Who Tends to Appreciate Atonal Music? Higher Perceived Personal Control Leads to an Increased Inclination to Prefer Atonal Music
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Perceived Lack of Structure and Disorderliness of Atonal Music
1.2. Perceived Personal Control and the Need for Structure
1.3. Atonal Music, Perceived Control, and the Need for Structure
1.4. The Current Study
2. Method
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials and Measures
2.2.1. Music Materials
2.2.2. Perceived Personal Control
2.2.3. Need for Structure
2.2.4. Aesthetic Experience of Atonal Music
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Common Method Bias Test
3.2. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
3.3. Mediation Effect of the Need for Structure
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Why Are We Afraid of Schoenberg? The Bund. Available online: http://www.bundpic.com/posts/post/5499139997ea0b32a23fd5dc (accessed on 19 November 2021).
- Simms, B.R. The Atonal Music of Arnold Schoenberg, 1908–1923; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Grant, M.J. Serial Music, Serial Aesthetics: Compositional Theory in Post-War Europe; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005; Volume 16. [Google Scholar]
- Adorno, T.W. Philosophy of New Music; Mohr, J.C.B., Ed.; University of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1949. [Google Scholar]
- Schulze, K.; Dowling, W.J.; Tillmann, B. Working memory for tonal and atonal sequences during a forward and a backward recognition task. Music Percept. 2011, 29, 255–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daynes, H. Listeners’ perceptual and emotional responses to tonal and atonal music. Psychol. Music 2011, 39, 468–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fricke, S. Contemporary music. In Musical Life in Germany-Structure, Facts and Figure; Schulmeistrat, S., Wallscheid, M., Eds.; German Music Council: Bonn, Germany, 2011; pp. 169–183. [Google Scholar]
- Wranning, J.; Wetterin, J. Openness to Experience and a Preference for Atonality: How Does the Personality Trait “Openness to Experience” Correlate to Enjoyment of Atonal Harmony, as Opposed to Tonal Harmony? Bachelor’s Thesis, School of Informatics, University of Skövde, Skövde, Sweden, 2020. Available online: https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/record.jsf?pid=diva2%3A1576156&dswid=-489 (accessed on 20 February 2022).
- Nusbaum, E.C.; Silvia, P.J. Shivers and timbres: Personality and the experience of chills from music. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2011, 2, 199–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ockelford, A.; Sergeant, D. Musical expectancy in atonal contexts: Musicians’ perception of “antistructure”. Psychol. Music 2012, 41, 139–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landau, M.J.; Kay, A.C.; Whitson, J.A. Compensatory control and the appeal of a structured world. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 694–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bharucha, J.J.; Krumhansl, C.L. The representation of harmonic structure in music: Hierarchies of stability as a function of context. Cognition 1983, 13, 63–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krumhansl, C.L.; Cuddy, L. A Theory of Tonal Hierarchies in Music. In Music Perception, Springer Handbook of Auditory Research; Jones, M.R., Fay, R.R., Popper, A.N., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2010; Volume 36, pp. 51–87. [Google Scholar]
- Rosch, E. Cognitive reference points. Cogn. Psychol. 1975, 7, 532–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tillmann, B.; Janata, P.; Birk, J.; Bharucha, J.J. Tonal centers and expectancy: Facilitation or inhibition of chords at the top of the harmonic hierarchy? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2008, 34, 1031–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Creel, S.C.; Newport, E.L.; Aslin, R.N. Distant melodies: Statistical learning of nonadjacent dependencies in tone sequences. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2004, 30, 1119–1130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van de Cruys, S.; Wagemans, J. Gestalts as predictions—Some reflections and an application to art. Gestalt. Theory 2011, 33, 325–344. [Google Scholar]
- Perle, G. Serial Composition and Atonality: An Introduction to the Music of Schoenberg, Berg, and Webern; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Jepma, M.; Verdonschot, R.G.; van Steenbergen, H.; Rombouts, S.A.; Nieuwenhuis, S. Neural mechanisms underlying the induction and relief of perceptual curiosity. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bevins, R.A. Novelty seeking and reward: Implications for the study of high-risk behaviors. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2001, 10, 189–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feist, G.J.; Brady, T.R. Openness to experience, non-conformity, and the preference for abstract art. Empir. Stud. Arts 2004, 22, 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kay, A.C.; Whitson, J.A.; Gaucher, D.; Galinsky, A.D. Compensatory control: Achieving order through the mind, our institutions, and the heavens. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 18, 264–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lachman, M.E.; Weaver, S.L. The Sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 763–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, C.Y.; Lee, L.; Yap, A.J. Control deprivation motivates acquisition of utilitarian products. J. Consum. Res. 2017, 43, 1031–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, A.; Kay, A.C. Compensatory control and ambiguity intolerance. Organ Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 2017, 140, 46–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kay, A.C.; Gaucher, D.; Napier, J.L.; Callan, M.J.; Laurin, K. God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 18–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mao, J.-Y.; Yang, S.-L.; Guo, Y.-Y. Are individuals from lower social class more susceptible to conspiracy theories? An explanation from the compensatory control theory. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 2020, 23, 372–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rutjens, B.T.; Van Harreveld, F.; Joop, V.D.P.; Kreemers, L.M.; Noordewier, M.K. Steps, stages, and structure: Finding compensatory order in scientific theories. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2013, 142, 313–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cutright, K.M. The Beauty of boundaries: When and why we seek structure in consumption. J. Consum. Res. 2012, 38, 775–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Friesen, J.P.; Kay, A.C.; Eibach, R.P.; Galinsky, A.D. Seeking structure in social organization: Compensatory control and the psychological advantages of hierarchy. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 106, 590–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Neuberg, S.L.; Newsom, J.T. Personal need for structure: Individual differences in the desire for simpler structure. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 65, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Leon, P.R.; Kay, A.C. Political ideology and compensatory control mechanisms. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2020, 34, 112–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mencke, I.; Omigie, D.; Wald-Fuhrmann, M.; Brattico, E. Atonal music: Can uncertainty lead to pleasure? Front. Neurosci. 2019, 12, 979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hamtiaux, A.; Houssemand, C. Adaptability, cognitive flexibility, personal need for structure, and Rigidity. Psychol. Res. 2012, 2, 563–585. Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/10993/13193 (accessed on 28 January 2022).
- Rietzschel, E.F.; Slijkhuis, J.M.; Van Yperen, N.W. Task structure, need for structure, and creativity. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 44, 386–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, J. Psychological Attributions of Different Social Classes to the Gap between the Rich and the Poor; World Book: Guangzhou, China, 2014. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, N.N. Revision of Chinese Version of Need for Structure Scale. Master’s Thesis, Peking University, Beijing, China, 2005. (In Chinese). [Google Scholar]
- Berlyne, D.E. Studies in the New Experimental Aesthetics; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck, H.J. An experimental study of aesthetic preferences for polygonal figures. J. Gen. Psychol. 1968, 79, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leder, H. Determinants of preference. When do we like what we know? Empir. Stud. Arts 2001, 19, 201–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 879–930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hayes, A. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Schäfer, T.; Sedlmeier, P. What makes us like music? Determinants of music preference. Psychol. Aesthet Creat. Arts 2010, 4, 223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dollinger, S.J. Research note: Personality and music preference: Extraversion and excitement seeking or openness to experience? Psychol. Music 1993, 21, 73–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rentfrow, P.J.; Gosling, S.D. The Do Re Mi’s of everyday life: The structure and personality correlates of music preferences. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 1236–1256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Blair, S. How lacking control drives fluency effects in evaluative judgment. Organ Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 2020, 156, 97–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Infurna, F.J.; Gerstorf, D.; Ram, N.; Schupp, J.; Wagner, G.G. Long-term antecedents and outcomes of perceived control. Psychol. Aging 2011, 26, 559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beck, J.T.; Rahinel, R.; Bleier, A. Company worth keeping: Personal control and preferences for brand leaders. J. Consum. Res. 2020, 46, 871–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kraus, M.W.; Piff, P.K.; Mendoza-Denton, R.; Rheinschmidt, M.L.; Keltner, D. Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 119, 546–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Perceived personal control | 4.56 | 0.76 | 1 | |||
2. Need for structure | 4.09 | 0.62 | –0.13 ** | 1 | ||
3. Liking of atonal music | 2.82 | 1.09 | 0.11 * | –0.15 ** | 1 | |
4. Interest in atonal music | 2.91 | 1.22 | 0.13 ** | –0.12 * | 0.70 *** | 1 |
Effect Value | Boot SE | Lower Boot CI | Upper Boot CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total effect of perceived control on the liking of atonal music | 0.16 | 0.07 | 0.03 | 0.30 |
Direct effect of perceived control on the liking of atonal music | 0.14 | 0.07 | 0.004 | 0.27 |
Indirect effect of perceived control on the liking of atonal music | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.06 |
Effect Value | Boot SE | Lower Boot CI | Upper Boot CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total effect of perceived control on interest in atonal music | 0.21 | 0.08 | 0.06 | 0.36 |
Direct effect of perceived control on interest in atonal music | 0.19 | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.34 |
Indirect effect of perceived control on interest in atonal music | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.003 | 0.06 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Liu, J.; Yang, S.-L.; Yu, F. Who Tends to Appreciate Atonal Music? Higher Perceived Personal Control Leads to an Increased Inclination to Prefer Atonal Music. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3248. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063248
Liu J, Yang S-L, Yu F. Who Tends to Appreciate Atonal Music? Higher Perceived Personal Control Leads to an Increased Inclination to Prefer Atonal Music. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(6):3248. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063248
Chicago/Turabian StyleLiu, Junfeng, Shen-Long Yang, and Feng Yu. 2022. "Who Tends to Appreciate Atonal Music? Higher Perceived Personal Control Leads to an Increased Inclination to Prefer Atonal Music" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 6: 3248. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063248
APA StyleLiu, J., Yang, S. -L., & Yu, F. (2022). Who Tends to Appreciate Atonal Music? Higher Perceived Personal Control Leads to an Increased Inclination to Prefer Atonal Music. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(6), 3248. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063248