns in negative situations [40]. Haller et al. [41] found in their study that adolescents with anxiety had a significant tendency to interpret situations more negatively. When compared to those without anxiety; students with anxious and depressive symptoms were prone to develop maladaptive attributional styles that triggered cognitive interference in attention and processing in view of the perception of a threat [42]; they also experienced the presence of interpretive biases in threatening [43] or ambiguous social contexts [44] and less executive functioning [45].
Regarding the second objective of the research, based on the analysis of gender and grade differences in the attributions of academic success and failure, the results showed that boys attributed their academic successes to uncontrollable internal causes (intelligence, good memory, talent) or external causes (easy exams, good luck, undemanding class work); but no differences were found with respect to girls in controllable internal attributions such as effort or dedication, or in attributions for academic failure. These results coincide with those obtained by Almeida et al. [20], where the male students attributed their good academic results to their abilities and the female students to their effort and personal work. For their part, Inglés et al. [46] found that the attributional style of the male students is somewhat more adaptive than that of the female students, since they attribute success to ability (internal cause, stable and uncontrollable) and failures to lack of effort (internal cause, unstable and controllable), which tends to preserve their self-esteem and self-concept [20].
Although most empirical research aimed at analyzing gender differences in students’ attributional patterns supports these findings [46], factors such as age and academic year have also been shown to have a significant implication in academic self-attributions [23,47,48,49]. Students in lower courses tend to attribute their academic results to external causes significantly more, such as study methods or the teacher, and they take more responsibility for their achievements and failures as they progress through the course, attributing them more to internal causes [23,48,49,50]. These results partially coincide with those obtained in the present investigation, where students from lower grades reported a greater tendency to attribute their academic successes to external and internal causes; and students from higher courses attributed their academic failures to internal controllable and external causes the most. Inglés et al. [46] showed a mixed attributional pattern in the lower grade students with respect to the rest of the grades, as they attributed their successes to a greater extent to controllable internal and external causes. These findings are in line with those obtained in this study, with lower grade students being the ones who most explained their successes by appealing to both internal and external causal factors.
The statistically significant differences for gender and academic year in students’ attributional patterns reiterate the importance of considering and promoting adaptive attributional patterns in the educational setting, characterized by attributions of successes to internal causes such as ability and effort and of failures to unstable causes such as a lack of effort or bad luck [9]. For this, the ASFAQ facilitates a correct evaluation of the attributions of academic success and failure and a greater controllability and malleability of the attributional strategies and styles in the educational context of primary and secondary school. Although the ASFAQ presents sufficient evidence for validity and reliability, it has limitations, such as the difficulty in generalizing the results to other population groups, which compromises the external validity (population and ecological) of the questionnaire, as well as establishing validity evidence that is convergent and discriminant. That is why the replication and expansion of the study, in addition to confirming the factorial structure, would allow for the establishment of evidence for convergent and discriminant validity.

5. Conclusions

This study demonstrates the psychometric properties of the ASFAQ for the evaluation of attributions of academic success and failure. In this way, the present research provides a scientific and rigorous instrument, with adequate validity and reliability, to determine the attributional styles used within the educational context of primary and secondary school and promote adaptive attitudes and behaviors that favor learning. In addition, the statistically significant differences in gender and academic year that were found in this research provide updated and relevant information for the development of psychoeducational interventions aimed at correcting students’ maladaptive attributional styles.
The limitation of the research lies in the fact that it was carried out on a sample of students from a single geographical region, the autonomous community of Castilla and Leon in Spain.
Future researchers should analyze the ASFAQ factor structure within different student populations, with the aim of improving the tool. In addition, future studies should analyze the structure of invariance with respect to other variables such as region, county, center size or type of center (e.g., private, public or concerted). Finally, it would be especially interesting to carry out a longitudinal study with the same students over the course of several academic years.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph19106045/s1. Academic Success and Failure Attribution Questionnaire.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.I.O.-C., B.L.-d.-B., S.M.-L., L.A.M.-M. and J.J.G.-B.; methodology, A.I.O.-C., P.R.-F., B.L.-d.-B. and S.M.-L.; software, P.R.-F., B.L.-d.-B. and S.M.-L.; validation, P.R.-F., B.L.-d.-B., S.M.-L. and J.J.G.-B.; formal analysis, P.R.-F., B.L.-d.-B. and S.M.-L.; investigation, A.I.O.-C., P.R.-F., B.L.-d.-B., S.M.-L. and J.J.G.-B.; resources, A.I.O.-C., B.L.-d.-B., S.M.-L. and J.J.G.-B.; data curation, P.R.-F., B.L.-d.-B. and S.M.-L.; writing—original draft preparation, P.R.-F.; writing—review and editing, P.R.-F.; visualization, P.R.-F., L.A.M.-M. and J.G.-S.; supervision, B.L.-d.-B., S.M.-L. and J.J.G.-B.; project administration, A.I.O.-C., B.L.-d.-B., S.M.-L. and J.J.G.-B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Burgos (UBU 032/2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Díaz, A.C.; Aguirre, S.I.; Jiménez, C.; Jurado, P.J. Attributive styles in university students: Comparisons by gender. Form. Univ. 2020, 13, 111–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Weiner, B. Attribution, Emotion, and Action. In Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior; Sorrentino, R.M., Higgins, E.T., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1986; pp. 281–312. [Google Scholar]
  3. Mok, M.M.C.; Kennedy, K.J.; Moore, P.J. Academic attribution of secondary students: Gender, year level and achievement level. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 31, 87–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Mkumbo, K.A.K.; Amani, J. Perceived University Students’ Attributions of Their Academic Success and Failure. Asian Soc. Sci. 2012, 8, 247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Gladstone, T.R.G.; Kaslow, N.J. Depression and Attributions in Children and Adolescents: A Meta-Analytic Review. J. Abnorm. Child. Psychol. 1995, 23, 597–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Lam, S.F.; Yim, P.S.; Law, J.S.F.; Cheung, R.W.Y. The effects of competition on achievement motivation in Chinese classrooms. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 74, 281–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Weiner, B. The Attribution Approach to Emotion and Motivation: History, Hypotheses, Home Runs, Headaches/Heartaches. Emot. Rev. 2014, 6, 353–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Graham, S.; Folkes, V.S. (Eds.) Attribution Theory: Applications to Achievement, Mental Health, and Interpersonal Conflict; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  9. Chan, L.K.S.; Moore, P.J. Development of attributional beliefs and strategic knowledge in years 5–9: A longitudinal analysis. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 161–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Borkowski, J.G.; Chan, L.K.; Muthukrishna, N. A Process-Oriented Model of Metacognition: Links Between Motivation and Executive Functioning Motivation and Executive Functioning. UNL Digital Commons, 2012. Available online: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/burosmetacognition (accessed on 15 September 2021).
  11. Nerstad, C.G.L.; Roberts, G.C.; Richardsen, A.M. Achieving success at work: Development and validation of the Motivational Climate at Work Questionnaire (MCWQ). J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 43, 2231–2250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Zimmerman, B.J. Self-Efficacy: An Essential Motive to Learn. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 25, 82–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Zimmerman, B.J.; Kitsantas, A. Acquiring writing revision skill: Shifting from process to outcome self-regulatory goals. J. Educ. Psychol. 1999, 91, 241–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Álvarez, A.; Suárez, N.; Tuero, E.; Núñez, J.C.; Valle, A.; Regueiro, B. Family involvement, adolescent self-concept and academic performance. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2015, 5, 293–311. [Google Scholar]
  15. Fernández-Bustos, J.G.; González-Martí, I.; Contreras, O.; Cuevas, R. Relationship between body image and physical self-concept in adolescent women. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 2015, 47, 25–33. [Google Scholar]
  16. Mkumbo, K.A.K.; Amani, J. Students’ attitudes towards school-based sex and relationships education in Tanzania View project. Asian Soc. Sci. 2012, 8, 247–255. [Google Scholar]
  17. Becerra-González, C.E.; Redil-Martínez, L.M. Motivation, Self-Efficacy, Attributional Style and Academic Performance of High School Students. Redie 2015, 17, 79–93. [Google Scholar]
  18. Lohbeck, A.; Grube, D.; Moschner, B. Academic self-concept and causal attributions for success and failure amongst elementary school children. Int. J. Early Years Educ. 2017, 25, 190–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Normandeau, S.; Gobeil, A. A Developmental Perspective on Children’s Understanding of Causal Attributions in Achievement-related Situations. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1998, 22, 611–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Almeida, L.S.; Miranda, L.; Guisande, M.A. Causal attributions for academic success and failure. Estud. Psicol. 2008, 25, 169–176. [Google Scholar]
  21. Eccles-Parsons, J.; Adler, T.; Meece, J.L. Sex differences in achievement: A test of alternate theories. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 46, 26–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Frieze, I.H.; Whitley, B.E.; Hanusa, B.; McHugh, M.C. Assessing the theoretical models for sex differences in causal attributions for success and failure. Sex. Roles 1982, 8, 333–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Inglés, C.J.; Díaz-Herrero, A.; García-Fernández, J.M.; Ruiz-Esteban, C. Gender and academic year as predictors of attributions in reading and mathematics in students of Compulsory Secondary Education. An. Psicol. 2011, 27, 381–388. [Google Scholar]
  24. Weiner, B. Attributional Thoughts about Consumer Behavior. J. Consum. Res. 2000, 27, 382–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Ibarra-Tancara, J. Atribucional styles in the perception of academic achievement and interpersonal relations in pregrado students of psychology. Rev. Psicol. 2019, 21, 73–88. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kaiser, H.F. The Application of Electronic Computers to Factor Analysis. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2016, 20, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Browne, M.W.; Cudeck, R. Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit. Sociol. Methods Res. 1993, 21, 230–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hu, L.T.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Hu, L.T.; Bentler, P.M. Evaluating model fit. In Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications; Hoyle, R.H., Ed.; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1995; pp. 76–79. [Google Scholar]
  30. Peterson, C.; Barrett, L.C. Explanatory Style and Academic Performance Among University Freshmen. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 53, 603–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Heikkilä, A.; Niemivirta, M.; Nieminen, J.; Lonka, K. Interrelations among university students’ approaches to learning, regulation of learning, and cognitive and attributional strategies: A person oriented approach. High. Educ. 2010, 61, 513–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Rotter, J.B. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol. Monogr. 1966, 80, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Lefcourt, H.M.; von Baeyer, C.L.; Ware, E.E.; Cox, D.J. The multidimensional-multiattributional causality scale: The development of a goal specific locus of control scale. Can. J. Behav. Sci. Can. Sci. Comport. 1979, 11, 286–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Peterson, C.; Semmel, A.; Yon Baeyer, C.; Abramson, L.Y.; Metalsky, G.I.; Seligman, M.E.P. The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognit. Ther. Res. 1982, 6, 287–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Tapia, J.A. Causality attribution and achievement motivation. II: Evolutive study of attributions influence in manifest level of achievement motivation. Stud. Psychol. 2014, 5, 27–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Inglés, C.J.; Rodríguez-Marín, J.; González-Pienda, J.A. Adaptation of the Sydney Attribution Scale in a Spanish college population. Psicothema 2008, 20, 166–173. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  37. Durán-Aponte, E.; Pujol, L. Attribution Scale of General Achievement Motivation (EAML-G): Adaptation and analysis of its psychometric properties. Estud. Pedagóg. 2013, 39, 83–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Mayora-Pernía, C.A. Locus of control and academic performance in university education: A bibliographic review. Rev. Electrónica Educ. 2015, 19, 1–23. [Google Scholar]
  39. Albert, M.A.; Dahling, J.J. Learning goal orientation and locus of control interact to predict academic self-concept and academic performance in college students. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2016, 97, 245–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Moore, D.W.; Schultz, N.R. Loneliness at adolescence: Correlates, attributions, and coping. J. Youth Adolesc. 1983, 12, 95–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Haller, H.; Lauche, R.; Cramer, H.; Rampp, T.; Saha, F.J.; Ostermann, T.; Dobos, G. Craniosacral Therapy for the Treatment of Chronic Neck Pain: A Randomized Sham-controlled Trial. Clin. J. Pain. 2016, 32, 441–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Blair, K.S.; Geraci, M.; Korelitz, K.; Otero, M.; Towbin, K.; Ernst, M.; Leibenluft, E.; Blair, R.J.; Pine, D.S. The Pathology of Social Phobia is Independent of Developmental Changes in Face Processing. Am. J. Psychiatry 2011, 168, 1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Schlier, B.; Helbig-Lang, S.; Lincoln, T.M. Anxious but Thoroughly Informed? No Jumping-to-Conclusions Bias in Social Anxiety Disorder. Cognit. Ther. Res. 2016, 40, 46–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Delgado, B.; Aparisi, D.; García-Fernández, J.M.; Sanmartín, R.; Redondo, J.; Inglés, C.J. Attributional styles in Spanish students of compulsory secondary education with high social anxiety self-reported. Rev. Lat. Psicol. 2018, 50, 89–97. [Google Scholar]
  45. Fujii, Y.; Kitagawa, N.; Shimizu, Y.; Mitsui, N.; Toyomaki, A.; Hashimoto, N.; Kako, Y.; Tanaka, T.; Asakura, S.; Koyama, T.; et al. Severity of generalized social anxiety disorder correlates with low executive functioning. Neurosci. Lett. 2013, 24, 42–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Inglés, C.J.; Díaz-Herrero, Á.; García-Fernández, J.M.; Ruiz-Esteban, C.; Delgado, B.; Martínez-Monteagudo, M.C. Academic self-attributions: Gender and grade differences in students of secondary education. Rev. Lat. Psicol. 2012, 44, 53–64. [Google Scholar]
  47. Barca, A.; Peralbo, M.; Cadavid, M. Atribuciones causales y rendimiento académico en alumnos de educación secundaria: Un estudio a partir de la subescala de atribuciones causales y multiatribucionales (EACM). Psicol. Teor. Prática 2003, 1, 17–30. [Google Scholar]
  48. Boruchovitch, E. A study of causal attributions for success and failure in mathematics among Brazilian students. Interam. J. Psychol. 2004, 38, 53–60. [Google Scholar]
  49. Gonzaba, L.; Morais, S.; Santos, J.; Jesus, S.N. Atribuições causais do sucesso e do fracasso académico: Estudo comparativo de estudantes do ensino secundário e do superior. In Actas da XI Conferência Internacional de Avaliação Psicológica: Formas e Contextos; Psiquilíbrios: Braga, Portugal, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  50. De la Torre, C.; Godoy, A. Individual differences in the causal attributions of teachers and their influence on the affective component. Interam. J. Psychol. 2003, 20, 237–246. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Model of three related factors of the Scale of Attributions of Academic Success.
Figure 1. Model of three related factors of the Scale of Attributions of Academic Success.
Ijerph 19 06045 g001
Figure 2. Model of three related factors of the Scale of Attributions of the Academic Failure.
Figure 2. Model of three related factors of the Scale of Attributions of the Academic Failure.
Ijerph 19 06045 g002
Table 1. Exploratory factor analysis of the “Academic Success Attribution Scale”.
Table 1. Exploratory factor analysis of the “Academic Success Attribution Scale”.
ComponentCommunalities
CIA UIAUEA
I pass because I try so hard in class0.7980.2310.0560.693
I pass because I spend a lot of time preparing for the exams0.7220.127−0.0090.538
I pass because I pay a lot of attention in the classes0.6030.3300.0180.473
I pass because I use some strategy to prepare for the exams (I organize, summarize, review, memorize the topics)0.6620.166−0.0490.468
I pass because I am very intelligent0.2280.7800.0680.665
I pass because I have a very good memory0.1780.7950.0170.664
I pass because I have a lot of talent, that is, I have a lot of natural capacity for studies0.2460.8020.0630.707
I pass because I have a calm character and I don’t get nervous in the exams0.1450.6160.1690.430
I pass because the teachers give easy exams0.3690.1290.6150.531
I pass because I have good luck−0.1450.1520.7230.566
I pass because the level of demand in my class is very low0.044−0.0110.7340.541
I pass because my teachers explain the topics very well0.2910.1320.6090.438
CIA = Controllable internal attributions; UIA = Uncontrollable internal attributions; UEA = Uncontrollable external attributions. Extraction method: Maximum likelihood. Rotation method: Varimax normalization.
Table 2. Exploratory factor analysis of the “Academic Failure Attribution Scale”.
Table 2. Exploratory factor analysis of the “Academic Failure Attribution Scale”.
ComponentCommunalities
CIA UIAUEA
I fail because I make little effort in class0.8230.1890.1990.752
because I spend little time preparing for exams0.8430.2140.1240.771
I fail because I pay little attention in class0.7850.2330.2730.745
I fail because I don’t use strategies to prepare for exams (organize, summarize, review, memorize topics)0.7750.2400.1150.672
I fail because I’m not very smart0.1910.8200.1830.742
I fail because I don’t have a good memory0.2480.7740.2570.727
I fail because I have little talent, that is, I have little natural capacity for studies0.2380.8350.2150.800
I fail because I have a nervous character and I cannot calm down in the exams0.2610.6240.2830.538
I fail because the teachers give difficult tests0.1430.1590.8340.742
I fail because I have bad luck0.1400.3270.6590.562
I fail because my teachers do not explain the subjects well0.1820.1270.7890.672
I fail because the level of demand in my class is very high0.2090.3310.7360.696
CIA = Controllable internal attributions; UIA = Uncontrollable internal attributions; UEA = Uncontrollable external attributions. Extraction method: Maximum likelihood. Rotation method: Varimax normalization.
Table 3. Goodness-of-fit indices of the proposed academic success attribution models.
Table 3. Goodness-of-fit indices of the proposed academic success attribution models.
Modelsχ2CMIN/DFCFITLIRMSEASRMR
M13 related factorsp < 0.0012.4900.9610.9450.0520.043
M23 factors of 1st order and 1 of 2nd orderp < 0.0013.0720.9490.9230.0820.047
M33 independent factors p < 0.0017.7170.8110.7510.1700.182
M41 unique factorp < 0.0018.2810.7960.7300.1140.073
CMIN/DF = ratio of chi2 over degrees of freedom; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker–Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized residual mean square root.
Table 4. Goodness-of-fit indices of the proposed academic failure attribution models.
Table 4. Goodness-of-fit indices of the proposed academic failure attribution models.
Modelsχ2CMIN/DFCFITLIRMSEASRMR
M13 related factorsp < 0.0012.3910.9810.9740.0500.034
M23 factors of 1st order and 1 of 2nd orderp < 0.0012.8590.9770.9650.0580.038
M33 independent factors p < 0.00111.3450.8420.80710.1360.294
M41 unique factorp < 0.00119.0440.7240.7250.1790.100
CMIN/DF = ratio of chi2 over degrees of freedom; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker–Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized residual mean square root.
Table 5. Results of the t-test for independent samples between the ASFAQ and gender.
Table 5. Results of the t-test for independent samples between the ASFAQ and gender.
ASFAQGenderT-Test for Independent Samples
Male (n = 284)Female (n = 278)
MSDMSDtp Value (Bilateral)
Attributions of academic successControllable internals14.7433.21015.2773.423−1.9070.057
Non-controllable internals13.4723.69112.0253.9734.514<0.001
External10.9012.89510.0582.9003.451<0.001
Attributions of academic failureControllable internals8.1094.4647.4134.0661.9320.054
Non-controllable internals7.8094.1438.2624.341−1.2640.206
External7.7533.8787.3053.6351.4120.159
SD = Standard Deviation; ASFAQ = Academic Success and Failure Attribution Questionnaire.
Table 6. Results of the ANOVA test between ASFAQ and the school year.
Table 6. Results of the ANOVA test between ASFAQ and the school year.
ASFAQSchool YearANOVA
Test
5° CPE (n = 148)6° CPE (n = 186)1° CSE (n = 134)2° CSE (n = 94)
MSDMSDMSDMSDFp Value
SuccessControllable internals16.148 b,c2.898 15.623 d,e3.11814.313 b,d,f3.05612.978 c,e,f3.60624.229<0.001
Non-controllable internals14.047 b,c3.66613.193 d,e3.59511.880 b,d3.89311.106 c,e3.80815.415<0.001
Externals11.695 a,b,c3.00210.854 a,d,e2.6929.619 b,d2.8309.074 c,e2.41523.058<0.001
FailureControllable internals7.317 c4.5057.5054.4547.8733.8908.829 c3.9742.7580.042
Non-controllable internals7.5064.4257.6614.1318.6644.2518.7024.0203.0330.029
Externals6.925 b,c3.9456.430 d,e3.0438.835 b,d4.0848.808 c,e3.3831.883<0.001
SD = Standard Deviation; ASFAQ = Academic Success and Failure Attribution Questionnaire; CPE = Compulsory Primary Education; CSE = Compulsory Secondary Education. a Sig < 0.05 in the post-hoc analysis (Bonferroni) between 5th and 6th CPE; b between 5th CPE and 1st CSE; c between 5th CPE and 2nd CSE; d between 6th CPE and 1st CSE; e between 6th CPE and 2nd CSE; f between 1st and 2nd CSE.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Obregón-Cuesta, A.I.; Rodríguez-Fernández, P.; León-del-Barco, B.; Mendo-Lázaro, S.; Mínguez-Mínguez, L.A.; González-Santos, J.; González-Bernal, J.J. Validation of an Academic Self-Attribution Questionnaire for Primary and Secondary School Students: Implications of Gender and Grade. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 6045. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19106045

AMA Style

Obregón-Cuesta AI, Rodríguez-Fernández P, León-del-Barco B, Mendo-Lázaro S, Mínguez-Mínguez LA, González-Santos J, González-Bernal JJ. Validation of an Academic Self-Attribution Questionnaire for Primary and Secondary School Students: Implications of Gender and Grade. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(10):6045. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19106045

Chicago/Turabian Style

Obregón-Cuesta, Ana I., Paula Rodríguez-Fernández, Benito León-del-Barco, Santiago Mendo-Lázaro, Luis A. Mínguez-Mínguez, Josefa González-Santos, and Jerónimo J. González-Bernal. 2022. "Validation of an Academic Self-Attribution Questionnaire for Primary and Secondary School Students: Implications of Gender and Grade" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 10: 6045. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19106045

APA Style

Obregón-Cuesta, A. I., Rodríguez-Fernández, P., León-del-Barco, B., Mendo-Lázaro, S., Mínguez-Mínguez, L. A., González-Santos, J., & González-Bernal, J. J. (2022). Validation of an Academic Self-Attribution Questionnaire for Primary and Secondary School Students: Implications of Gender and Grade. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(10), 6045. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19106045

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop