Informal STEM Learning for Young Children: A Systematic Literature Review
Abstract
:1. Background
- What are the effects of informal STEM learning on school readiness?
- What is the impact of informal STEM learning on children’s social-emotional development?
- (a)
- Does informal STEM learning have a different impact on children who are dual language learners?
2. Method
2.1. Search Strategy
2.2. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
2.3. Screening and Coding Procedures
3. Results
3.1. Characteristics of ISL Research
Author(s) | Title | Participants |
---|---|---|
Acosta et al. (2021) [53] | Whether and how knowledge moderates linkages between parent–child conversations and children’s reflections about tinkering in a children’s museum | 111, 5–10 yo; 60 male, 51 female; 48 White, 25 Latino, 12 Black, 4 Asian, 6 mixed; majority of parents had a bachelor’s degree or higher |
Alexander et al. (2012) [49] | Longitudinal analysis of the relations between opportunities to learn about science and the development of interests related to science | 215, 4 yo, 86 White, 6 Black, 3 Latinx,, very small % Asian or Native American |
Allen et al. (2019) [47] | From quality to outcomes: a national study of afterschool STEM programming | 1599, youth 4th–12 grade, 45% female, 25% Black, 14% Latinx, 30% White, 2% Native, 3% Asian |
Andrews and Wang (2019) [34] | Young children’s emergent science competencies in everyday family contexts: A case study | One 7 year old female |
Booth et al. (2020) [54] | Parents’ causal talk: links to children’s causal stance and emerging scientific literacy | 153, 3 yo, 71 boys; 27.5% of mothers had no higher than high school education; 13.1% African American, 73.9% White, 2.6% Asian, 10.5% multiple races |
Callanan et al. (2017) [38] | Family science talk in museums: predicting children’s engagement from variations in talk and activity | 83, 3–11 yo, 40 boys, ethnically diverse, majority middle to upper class; years of parents’ schooling 12–24 years |
Carol-Ann Burke (2020) [39] | Informal science educators and children in a low-income community describe how children relate to out-of-school science education | 32, 9–14 yo, from low SES families, 23 instructors, 2 exhibit developers, 11 community leaders |
Chung et al. (2019) [40] | Quick response code scanning for children’s informal learning | 91 youth mean age 8.54, 43 female, 36 male, SES median income $53K |
Eberbach and Crowley (2017) [7] | From seeing to observing: how parents and children learn to see science in a botanical garden | 79, 6–10 yo, 49 girls, 30 boys, 90% white, 6% Asian, 4% Black, 92% of parents had college degree |
Ehsan et al. (2021) [55] | Computational thinking embedded in engineering design: capturing computational thinking of children in an informal engineering design activity | 10, 5–7 yo, 8 boys, 2 Black, 3 multiracial, 5 White |
Gold et al. (2021) [41] | Engineering play with blocks as an informal learning context for executive function and planning | 110 preschoolers, 44% female; 25% children with disabilities, majority had low SES |
Goldstein et al. (2019) [50] | Researching a new pathway for promoting children’s active outdoor science exploration in urban settings | 81, 6–9 yo and 34 parents |
Gomes and Fleer (2019) [35] | The development of a scientific motive: how preschool science and home play reciprocally contribute to science learning | One 4 yo old boy, both parents have tertiary college degrees |
Haden et al. (2014) [31] | Supporting family conversations and children’s STEM learning in a children’s museum | 130, 4–8 yo, 61 girls; 71 White, 33 black, 26 Hispanic, 87% of parents w/college degree |
Hightower et al. (2021) [56] | Maybe we do more science than I had initially thought’: How parental efficacy affects preschool-aged children’s science and math activities and media use | 199 parents of 3–5 yo, 8 White, 2 Black, 1 Asian, 1 Latino; all had received education beyond high school |
Joy et al. (2021) [57] | Understanding parents’ roles in children’s learning and engagement in informal science learning sites | 63, 3–18 yo, 60.3% female, 31 families and 44 parents |
Kızıltaş and Sak (2018) [51] | Integrating field-trip activities with other activities in the preschool curriculum: its effects on the preschoolers’ social–emotional skills | 36, 4–5 yo Turkish children, exp group: 10 girls, 8 boys; control group: 9 girls, 9 boys |
Katz (2011) [36] | A case study of the use of internet photobook technology to enhance early childhood “scientist” identity | one 6 yo boy, interviewed again at 8 yo |
Kisiel et al. (2012) [58] | Evidence for family engagement in scientific reasoning at interactive animal exhibits | 41 families with 3–17 yo, 77.3% of parents had post-secondary schooling |
Kornelaki and Plakitsi (2018) [1] | Thunderbolt hunt. Educational program for students from 5 to 9 years old in the archaeological museum of Ioannina | 136, 5–8 yo and 12 teachers, 3 classes from private schools, 5 from public schools |
Leonard et al. (2016) [48] | Social justice, place, and equitable science education: broadening urban students’ opportunities to learn | 33, 8–12 yo, urban students, 8 Black, 22 LatinX |
Leyva et al. (2021) [42] | Relations between subdomains of home math activities and corresponding math skills in 4-year-old children | 78, 4 yo and their parents, mostly middle-income and White |
Luisa et al. (2021) [37] | Children’s protagonism in a science exhibition: an exploratory study of an exhibition in Rio de Janeiro (Brazil) | 392, 5–8 yo children from a low SES community |
Marcus et al. (2017) [59] | STEM Learning and transfer in a children’s museum and beyond | 40, 5–6 yo and their mothers, 20 male, 45% White, 25% Asian, 12.5% Black, 7.5% Hispanic, 2.5% Middle Eastern, 7.5% mixed; mean level of maternal education: 16.31 years |
Marcus et al. (2018) [60] | Promoting children’s learning and transfer across informal science, technology, engineering, and mathematics learning experiences | 64, 4–8 yo, 67% white, 11% Black, 9% Asian, 5% mixed race, 80% of mothers had a Bachelor’s degree or higher, 63% of fathers had a Bachelors degree or higher |
Morais (2015) [61] | Storytelling with chemistry and related hands-on activities: informal learning experiences to prevent “chemophobia” and promote young children’s scientific literacy | 29, 8–10 yo |
Mulvey et al. (2020) [62] | Interest and learning in informal science learning sites: differences in experiences with different types of educators | 979 children (409 early childhood, 378 middle, 215 adolescent), 59.8% female, 60.9% White; 1184 adults 72.6% female, 71.2% White |
Pagano et al. (2019) [63] | Conversational reflections about tinkering experiences in a children’s museum | 248 family groups 6–11 yo |
Pattison et al. (2020) [64] | Understanding early childhood engineering interest development as a family-level systems phenomenon: findings from the head start on engineering project | 15 families with preschool children, 8 families who reported speaking Spanish at home |
Plummer and Small (2018) [52] | Using a planetarium fieldtrip to engage young children in three-dimensional earning through representations, patterns, and lunar phenomena | 46, 6–7 yo, 23 boys |
Ramani et al. (2015) [65] | Math talk during informal learning activities in Head Start families | 33, 3–5 yo, 60% female; all enrolled in Head Start; 39% ESL; 67% Black, 12% Hispanic, 12% mixed, 9% white; 33% speak English and another language |
Schellinger et al. (2019) [43] | Using technology-enhanced inquiry-based instruction to foster the development of elementary students’ views on the nature of science | 129, 4th and 5th graders, approx half male, 43 low SES |
Strawhacker and Bers (2018) [46] | Promoting positive technological development in a kindergarten makerspace: a qualitative case study | 20, 5–7 yo 67% White, 11% Black, 6% Hispanic, 9% Asian, 5% mixed; 80% of the children’s mothers and 63% of fathers held a bachelor’s degree or higher |
Vandermaas-Peeler et al. (2016) [66] | Parent guidance of young children’s scientific and mathematical reasoning in a science museum | 23, 4–6 yo and their families, 13 girls |
Willard et al. (2019) [67] | Explain this, explore that: a study of parent–child interaction in a children’s museum | 65, 4–6 yo, 30 girls, 35 boys, 47 White, 7 Hispanic, 4 Asian, 1 Black |
Zhang et al. (2020) [16] | Parent/child number application activities predict children’s math trajectories from preschool to primary school | 196, 5 yo from 20 preschools in Guangdong province in south China; 95 boys and 101 girls |
Zheng and Libertus (2021) [44] | Individual differences in parental support for numeracy and literacy in early childhood | 259 parents of 3–6 yo, 13% of parents had a high school diploma or less, 39% had a bachelor’s degree or higher, mean income $60K |
Zucker et al. (2021) [45] | Expectancy-value theory & preschool parental involvement in informal STEM learning | 208, 3–5 yo, mostly middle class, 70% of parents had a bachelor’s degree or higher |
Author(s) | Research Design |
---|---|
Acosta et al. (2021) [53] | Quantitative design-based approach |
Alexander et al. (2012) [49] | Quantitative, prospective longitudinal, correlational |
47. Allen et al. (2019) [47] | Quantitative |
Andrews and Wang (2019) [34] | Qualitative case study |
Booth et al. (2020) [54] | Quantitative |
Callanan et al. (2017) [38] | Quantitative design based research, quasi-experimental |
Carol-Ann Burke (2020) [39] | Multi-methods |
Chung et al. (2019) [40] | Quasi-experimental and mixed methods; design-based |
Eberbach and Crowley (2017) [7] | Quantitative quasi-experimental |
Ehsan et al. (2021) [55] | Qualitative case study |
Gold et al. (2021) [41] | Quantitative correlational |
Goldstein et al. (2019) [50] | Mixed methods |
Gomes and Fleer (2019) [35] | Qualitative case study |
Haden et al. (2014) [31] | Quantitative experimental |
Hightower et al. (2021) [56] | Exploratory sequential mixed methods |
Joy et al. (2021) [57] | Quantitative descriptive |
Kızıltaş and Sak (2018) [51] | Quantitative experimental, static group pre/post test design |
Katz (2011) [36] | Qualitative |
Kisiel et al. (2012) [58] | Qualitative case study |
Kornelaki and Plakitsi (2018) [1] | Mixed methods |
Leonard et al. (2016) [48] | Mixed-methods, quasi-experimental |
Leyva et al. (2021) [42] | Quantitative |
Luisa et al. (2021) [37] | Qualitative |
Marcus et al. (2017) [59] | Quantitative experimental |
Marcus et al. (2018) [60] | Quantitative experimental |
Morais (2015) [61] | Qualitative, content analysis |
Mulvey et al. (2020) [62] | Quantitative |
Pagano et al. (2019) [63] | Quantitative, comparative |
Pattison et al. (2020) [64] | Qualitative case study |
Plummer and Small (2018) [52] | Mixed methods |
Ramani et al. (2015) [65] | Quantitative |
Schellinger et al. (2019) [43] | Quantitative |
Strawhacker and Bers (2018) [46] | Qualitative ethnographic case study |
Vandermaas-Peeler et al. (2016) [66] | Quantitative experimental |
Willard et al. (2019) [67] | Quantitative experimental |
Zhang et al. (2020) [16] | Quantitative correlational |
Zheng and Libertus (2021) [44] | Quantitative correlational |
Zucker et al. (2021) [45] | Quantitative |
Author(s) | Main Findings |
---|---|
Acosta et al. (2021) [53] | Parent STEM talk during experience increased child’s STEM talk during and after the experience; if a child had prior experience, and/or received orientation, more STEM talk occurred during tinkering and reflections |
Alexander et al. (2012) [49] | Early science interests were strong predictors of later opportunities to engage in ISL, whereas the opposite pattern (early opportunities predicting later science interests) was not found |
Allen et al. (2019) [47] | Increases in STEM engagement, identity, career interest, career knowledge, relationships, critical thinking, and perseverance; largest gains when engaging with activities for 4 weeks or more; higher-quality programming led to more growth |
Andrews and Wang (2019) [34] | Child’s emergent science competencies were playful with a developing understanding of NOS; family learning included spontaneous and purposeful learning; mother’s scaffolding played important role |
Booth et al. (2020) [54] | The higher the degree to which parents talk about causally relevant information, the stronger is the child’s causal stance; the higher the degree to which parents invite the child to generate their own explanations, the more advanced their scientific literacy will be |
Callanan et al. (2017) [38] | With priming experience, children’s engaged talk was strongly predicted by the frequency of parents’ critical thinking questions; children of more elaborative parents seemed to learn more in a museum exhibit; asking questions may encourage children’s engagement but providing explanations may reduce children’s engaged talk |
Carol-Ann Burke (2020) [39] | Educators underestimated level of interest children had in ISL and the range of home ISL activities in which children were participating; intentional and repeated hiding of the word science can communicate to child that ISL is reserved for more elite social groups |
Chung et al. (2019) [40] | Effective ISL tools promote interest; QR code scanning was effective in promoting knowledge gains; ISL can be used to support increased future learning |
Eberbach and Crowley (2017) [7] | When families engaged in more disciplinary talk during experience, children were more likely to learn from it; simple training was sufficient to improve parent disciplinary talk |
Ehsan et al. (2021) [55] | K-2 children are capable of engaging in computational thinking (CT) when designing a solution to an engineering problem; engineering design can be appropriate and promising context for practicing CT |
Gold et al. (2021) [41] | Fostering young children’s early engineering thinking using play might improve other learning and cognitive domains and overall school readiness |
Goldstein et al. (2019) [50] | Toolkit promotes urban youth and families’ participation and engagement with science concepts and practices across a range of informal, outdoor contexts; this likely relates to educator and parent support |
Gomes and Fleer (2019) [35] | Parents often do not have an understanding of how children can learn science in play; home play experiences have rich possibilities that together with the preschool activities can contribute to developing a scientific motive |
Haden et al. (2014) [31] | Adults who received conversation instructions asked more Wh-type questions; adults in the Inspector Sturdy Build + Talk group produced more STEM-related talk; families who received building tip had highest ratio of braces-to-total-pieces; children who received building instructions mentioned more types of STEM related content in photo-narrative task |
Hightower et al. (2021) [56] | Parental perceived efficacy in supporting child’s early STEM learning is related to the number of related informal activities their children engage in |
Joy et al. (2021) [57] | Parents’ requests for science information and interactive exhibits may be important factors for learning behaviors in children; when parents asked more questions, children more likely to observe exhibit; if exhibit was not interactive, children more likely to provide science explanations and less likely to engage with exhibit |
Kızıltaş and Sak (2018) [51] | Pretest scores of groups were not significantly different; posttest scores showed positive effect of field-trip activities on social–emotional skills of children in experimental group; follow-up test 12 weeks later found positive effects persisted |
Katz (2011) [36] | Children’s play activities can lead into established science |
Kisiel et al. (2012) [58] | In ISL contexts, observation and interaction play an important role in engaging visitors in practices of scientific argumentation and reasoning |
Kornelaki and Plakitsi (2018) [1] | Learning community influences learning process and science education |
Leonard et al. (2016) [48] | Participants’ science knowledge increased significantly in both settings; interacting with actual artifacts helped students anchor learning to activities and develop specific science knowledge |
Leyva et al. (2021) [42] | Home math activities in some subdomains (i.e., adding/subtracting, set comparison, and patterning) were aligned with children’s corresponding competences, but others were not |
Luisa et al. (2021) [37] | Strategies used by mediators can directly affect children’s behavior; mediation overrides design of exhibition when it comes to children’s experiences, highlighting importance of training for explainers |
Marcus et al. (2017) [59] | Dyads in engineering information group used more pieces to brace the structures; EIG had a higher ratio of functional pieces to total pieces; EI did not lead to differences in frequency of STEM talk; exception was children’s technology talk, which was more frequent for control group |
Marcus et al. (2018) [60] | Engineering instructions, either alone or in combination with transfer instructions led to use of engineering principle; ETI was linked to some differences across groups in parents’ and children’s STEM talk; parents’ STEM talk varied with child age |
Morais (2015) [61] | The hands-on activities and storytelling may engage students through listening, reading, imagining, understanding, making, and explaining, and thus can generate interest in science and scientific research |
Mulvey et al. (2020) [62] | Visitors who interacted with youth or adult educators believed they learned more, reported more interest in topics, and showed improved content knowledge over those who interacted just with the exhibit |
Pagano et al. (2019) [63] | The most detailed reflections occurred among families who participated in the program with a design challenge; families who had creations with them during reflections elaborated more; children who had combined experience of tinkering with design challenge and reminiscing with their creation demonstrated highest levels of STEM talk |
Pattison et al. (2020) [64] | Evidence of critical shifts in parent awareness, knowledge and values; family re-engagement with engineering activities; and increased family use of engineering design process |
Plummer and Small(2018) [52] | Students developed more sophisticated three-dimensional learning as they participated in planetarium field trip experience and classroom instruction |
Ramani et al. (2015) [64] | The frequency of engaging in number-related activities at home predicted children’s foundational numerical knowledge; quality of math talk used while engaging in number activities predicted children’s advanced numerical knowledge |
Schellinger et al. (2019) [43] | Technology-rich, inquiry instruction across formal and informal settings can shape elementary students’ views of some aspects of NOS |
Strawhacker and Bers (2018) [46] | Children engaged in most Positive Technological Development (PTD) aspects, but showed somewhat less evidence of collaboration and community building; the space demonstrated support in most areas except for community building |
Vandermaas-Peeler et al. (2016) [66] | Parents in instruction group provided elaborated guidance to enhance children’s evaluations of experiments, and their children responded with increased accuracy |
Willard et al. (2019) [67] | Instructional intervention to parents influenced interactions with child; parents’ behavior affected how children engage with exhibit; children’s ability to understand and recreate gear machines on own predicted by previous interactions with parent in a gear exhibit |
Zhang et al. (2020) [16] | Frequency of parent–child formal math activities not associated with children’s math trajectories; frequency of informal math activities was associated with math skill levels in preschool; parental involvement in application activities during preschool years predicted rate of growth in formal math skills through first grade |
Zheng and Libertus (2021) [44] | SES variables were related to active literacy activities, whereas few SES differences were seen in parents’ numeracy activities; several domain-specific associations between parental beliefs and enrichment activities were seen |
Zucker et al. (2021) [45] | Parents do not engage in STEM daily with their preschoolers, even when considering simple activities such as counting or describing the weather; parents who feel empowered to do science and math engage their preschooler in informal STEM learning more often |
3.2. Effects of ISL on School Readiness by ISL Types
3.3. Impact of ISL on Social-Emotional Development
3.4. Impact of ISL on Children Who Are Dual Language Learners
4. Discussion
4.1. Inquiry Themes Emerging from This Review
4.2. Relationship between ISL and School Readiness/Early STEM
4.3. Relationship between ISL and Social-Emotional Development
4.4. Research Implications
4.5. Practical Implications
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Kornelaki, A.C.; Plakitsi, K. Thunderbolt Hunt. Educational Program for Students from 5 to 9 Years Old in the Archaeological Museum of Ioannina. World J. Educ. 2018, 8, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bell, P.; Lewnstein, B.; Shouse, A.W.; Feder, M.A. Learning Science in Informal Environments: People, Places, and Pursuits; National Research Council, Ed.; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Falk, J.H.; Dierking, L.D. The 95 percent solution. Am. Sci. 2010, 98, 486–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osborne, J.; Dillon, J. Research on learning in informal contexts: Advancing the field? Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2007, 29, 1441–1445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stevens, R. Introduction: What counts as math and science? In LOST Opportunities: Learning in Out-of-School Time; Bevan, B., Stevens, R., Bell, P., Razfar, A., Eds.; Springer: London, UK, 2013; pp. 3–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ash, D. Negotiation of biological thematic conversations in informal learning settings. In Learning Conversations in Museums; Leinhardt, G., Crowley, K., Knutson, K., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2002; pp. 357–400. [Google Scholar]
- Eberbach, C.; Crowley, K. From Seeing to Observing: How Parents and Children Learn to See Science in a Botanical Garden. J. Learn. Sci. 2017, 26, 608–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerber, B.L.C.; Bavallo, A.M.L.; Marek, E.A. Relationships among informal learning environments, teaching procedures, and scientific reasoning ability. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2001, 23, 533–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ackerman, D.J.; Barnett, W.S. Prepared for Kindergarten: What Does “Readiness” Mean? Preschool Policy Brief; National Institute for Early Education Research (NIEER): New Brunswick, NJ, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Child Trends. Early School Readiness: Indicators of Child and Youth Well-Being; Child Trends: Washington, DC, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- The Virginia Kindergarten Readiness Project. Executive Summary & Legislative Report; University of Virginia: Charlottesville, VA, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Zill, N.; Moore, K.A.; Smith, E.W.; Stief, T.; Coiro, M.J. The life circumstances and development of children in welfare families: A profile based on national data. In Escape from Poverty: What Makes a Difference for Children; Chaselansdale, L., Brooks-Gunn, J., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Bulotsky-Shearer, R.J.; Lopez, L.M.; Mendez, J.L. The validity of interactive peer play competencies for Latino preschool children from low-income households. Early Child. Res. Q 2016, 34, 78–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- U.S Census Bureau. 2017. Available online: https://www.census.gov/programs-surveys/acs/news/data-releases/2017/release.html (accessed on 22 January 2020).
- Aud, S.; Hussar, W.; Kena, G.; Bianco, K.; Frohlich, L.; Kemp, J.; Tahan, K. The Condition of Education 2011; NCES 2011-033; U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics: Washington, DC, USA, 2011.
- Zhang, X.; Hu, B.Y.; Zou, X.; Ren, L. Parent-Child Number Application Activities Predict Children’s Math Trajectories from Preschool to Primary School. J. Educ. Psychol. 2020, 112, 1521–1531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuller, B.; Bridges, M.; Bein, E.; Jang, H.; Jung, S.; Rabe-Hesketh, S.; Kuo, A. The health and cognitive growth of Latino toddlers: At risk or immigrant paradox? Matern. Child Health J. 2009, 13, 755–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mancilla-Martinez, J.; Lesaux, N.K. The gap between Spanish Speakers’ word reading and word knowledge: A longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 1544–1560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Washington-Nortey, P.M.; Zhang, F.; Xu, Y.; Brown Ruiz, A.; Chen, C.; Spence, C. The impact of peer interactions on language development among preschool English language learners: A systemic review. Early Child. Educ. J. 2020, 50, 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, Y.; Chen, C.; Spence, C.; Washington-Nortey, M.; Zhang, F.; Brown Ruiz, A. Supporting young Spanish speaking English learners through teacher scaffolding and reciprocal peer tutoring. Early Child Dev. Care 2021, 192, 1324–1336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- August, D.; Shanahan, T. Response to a review and update on developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on langauge minority children and youth. J. Lit. Res. 2010, 42, 341–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garcia, E.E.; Frede, E.C. Young English Language Learners Current Research and Emerging Directions for Practice and Policy; Teachers College: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Brouzos, A.; Misailidi, P.; Hadjimattheou, A. Associations between emotional intelligence, socio-emotional adjustment, and academic achievement in childhood: The influence of age. Can. J. Sch. Psychol. 2014, 29, 83–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franco, M.D.G.; Beja, M.J.; Candeias, A.; Santos, N. Emotion understanding, social competence and school achievement in children from primary school in Portugal. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamerslag, R.; Oostdam, R.; Tavecchio, L. Inside school readiness: The role of socioemotional and behavioral factors in relation to school, teachers, peers and academic outcome in kindergarten and first grade. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 2018, 26, 80–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Portela-Pino, I.; Alvariñas-Villaverde, M.; Pino-Juste, M. Socio-Emotional Skills as Predictors of Performance of Students: Differences by Gender. Sustainability 2021, 13, 4807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ricciardi, C.; Manfra, L.; Hartman, S.; Bleiker, C.; Dineheart, L.; Winsler, A. School readiness skills at age four predict academic achievement through 5th grade. Early Child. Res. Q 2021, 57, 110–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, J. Caring Classrooms/Intelligent Schools: The Social Emotional Education of Young Children; Teachers College Press: New York, NY, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Chiu, J.L.; Chi, M.T. Supporting self-explanation in the classroom. In Applying Science of Learning in Education: Infusing Psychological Science into the Curriculum; Benassi, V.A., Overson, C.E., Hakala, C.M., Eds.; Society for the Teaching of Psychology: Washington, DC, USA, 2014; pp. 91–103. Available online: http://teachpsych.org/ebooks/asle2014/index.php (accessed on 1 January 2020).
- Rau, M.A.; Aleven, V.; Rummel, N. Successful learning with multiple graphical representations and self-explanation prompts. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 107, 30–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haden, C.A.; Jant, E.A.; Hoffman, P.C.; Marcus, M.; Geddes, J.R.; Gaskins, S. Supporting family conversations and children’s STEM learning in a children’s museum. Early Child. Res. Q 2014, 29, 333–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hirsch-Pasek, K.; Adamson, L.B.; Bakeman, R.; Owen, M.T.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Pace, A.; Yust, P.K.S.; Suma, K. The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 1071–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toub, T.S.; Rajan, V.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K. Play versus discovery learning dichotomy. In Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education; Geary, D., Berch, D., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 117–141. [Google Scholar]
- Andrews, K.J.; Wang, X.C. Young children’s emergent science competencies in everyday family contexts: A case study. Early Child Dev. Care 2019, 189, 1351–1368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gomes, J.; Fleer, M. The Development of a Scientific Motive: How Preschool Science and Home Play Reciprocally Contribute to Science Learning. Res. Sci. Educ. 2019, 49, 613–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Katz, P. A Case Study of the Use of Internet Photobook Technology to Enhance Early Childhood “Scientist” Identity. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2011, 20, 525–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luisa, M.; Catarina, C.; Luana, R.; Rowe, S.; Renata, F. Children’s Protagonism in a Science Exhibition: An Exploratory Study of an Exhibition in Rio de Janeiro (Brazil). Res. Sci. Educ. 2021, 51, 1307–1324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Callanan, M.A.; Castañeda, C.L.; Luce, M.R.; Martin, J.L. Family Science Talk in Museums: Predicting Children’s Engagement From Variations in Talk and Activity. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 1492–1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carol-Ann Burke, L.E. Informal science educators and children in a low-income community describe how children relate to out-of-school science education. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2020, 42, 1673–1696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chung, T.; Wilsey, S.; Mykita, A.; Lesgold, E.; Bourne, J. Quick response code scanning for children’s informal learning. Int. J. Inf. Learn. Technol. 2019, 36, 38–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gold, Z.S.; Elicker, J.; Evich, C.D.; Mishra, A.A.; Howe, N.; Weil, A.E. Engineering play with blocks as an informal learning context for executive function and planning. J. Eng. Educ. 2021, 110, 803–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leyva, D.; Libertus, M.E.; McGregor, R. Relations between Subdomains of Home Math Activities and Corresponding Math Skills in 4-Year-Old Children. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schellinger, J.; Mendenhall, A.; Alemanne, N.; Southerland, S.A.; Sampson, V.; Marty, P. Using Technology-Enhanced Inquiry-Based Instruction to Foster the Development of Elementary Students’ Views on the Nature of Science. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2019, 28, 341–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zheng, P.; Libertus, M. Individual Differences in Parental Support for Numeracy and Literacy in Early Childhood. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zucker, T.A.; Montroy, J.; Master, A.; Assel, M.; McCallum, C.; Yeomans-Maldonado, G. Expectancy-value theory & preschool parental involvement in informal STEM learning. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2021, 76, 101320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strawhacker, A.; Bers, M.U. Promoting Positive Technological Development in a Kindergarten Makerspace: A Qualitative Case Study. Eur. J. STEM Educ. 2018, 3, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, P.J.; Chang, R.; Gorrall, B.K.; Waggenspack, L.; Fukuda, E.; Little, T.D.; Noam, G.G. From quality to outcomes: A national study of afterschool STEM programming. Int. J. STEM Educ. 2019, 6, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leonard, J.; Chamberlin, S.A.; Johnson, J.B.; Verma, G. Social Justice, Place, and Equitable Science Education: Broadening Urban Students’ Opportunities to Learn. Urban Rev. 2016, 48, 355–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alexander, J.M.; Johnson, K.E.; Kelley, K. Longitudinal Analysis of the Relations between Opportunities to Learn about Science and the Development of Interests Related to Science. Sci. Educ. 2012, 96, 763–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, M.; Famularo, L.; Kynn, J.; Pierson, E. Researching a New Pathway for Promoting Children’s Active Outdoor Science Exploration in Urban Settings. J. Outdoor Recreat. Educ. Leadersh. 2019, 11, 101–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kızıltaş, E.; Sak, R. Integrating field-trip activities with other activities in the preschool curriculum: Its effects on the preschoolers’ social–emotional skills. Int. J. Child Care Educ. Policy 2018, 12, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Plummer, J.D.; Small, K.J. Using a planetarium fieldtrip to engage young children in three-dimensional learning through representations, patterns, and lunar phenomena. Int. J. Sci. Educ. Part B 2018, 8, 193–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Acosta, D.I.; Polinsky, N.J.; Haden, C.A.; Uttal, D.H. Whether and How Knowledge Moderates Linkages between Parent–Child Conversations and Children’s Reflections about Tinkering in a Children’s Museum. J. Cogn. Dev. 2021, 22, 226–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Booth, A.E.; Shavlik, M.; Haden, C.A. Parents’ Causal Talk: Links to Children’s Causal Stance and Emerging Scientific Literacy. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 2055–2064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ehsan, H.; Rehmat, A.P.; Cardella, M.E. Computational thinking embedded in engineering design: Capturing computational thinking of children in an informal engineering design activity. Int. J. Technol. Des. Educ. 2021, 31, 441–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hightower, B.; Sheehan, K.J.; Lauricella, A.R.; Wartella, E. “Maybe we do more science than I had initially thought”: How parental efficacy affects preschool-aged children’s science and math activities and media use. Early Child. Educ. J. 2021, 50, 1021–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joy, A.; Law, F.; McGuire, L.; Mathews, C.; Hartstone-Rose, A.; Winterbottom, M.; Rutland, A.; Fields, G.E.; Mulvey, K.L. Understanding Parents’ Roles in Children’s Learning and Engagement in Informal Science Learning Sites. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kisiel, J.; Rowe, S.; Vartabedian, M.A.; Kopczak, C. Evidence for family engagement in scientific reasoning at interactive animal exhibits. Sci. Educ. 2012, 96, 1047–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marcus, M.; Haden, C.A.; Uttal, D.H. STEM Learning and Transfer in a Children’s Museum and Beyond. Merrill-Palmer Q 2017, 63, 155–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marcus, M.; Haden, C.A.; Uttal, D.H. Promoting children’s learning and transfer across informal science, technology, engineering, and mathematics learning experiences. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 175, 80–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morais, C. Storytelling with Chemistry and Related Hands-On Activities: Informal Learning Experiences To Prevent “Chemophobia” and Promote Young Children’s Scientific Literacy. J. Chem. Educ. 2015, 92, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mulvey, K.L.; McGuire, L.; Hoffman, A.J.; Goff, E.; Rutland, A.; Winterbottom, M.; Balkwill, F.; Irvin, M.J.; Fields, G.E.; Burns, K.; et al. Interest and learning in informal science learning sites: Differences in experiences with different types of educators. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0236279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pagano, L.C.; Haden, C.A.; Uttal, D.H.; Cohen, T. Conversational reflections about tinkering experiences in a children’s museum. Sci. Educ. 2019, 103, 1493–1512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pattison, S.; Svarovsky, G.; Ramos-Montañez, S.; Gontan, I.; Weiss, S.; Núñez, V.; Corrie, P.; Smith, C.; Benne, M. Understanding Early Childhood Engineering Interest Development as a Family-Level Systems Phenomenon: Findings from the Head Start on Engineering Project. J. Pre-Coll. Eng. Educ. Res. (J-PEER) 2020, 10, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramani, G.B.; Rowe, M.L.; Eason, S.H.; Leech, K.A. Math talk during informal learning activities in Head Start families. Cogn. Dev. 2015, 35, 15–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandermaas-Peeler, M.; Massey, K.; Kendall, A. Parent guidance of young children’s scientific and mathematical reasoning in a science museum. Early Child. Educ. J. 2016, 44, 217–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willard, A.K.; Busch JT, A.; Cullum, K.A.; Letourneau, S.M.; Sobel, D.M.; Callanan, M.; Legare, C.H. Explain This, Explore That: A Study of Parent–Child Interaction in a Children’s Museum. Child Dev. 2019, 90, e598–e617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Alexandre, S.; Xu, Y.; Washington-Nortey, M.; Chen, C. Informal STEM Learning for Young Children: A Systematic Literature Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 8299. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19148299
Alexandre S, Xu Y, Washington-Nortey M, Chen C. Informal STEM Learning for Young Children: A Systematic Literature Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(14):8299. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19148299
Chicago/Turabian StyleAlexandre, Suzanne, Yaoying Xu, Melissa Washington-Nortey, and Chinchih Chen. 2022. "Informal STEM Learning for Young Children: A Systematic Literature Review" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 14: 8299. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19148299
APA StyleAlexandre, S., Xu, Y., Washington-Nortey, M., & Chen, C. (2022). Informal STEM Learning for Young Children: A Systematic Literature Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(14), 8299. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19148299