The Easier the Better: How Processing Fluency Influences Self-Efficacy and Behavioral Intention in Pro-Social Campaign Advertising
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Self-Efficacy in Pro-Social Behavior
3. Processing Fluency, Self-Efficacy, and Behavioral Intention
4. Study 1
4.1. Participants and Procedure
4.2. Results
4.3. Discussion
5. The Influence of Manipulated Fluency: Match between Color and Framing as a Source of Fluency
6. Study 2
6.1. Participants and Procedure
6.2. Results
7. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Details of the Study Instruments
Behavioral intentions |
How likely are you to engage the campaign in the advertising? (1 = highly unlikely, and 7 = highly likely) How inclined are you to engage the campaign in the advertising? (1 = not very inclined, and 7 = very inclined) How willing are you to provide personal information on their website and engage the campaign in the advertising? (1 = very unwilling, and 7 = very willing) How likely are you to recommend participation in the campaign in the advertising to others? (1 = highly unlikely, and 7 = highly likely) |
Perceived efficacy |
I feel that through this environmental protection campaign I can make a difference I feel that I know how to go about an environmental protection campaign I believe that I know what steps I will take to engage in this environmental protection campaign |
Processing fluency |
It was difficult to process the information It was easy to understand It was difficult to comprehend It was easy to process the information |
Involvement as Control Variable |
Participating in the pro-environmental program is valuable to me (1 = worthless, and 7 = valuable) Reducing the use of the disposable material is relevant to me (1 = irrelevant, and 7 = relevant) Reducing the use of the disposable material is a concern to me (1 = of no concern to me, and 7 = of concern to me) Reducing waste is important to me (1 = unimportant, and 7 = important) |
Demographics |
Age Sex (male, female) |
Instruction | |
Please look at campaign poster regarding organ donation. | |
Stimuli: Manipulation of the primary color of advertising and messages | |
Blue and gain-framing message condition | Blue and loss-framing message condition |
Red and gain-framing message | Red and loss-framing message |
Behavioral intentions: The similar items used in Study 1 | |
How likely are you to engage in the organ donation program? (1 = highly unlikely, and 7 = highly likely) How inclined are you to engage in the organ donation program? (1 = not very inclined, and 7 = very inclined) How willing are you to provide personal information on their website and engage the organ donation program? (1 = very unwilling, and 7 = very willing) How likely are you to recommend the organ donation program to others? (1 = highly unlikely, and 7 = highly likely) | |
Perceived efficacy: The similar items used in Study 1 (1 = Strongly disagree, and 7 = Strongly agree) | |
I feel that through this organ donation I can make a difference I feel that I know how to go about organ donation I believe that I know what steps I will take to donate organs | |
Demographics | |
Age Sex (male, female) |
Appendix B. Summary of Study Results
Hypothesis | Results |
---|---|
H1. Self-efficacy has a positive effect on behavioral intention toward pro-social behavior | Supported |
H2. Fluency has a positive effect on perceived self-efficacy toward the pro-social behavior | Supported |
H3. Self-efficacy mediates the influence of fluency on behavioral intention toward the pro-social campaign | Supported |
Hypothesis | Results |
---|---|
H4. Color will moderate the relationship between message framing and self-efficacy toward an organ donation campaign. | Partially Supported |
H4a. Consumers will show greater self-efficacy with the loss framing condition than with the gain framing when the primary color of advertising is red. | Supported |
H4b. Consumers will show greater self-efficacy with the gain framing condition than with the loss framing when the primary color of advertising is blue. | Not Supported |
H5. Color will moderate the relationship between message framing and behavioral intention to join an organ donation campaign. | Partially Supported |
H5a. Consumers will show greater behavioral intention with the loss framing condition than with the gain framing when the primary color of advertising is red. | Supported |
H5b. Consumers will show greater behavioral intention with the gain framing condition than with the loss framing when the primary color of advertising is blue. | Not Supported |
H6. Self-efficacy mediates the effects of the two-way interaction between primary color and framing on behavioral intention toward the pro-social campaign | Supported |
References
- Rice, R.E.; Atkin, C.K. (Eds.) Public Communication Campaigns; Sage: Newcastle upon Tyne, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Geyer, R.; Jambeck, J.R.; Law, K.L. Production, use, and fate of all plastics ever made. Sci. Adv. 2017, 3, e1700782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bamberg, S.; Moser, G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 14–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, C.M.; Le-Klähn, D.-T.; Ram, Y. Tourism, Public Transport and Sustainable Mobility; Channel View: Bristol, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Venhoeven, L.A.; Bolderdijk, J.W.; Steg, L. Explaining the Paradox: How Pro-Environmental Behaviour can both Thwart and Foster Well-Being. Sustainability 2013, 5, 1372–1386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ajzen, I. From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In Action Control; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1985; pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
- Locke, E.A.; Latham, G.P. Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. Am. Psychol. 2002, 57, 705–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bandura, A. Human agency in social cognitive theory. Am. Psychol. 1989, 44, 1175–1184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pajares, F.; Urdan, T. Self-Efficacy Beliefs of Adolescents; IAP-Information Age Pub. Inc.: Greenwich, CT, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura, A. The primacy of self-regulation in health promotion. Appl. Psychol. 2005, 54, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feltz, D.L.; Short, S.E.; Sullivan, P.J. Self-Efficacy in Sport; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Combs, G.M.; Luthans, F. Diversity training: Analysis of the impact of self-efficacy. Hum. Resour. Dev. Q. 2007, 18, 91–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schwarz, N. Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. J. Consum. Psychol. 2004, 14, 332–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tapia-Fonllem, C.; Corral-Verdugo, V.; Fraijo-Sing, B.; Durón-Ramos, M.F. Assessing sustainable behavior and its correlates: A measure of pro-ecological, frugal, altruistic and equitable actions. Sustainability 2013, 5, 711–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Self-Efficacy in Changing Societies; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Kanfer, R.; Ackerman, P.L. A self-regulatory skills perspective to reducing cognitive interference. In Cognitive Interference: Theories, Methods, and Findings; Sarason, I.G., Pierce, G.R., Pierce, B.R., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1996; pp. 153–171. [Google Scholar]
- De Young, R. New ways to promote pro-environmental behavior: Expanding and evaluating motives for environmentally responsible behavior. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 509–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newcomb, M.D.; Harlow, L.L. Life events and substance use among adolescents: Mediating effects of perceived loss of control and meaninglessness in life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 564–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DiClemente, C.C. Self-efficacy and smoking cessation maintenance: A preliminary report. Cogn. Ther. 1981, 5, 175–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tabernero, C.; Hernandez, B. Self-Efficacy and Intrinsic Motivation Guiding Environmental Behavior. Environ. Behav. 2011, 43, 658–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tang, Z.J.; Chen, X.H.; Luo, J.H. Determining Socio-Psychological Drivers for Rural Household Recycling Behavior in Developing Countries: A Case Study From Wugan, Hunan, China. Environ. Behav. 2011, 43, 848–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thøgersen, J.; Grønhøj, A. Electricity saving in households—A social cognitive approach. Energy Policy 2010, 38, 7732–7743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubell, M. Environmental activism as collective action. Environ. Behav. 2002, 34, 431–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Basil, D.Z.; Ridgway, N.M.; Basil, M.D. Guilt and giving: A process model of empathy and efficacy. Psychol. Mark. 2008, 25, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giles, M.; McClenahan, C.; Cairns, E.; Mallet, J. An application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour to blood donation: The importance of self-efficacy. Health Educ. Res. 2004, 19, 380–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clowes, R.; Masser, B.M. Right here, right now: The impact of the blood donation context on anxiety, attitudes, subjective norms, self-efficacy, and intention to donate blood. Transfusion 2012, 52, 1560–1565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brug, J.; Vugt, M.V.; van Den Borne, B.; Brouwers, A.; Hooff, H.V. Predictors of willingness to register as an organ donor among Dutch adolescents. Psychol. Health 2000, 15, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hyde, M.K.; White, K.M. To be a donor or not to be? Applying an extended theory of planned behavior to predict posthumous organ donation intentions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 39, 880–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reubsaet, A.; Brug, J.; De Vet, E.; Van Den Borne, B. The effects of practicing registration of organ donation preference on self-efficacy and registration intention: An enactive mastery experience. Psychol. Health 2003, 18, 585–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCarty, J.A.; Shrum, L. The influence of individualism, collectivism, and locus of control on environmental beliefs and behavior. J. Public Policy Mark. 2001, 20, 93–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, K.; Ryu, G. The Effect of Regulatory Focus on Individuals’ Donation Behavior. Sustainability 2018, 10, 760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, A.Y.; Labroo, A. Effects of conceptual and perceptual fluency on affective judgment. J. Mark. Res. 2004, 41, 151–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reber, R.; Schwarz, N.; Winkielman, P. Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 8, 364–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whittlesea, B.W. Illusions of familiarity. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1993, 19, 1235–1253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winkielman, P.; Schwarz, N.; Fazendeiro, T.; Reber, R. The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion; Musch, J., Klauer, K.C., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2003; pp. 189–217. [Google Scholar]
- Fazendeiro, T.; Winkielman, P.; Luo, C.; Lorah, C. False recognition across meaning, language, and stimulus format: Conceptual relatedness and the feeling of familiarity. Mem. Cogn. 2005, 33, 249–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Alter, A.L.; Oppenheimer, D.M. Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 13, 219–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pandelaere, M.; Millet, K.; Van den Bergh, B. Madonna or Don McLean? The effect of order of exposure on relative liking. J. Consum. Psychol. 2010, 20, 442–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reber, R.; Winkielman, P.; Schwarz, N. Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychol. Sci. 1998, 9, 45–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shah, A.K.; Oppenheimer, D.M. Easy does it: The role of fluency in cue weighting. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2007, 2, 371–379. [Google Scholar]
- White, K.; MacDonnell, R.; Dahl, D.W. It’s the Mind-Set That Matters: The Role of Construal Level and Message Framing in Influencing Consumer Efficacy and Conservation Behaviors. J. Mark. Res. 2011, 48, 472–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rhodes, M.G.; Castel, A.D. Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2008, 137, 615–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alter, A.L.; Oppenheimer, D.M.; Epley, N.; Eyre, R.N. Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2007, 136, 569–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carpenter, S.K.; Olson, K.M. Are pictures good for learning new vocabulary in a foreign language? Only if you think they are not. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2012, 38, 92–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- White, K.; Peloza, J. Self-benefit versus other-benefit marketing appeals: Their effectiveness in generating charitable support. J. Mark. 2009, 73, 109–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chae, H.; Hoegg, J. The future looks “Right”: Effects of the horizontal location of advertising images on product attitude. J. Consum. Res. 2013, 40, 223–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, A.Y.; Aaker, J.L. Bringing the frame into focus: The influence of regulatory fit on processing fluency and persuasion. J. Per. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 86, 205–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zaichkowsky, J.L. Measuring the involvement construct. J. Consum. Res. 1985, 12, 341–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, A.F. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling (White Paper). Available online: http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf (accessed on 21 August 2018).
- Zhao, X.S.; Lynch, J.G.; Chen, Q.M. Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths about Mediation Analysis. J. Consum. Res. 2010, 37, 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Bock, T.; Pandelaere, M.; Van Kenhove, P. When colors backfire: The impact of color cues on moral judgment. J. Consum. Psychol. 2013, 23, 341–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seo, J.Y.; Scammon, D.L.J.M.L. Do green packages lead to misperceptions? The influence of package colors on consumers’ perceptions of brands with environmental claims. Mark. Lett. 2017, 28, 357–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elliot, A.J.; Maier, M.A.; Binser, M.J.; Friedman, R.; Pekrun, R. The effect of red on avoidance behavior in achievement contexts. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 35, 365–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Elliot, A.J.; Maier, M.A.; Moller, A.C.; Friedman, R.; Meinhardt, J. Color and psychological functioning: The effect of red on performance attainment. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2007, 136, 154–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meyers-Levy, J.; Peracchio, L.A. Understanding the effects of color: How the correspondence between available and required resources affects attitudes. J. Consum. Res. 1995, 22, 121–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fraser, T.; Banks, A. Designer’s Color Manual: The Complete Guide to Color Theory and Application; Chronicle Books: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Mehta, R.; Zhu, R.J. Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances. Science 2009, 323, 1226–1229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 1981, 211, 453–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deng, X.; Kahn, B.E. Is your product on the right side? The “location effect” on perceived product heaviness and package evaluation. J. Mark. Res. 2009, 46, 725–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, A.Y.; Keller, P.A.; Sternthal, B. Value from regulatory construal fit: The persuasive impact of fit between consumer goals and message concreteness. J. Consum. Res. 2009, 36, 735–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maio, G.R.; Haddock, G. Attitude change. In Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles; Kruglanski, A.W., Higgins, E.T., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 565–586. [Google Scholar]
- Labroo, A.A.; Pocheptsova, A. Metacognition and consumer judgment: Fluency is pleasant but disfluency ignites interest. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2016, 10, 154–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Mean | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Fluency | 4.36 | 0.99 | – | |||
2. Self-Efficacy | 4.38 | 1.45 | 0.486 ** | – | ||
3. Behavioral Intention | 4.59 | 1.05 | 0.399 ** | 0.778 ** | – | |
4. Involvement | 3.46 | 1.49 | 0.245 | 484 ** | 475 ** | – |
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kim, H.J.; Jang, J.M. The Easier the Better: How Processing Fluency Influences Self-Efficacy and Behavioral Intention in Pro-Social Campaign Advertising. Sustainability 2018, 10, 4777. https://doi.org/10.3390/su10124777
Kim HJ, Jang JM. The Easier the Better: How Processing Fluency Influences Self-Efficacy and Behavioral Intention in Pro-Social Campaign Advertising. Sustainability. 2018; 10(12):4777. https://doi.org/10.3390/su10124777
Chicago/Turabian StyleKim, Hee Jin, and Jung Min Jang. 2018. "The Easier the Better: How Processing Fluency Influences Self-Efficacy and Behavioral Intention in Pro-Social Campaign Advertising" Sustainability 10, no. 12: 4777. https://doi.org/10.3390/su10124777
APA StyleKim, H. J., & Jang, J. M. (2018). The Easier the Better: How Processing Fluency Influences Self-Efficacy and Behavioral Intention in Pro-Social Campaign Advertising. Sustainability, 10(12), 4777. https://doi.org/10.3390/su10124777