3.1. Eye Movement Data Collection
The average pupil diameter of the subjects increased during the first part of the experiment, peaked, and then began to decrease. The subjects were exposed to the home control system applet based on the dynamic experimental procedure. Therefore, their pupil diameters were in two dynamic phases, the first being the rising phase, which can be referred to as the stimulus arousal zone. Subsequently, as the stimulus continued, the subject began to feel fatigued, and the novelty decreased, leading to the fatigue phase. Astigmatism during eye movement data acquisition also affects calibration. This limits the range of user groups for eye interaction, but phakic or myopic eyes have little effect on the acquisition of eye movement data. A total of 208,200 pupil diameter data were collected for a single person, and 8,744,400 EDA (Electrodermal Activity) data were collected for a single subject. The mean values of pupil diameter change for the different stages were classified according to the behavioural stage.
Campenhausen found that individuals who focused on a single stimulus experienced a significant increase in eye velocity and a decrease in look nystagmus [
39]. Studies have shown that eye movement indices such as peak sweep speed and blink frequency are related to the attentional control process [
40]. To further investigate the specific grading of this process, Bachurina experimentally investigated the relationships between eye movements and mental attention tasks, concluding that peak sweep speed and blink frequency decreased with mental attention demands and were negatively correlated with self-evaluation of mental work [
41]. Eye movement velocity is also used in practical occupational safety assessments to assess the attentional focus of actual workers. Zhu used video capturing of workers working with safe and unsafe behaviours to determine the level of attention by eye movements [
42]. It was found that workers’ attention levels were higher during high-risk operations than in safe operations and that workers’ eye movement speed was higher in risky than in safe operations. The eye movement data focus on pupil diameter, blink count, etc. We counted the data of each type of eye movement of the subjects of different genders while performing the interface operations, as shown in
Table 2 and
Table 3.
Table 2 represents the eye movement data for subjects of different genders during manipulation task 1, and
Table 3 represents eye movement data for subjects of different genders during manipulation task 2. The eye movement data under each task module was subdivided into data for the left and right eyes, comprising four sets of items—1: eye-velocity processing data, 2: left-pupil processing data, 3: right-pupil processing data, and 4: mean-pupil processing data. The data were mainly analysed for maximum, minimum, sd, variance, mean and median. During the overall operation of interface 1, the primary interaction task was to find the intelligent sofa operation panel, which can be categorised as a complex task of finding and locating.
The main interaction task in Task 1 was to find the adjustment icon for the intelligent sofa, which can be considered a finding task in a complex panel. For the eye-velocity processing data during this task, it can be seen in
Table 2 that the average time taken for the male elderly subjects to complete this task was 25.6 s, whereas it was 15.83 for the female elderly subjects. Multiple statistics suggest that males maintained high levels of concentration for more extended periods than females when completing the finding task. The main task in Task 2 was adjusting the tilt angle of the intelligent sofa, which can be categorised as a complex task of goal attainment. For the eye-velocity processing data of this task, it can be seen from
Table 3 that the average of male elderly subjects when completing this task was 11.46 s, whereas it was 17.04 s for female elderly subjects. The multiple data values suggest that females were more attentive than males in completing the task.
Blink behaviour was often used to indicate internal and external attentional focus. Blinking interrupts visual input, temporarily inhibits visual processing, and is reduced when the task requires processing external visual information [
43,
44,
45]. Wide eyes are associated with experimental tasks of this type, whereas browsing tasks are external tasks requiring frequent sweeping of one’s surroundings [
46,
47]. Annerer, in order to explore the relationship between wide eyes and attention in different task situations, used experiments to simulate different task types and verified through machine learning that in visuospatial tasks, the external focus of attention was associated with more wide eyes than internal attentional focus [
48]. Furthermore, the sweeping gaze remained consistently high in high-attentional situations. This experiment recorded the number of blinks, the average number of blinks, the number of saccades, the average number of saccades, and the time taken for saccades for all subjects while performing complex external interface operations, as shown in
Table 4. The average number of saccades for male elderly subjects was 3.37, whereas for female elderly subjects, it was 3.42. Therefore, this indicates that female elderly subjects were more attentive than male elderly subjects throughout the experiment.
The pupil diameter reflects the subject’s mental load: a larger pupil diameter reflects a more significant mental load. The pupil diameters of the different genders were measured and counted.
Table 5 shows that the average pupil diameter of male subjects during the complex interaction task was 4.26 mm, which was larger than that of female subjects. This directly reflects that the mental load of the male elderly subjects was higher than that of the female elderly subjects during the interaction with the intelligent home software interface.
Heatmaps can be used to discover the visual objects that attract the most attention, compare the strengths and weaknesses of the visual objects concerning the user’s attention, and have the advantage of supporting multi-user data displays.
Figure 4a–d show the hotspot and eye-tracking graphs for men during page browsing and task completion, and
Figure 4e–h show the hotspot and eye-tracking graphs for women during page browsing and task completion. In the search task for interface 1, the hotspot map shows that female subjects were more likely to be attracted to the rest of the interface, combined with the trajectory map. Female subjects continued to browse the rest of the interface after searching for the right panel. They were also more attracted to the icons than the textual information, and their eyes stayed longer. In the manipulation task of interface 2, the hotspot diagram reflects that the female’s attention is entirely focused on manipulating the slider bar. In contrast, the male subjects’ eyes are also drawn to the rest of the iconic information.
3.2. HRV Data Collection
The HRV physiological signal acquisition of elderly subjects was carried out. The HRV physiological signal can be divided into an ECG (Electrocardiogram) signal and a PPG (PhotoPlethysmoGraphy) signal. The HRV signal was first pre-processed using the wavelet noise reduction method. The frequency range of the ECG signal was 0.01∼200 Hz; the frequency range of the pulse signal was 0.1∼40 Hz. The noise signal was removed by high-pass and low-pass filtering, and the influential data band was retained. Band-stop filtering is switched on mainly to remove industrial frequency interference from the environment by locating intervals that vary from the previous interval by more than a user-defined percentage (typically 20%) as irregular intervals. The anomaly detection formula is shown in Equation (
1).
The mean-square correction was used for ectopic intervals. The mean-square method replaces the ectopic interval with the mean of the adjacent IBI intervals centred on the ectopic interval, as shown in Equation (
2). Similarly, the median method replaces the ectopic interval with the median of the adjacent IBI intervals. Finally, it replaces the ectopic interval with the cubic spline function, as shown in Equation (
3).
Time-domain HRV analysis evaluates heart-rate variability by calculating a series of mathematical and statistical indicators of the R-R interval, revealing the pattern of signal changes over time. The time-domain indicators mainly correspond to the magnitude of sympathetic and parasympathetic tone and thus to the overall degree of activation of the autonomic nervous system. Our ECG signals had a short duration, so they were mainly processed in the RMSSD band for time-domain analysis, as shown in Equation (
4).
The RMSSD bands were processed for time-domain analysis, as shown in
Table 6. The combined statistical analysis in
Figure 5 shows that the RMSSD bands of the elderly female subjects were much larger than those of the males. This reflects that their heart rate is lower than that of males when performing complex tasks, and they are in a more relaxed state.