Next Article in Journal
Thermal and Seismic Capacity Improvements for Masonry Building Heritage: A Unified Retrofitting System
Next Article in Special Issue
A Behavioral Perspective on Community Resilience during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Case of Paraisópolis in São Paulo, Brazil
Previous Article in Journal
A Comparison of Two-Stage and Traditional Co-Composting of Green Waste and Food Waste Amended with Phosphate Rock and Sawdust
Previous Article in Special Issue
Universalists or Utilitarianists? The Social Representation of COVID-19 Pandemic in Brazil
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sex Differences in Spatial Activity and Anxiety Levels in the COVID-19 Pandemic from Evolutionary Perspective

Institute of Ethnology and Anthropology, Russian Academy of Sciences, 119334 Moscow, Russia
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2021, 13(3), 1110; https://doi.org/10.3390/su13031110
Submission received: 26 November 2020 / Revised: 5 January 2021 / Accepted: 18 January 2021 / Published: 21 January 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior in Pandemics)

Abstract

:
Despite the enforced lockdown regime in late March 2020 in Russia, the phenomenon of the continued virus spreading highlighted the importance of studies investigating the range of biosocial attributes and spectrum of individual motivations underlying the permanent presence of the substantial level of spatial activity. For this matter, we conducted a set of surveys between March and June 2020 (N = 492). We found that an individual’s health attitude is the most consistent factor explaining mobility differences. However, our data suggested that wariness largely determines adequate health attitudes; hence, a higher level of wariness indirectly reduced individual mobility. Comparative analysis revealed the critical biosocial differences between the two sexes, potentially rooted in the human evolutionary past. Females were predisposed to express more wariness in the face of new environmental risks; therefore, they minimize their mobility and outdoor contacts. In contrast to them, the general level of spatial activity reported by males was significantly higher. Wariness in the males’ sample was less associated with the novel virus threat, but to a great extent, it was predicted by the potential economic losses variable. These findings correspond to the evolutionary predictions of sexual specialization and the division of family roles.

1. Introduction

Human spatial activity and daily dynamics of an individual’s mobility have obtained unprecedented importance in the COVID-19 pandemic. Nowadays, human spatial activity and related motor activity are the basis of urban life, which forms the space and environment of modern humans. The study of spatial activity can give us important information about a wide range of socio-economic phenomena and expand our understanding of human behavior in the context of its interaction with the modern highly urbanized environment both in periods of stability and in periods of critical environmental changes, including the emergence of a new infectious disease. The biosocial response to threats associated with new viral infections involves major changes in individual mobility. Individual choices on how to react to a coronavirus threat during the 2020 pandemic (to follow or to violated the official instructions) shape the course of the virus’s spread and the risks facing human populations. For instance, reducing spatial activity, increasing interpersonal distance during communication, and limiting contacts are mechanisms developed by evolution as a biosocial adaptation to new infectious diseases [1]. At the same time, quarantine procedures that were initialized by governments pursued social distancing, hence, reducing individual contacts and reducing the degree of physical activity related to daily and weekly spatial mobility. Concurrently, this public health emergency instructions was taking place in a media environment saturated with misinformation, partisan infighting, and messaging undermining health experts [2]. Despite the strict lockdown regime introduced in late March 2020 in most of the regions of Russia, the ongoing virus spreading highlighted the importance of investigating the range of biosocial and psychological attributes, which potentially could explain the models of the substantial level of spatial activity. Studies of past epidemics and pandemics (Ebola 2014/2016, H1N1 2009/2010, avian influenza in 2006) have shown that a significant portion of the population is susceptible to anxiety, altering health attitudes related to the desire to protect themselves and their beloved during periods of active viral threats [3,4,5,6]. The novel COVID-19 coronavirus (SARS-CoV-2) pandemic has affected nearly every aspect of economic, social, and political life. The pandemic had become a significant source of psychological and physical stress for a considerable number of people all over the world due the attitude toward socioeconomic crisis triggered by COVID-19, social conflicts, exacerbated health-care systems, restrictions and fear of punishments by authorities, and other stress-related factors [7].
Introducing mandatory self-isolation measures imposes restrictions on a wide range of basic human needs, which are associated with the critical aspects of life-sustaining functions. For instance, spatial activity plays a crucial role in individual subsistence, initiating a multitude of processes related to resource acquisition, reproductive, environmental, and other biosocial tasks [8,9,10,11]. Currently we are witnessing substantial individual variations in knowledge and concern about COVID-19 and the willingness to change spatial behavior in the face of the pandemic. In a biological sense, the vital motivational stimuli associated with spatial and motor behavior that is directly affecting the distance of individual movement are an individual’s fundamental needs in resource acquisition and in performing their reproductive functions and efforts [8,9]. At the same time, the evolution of monogamy in humans and strong pair-bonding assumes intensified male parental investment [10,11], with a certain degree of specialization of each sex within a pair. Men are more likely to provision material resources and income to the family budget, while women are more focused on the direct care of offspring. As it has been argued that, in human evolutionary past, males exceeded females in Euclidean spatial abilities, since they faced more significant navigational challenges, for reasons of either polygyny [12,13,14] or the navigational demands of prey hunting [15]. Spatial tasks other than navigation may also have undergone substantial selection pressure in males, particularly the demands in spatial target analysis and enhanced competitiveness in fighting to obtain resources [16]. This division of functional responsibilities in the family has been recorded in many traditional societies [17,18,19,20,21]. Spatial movements and activity patterns, for instance, the most likely distances and directions of the people’s daily mobility, have been profoundly investigated in the contemporary societies [22,23,24,25]. The study of human mobility in urban China, for example, have shown that men’s and women’s average day travel distances were statistically different, with an expected daily distance for men of about 2 km, and lesser daily mobility for women. These findings support the hypothesis of evident sex differences in models of daily mobility in post-industrial societies [26].
The degree of daily mobility can be changed for both sexes in the periods of different environmental threats accompanied by a substantial growth of anxiety levels within a population. Numerous studies report that more than 50% of respondents experience a high level of anxiety during viral epidemics or pandemics [3,27], which leads to changes in various behavioral patterns including spatial activity. Adaptive response mechanisms to the environmental challenges differ for men and women of various sex-age groups.
Women are known to be more cautious and prone to avoid most types of risks as compared to men [28,29]. Slovic [30] has examined sex differences in risk perception in the health/safety domain. Male and female respondents differed significantly in their perception of all risk categories related to health and safety. Several studies where domain-specific risk was investigated confirmed that female respondents were less likely to be engaging in risky behaviors; they also perceived threats to a greater extent [31,32], especially in categories related to health and safety. Kruger with coauthors highlighted a specific evolutionary valid domain—environmental risks, possibly the most ancient domain, stemming from our ancestral history of foraging, hunting, and avoiding predators and parasites. They found that men were significantly more risk-taking than women in all evolutionary valid risk domains (such as between-group and within-group competition, mating and resource allocation for mate attraction and environmental risks) [33].
A number of studies investigated the relationship between anxiety and propensity to risk-taking behavior in different decision-making domains [34,35,36,37]. Various research have documented that individual differences in anxiety are associated with risk-avoidant decision-making. Maner with coauthors showed that individuals with high levels of all primary forms of anxiety (social stress, anxiety trait, and worry) and participants from an anxiety-disordered sample demonstrated a tendency to avoid risk in behavioral risk-taking tasks [38]. In several studies, sex differences in anxiety states were documented: women exhibit greater fear and are more likely to develop anxiety disorders than men. The sex differences are present in early childhood, increase during the next years, and continue through adolescence [39]. Similar differences exist in adult samples [40,41,42].
The influence of the age factor on the level of disease concerns changing the degree of spatial activity is expected; in particular, a propensity to risk-taking decision making reveals significant age differences. Risky behavior is most expressed in young people because selection pressure was ancestrally more significant for this demographic group than for any other age groups. Youth more than any other age group is engaged in risky behavior in various areas, including health risks [43]. As a result, young people could experience less wariness associated with the potential virus threat and are less likely to limit their movement activity. Thus, Jungmann and Witthöft, in their investigation of the factors related to coronavirus level of worry, have found that respondents belonging to the middle age category (30–59 years) differed from the younger (up to 30 years) age group in the direction of more significant level of anxiety [44].
The higher wariness in the different sex-age groups can also be related to the fear of economic losses: the responsibilities pressure on men in their middle ages is high; they need to care about providing for their less protected relatives—children, elderly parents, wives who do not have their own income. Thus, within different sex-age groups, spatial activity during the pandemic may differ. A rapid growth in economic anxiety has been documented worldwide since the beginning of the COVID-19 epidemic. Based on data collected in 194 countries, Fetzer with coauthors [45] found that beliefs about the high mortality from the coronavirus and its contagiousness were substantially positively associated with economic wariness. However, the reason for this association may be the individual level of anxiety and subjective beliefs about pandemic risks, which in particular depend on fundamental sex differences in level of worry.
The goal of this study was to analyze the sex differences in spatial mobility, level of worry and resource acquisition during the time of the first wave COVID-19 quarantine in urban samples from postindustrial societies.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample and Data

The current study included 490 participants (427 from Russia, 21 from Ukraine, 9 from Kazakhstan, 8 from Belarus, 4 from Germany, 2 from Poland, 2 from Switzerland, and 19 from other countries). Hence, the sample’s distribution was highly diverse in terms of studied geographical regions and population density in place of individual residence. The Russian language was the only inclusion criterion for recruitment of participants saves the age that was supposed to be above 17 years old. The sample was gender-balanced (female N = 225; males N = 265) with the mean age of the participants being 35.49 (SD = 10.03).

2.2. Procedures

Participants were recruited primarily via “Pickabu,” one of the most popular entertainment websites in the Russian-language segment of the Internet, which allow their users to aggregate social news via messages. In a set of public messaging potential participants were asked to take part in research devoted to coronavirus investigations and complete the online survey about their spatial activity and mobility during the pandemic. Recruitment did not offer any compensation. Participants provided informed consent and completed the questionnaires via secure data collection software. All research participants received unique subject IDs that do not identify them as individuals.
The study took place from 29 March 2020 to 27 June 2020 (Median 12-APR-2020), during the virus outbreak in Russia accompanied by an obligatory strict quarantine regime.

2.3. Field Measurements

In this study, we implemented a preregistered self-report questionnaire suitable for measuring the mobility and spatial behavior during the pandemic COVID-19. We focused on examining the one-factor structure of the three items that constitute the individual spatial behavior. A latent variable was manifested within three items: (1) weekly most distant place from home as the measure of elapsed time in minutes required to get to that place, (2) daily most distant place from home in minutes, and (3) the number of trips away from home during last week. The reliability of these three items was assessed by exploratory factor analysis with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.64, which may be classified as an acceptable level of reliability according to generally accepted rules.
The health attitude variable was obtained through the following item: “How do you assess the reliability of the threat posed by the new virus?” Participants were asked to rate the degree of danger that this new virus could have on a 10-point Likert scale.
Level of worry was determined by the question: “How do you feel about the current situation with the coronavirus COVID-19? Indicate the degree of your level of worry.” Items were anchored on a 10-point Likert scale; participants responded by indicating the degree to which they agreed with the item description (from 1 “calm” to 10 “strong level of worry”).
The question about the self-estimated level of the negative effect that could be imposed by quarantine restrictions on the family income—economic losses or negative impact of the household subsistence—was of particular interest. Participants were asked to rate the degree of economic losses that COVID-19 could impose, rated on a 10-point Likert scale.
The primary demographic attributes and items concerned a household structure were included in the survey. Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1 and Table 2 for males and females, respectively. Correlations between tested traits in the study sample are represented in Table 3.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS statistical software (IBM Corp) and via R statistical programming environment. Here we used the lavaan package [46] for R. The analyses give readers an in-depth look at the relationships between the key variables highlighting the effects in regression in the structural equation modeling (SEM) framework. Structural equation modeling (SEM) is among the fastest growing statistical techniques in evolutionary psychology and ecology. It provides a new way to explore and quantify relationships in diversity of systems with numbers of predictors and responses with complex causal connections. SEM unites multiple variables in a single causal network, thereby allowing simultaneous tests of multiple hypotheses. The idea of causality is central to SEM as the technique implicitly assumes that the relationships among variables represent causal links. Because variables can be both predictors and responses, SEM is also a useful tool for quantifying both direct and indirect (cascading) effects. The structural model displays the interrelations among latent constructs and observable variables in the proposed model as a succession of structural equations—akin to running several regression equations. In traditional SEM, the relationships among variables (i.e., their linear coefficients) are estimated simultaneously in a single variance-covariance matrix.

3. Results

3.1. The Structural Model

Preliminary analyses (Table 3) demonstrated that individual mobility (spatial activity level) is neither significantly associated with the age of the respondent, nor with the number of children in a family, nor with the presence of elderly people within the household. It has not revealed any correlation with the individual marital status. We could not find any impact of economic losses on the individual level of spatial activity and mobility.
The most consistent predictor of the observed range in variation of activity level demonstrated in our study was a health attitude variable. The latter variable reflects individual coronavirus perception, subjective beliefs about pandemic risks and understanding of the threat of a new virus.
Simultaneously, the health attitude had a profound association with the reported level of worry, which assumes that the anxiety state significantly impacted the personal response in the health attitude parameter.
To represent the casual relationships between the heath attitude, level of worry reported by the respondents, and the scope of examined predictors, we performed the structural modeling in the lavaan package in R, which enabled us to represent the theoretical predictions of the relationship between the studied factors within one model.
Table 4 shows the causal structure of the model’s pathways with the parameter estimates included in the model and the model indexes. All pathways in the current model appeared to be significant; pathways with lower significance levels were removed from the model (Figure 1).
According to our results, the most consistent predictor of the spatial activity or individual mobility was an adequate health attitude, which means that the individual level of subjective beliefs about pandemic risks and the understanding of the threat of the new coronavirus disease was crucial for decision-making about the spatial activity pattern. Our data suggest that activity levels have a causal relationship with the variable “health attitude,” and 36% of the individual spatial behavior variation could be explained by the personal concern of the potential virus threat. A higher level of concern about the virus threat significantly minimizes the individual level of spatial activity.
In turn, our model revealed that a health attitude pattern is positively associated with individual level of worry (Estimates = 0.513; z-value = 12.912; P(>|z|) < 0.001).
In the next casual pathways, we demonstrated that the level of worry was significantly associated with sex (Estimates = 0.345; z-value = 3.954; P(>|z|) < 0.001), in that females reported a higher level of wariness in a context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Economic losses (Estimates = 0.196; z-value = 4.475; P(>|z|) < 0.001) had a notable impact on the wariness, and finally, family size also showed a significant positive association (Estimates = 0.073; z-value = 2.740; P(>|z|) = 0.006).
We did not find any significant associations between the three dependent variables—(1) activity, (2) health attitude, and (3) anxiety level—and the presence of family members vulnerable to COVID-19: children or elderly people. However, a total number of individuals in a household was a significant factor, that increased the personal concern about the virus. We suppose this association was due to the higher probability of being infected by the COVID-19 within a larger family, or in a place of co-residence of several people, given that the higher number of inhabitants could affect the community’s health, and increase the respiratory infection rate among flat-dwellers. Figure 2 reveals the significant positive increase of anxiety with the increase of family size (or with the increase of the number of co-habitants) in a place of residence.

3.2. Sex-Specific Differences in the Multi-Sample SEMs Model Parameters

Given that the full group model may mask the effects specific to each sex, the multiple group model has been implemented. A detected non-invariance in the model intercepts (Chi-Squared difference p (χ2) < 0.00305)) and partly non-significant pathways represent the evidence in favor of systematic sex-specific differences in the model parameters being examined. We performed two SEMs separately for each sex. Figure 3 and Figure 4 demonstrate the path diagrams for the males’ (panel A) and the females’ (Panel B) sample.
To operationalize the differences between the two sexes in our models, a system of orthogonal contrasts has been introduced. By comparing the magnitudes and direction of the interactions to those of the main sex-specific effects, we demonstrated the strong negative predictive reliance of the health attitude (understanding of the threat) to the activity level for both sexes; however, male activity was significantly higher than female activity (B = 16, p = 0.019886, t-value = 2.337). We also detected that the level of anxiety in males was lower (B = 32, p = 0.0623, t-value = −1.869). Hence, for the male sample, wariness was a less important factor in forming the adequate health attitude.
Observed differences—a level of wariness—also demonstrated more generous contributions into the level of concern about the virus threat in females (Male: Parameter Estimate = 0.439, P(>|z|< 0.001, z-value = 7.36); Female: Parameter Estimate = 0.598, P(>|z| < 0.001, z-value = 11.007).
In both groups (males and females), the causality pathways from the level of worry to the health attitude are highly significant with major parameter estimates, suggesting the leading role of anxiety in resulting health attitude parameters (Figure 5). More than a half (252 out of 592 scored 6 and over, min = 1, max = 10, Mean = 5.74, SD = 2.5) of the respondents reported moderate to severe wariness and anxiety states associated with COVID-19, which made it possible to include this variable into a set of the main factors indirectly affecting spatial activity.

3.3. Sex Differences in Mobility and Potential Explanation in a Family Role of Men and Women

To investigate the potential cues and premises underlining the sex dimorphism in spatial activity, additional testing was implemented. The results suggested significant differences between the individuals with partners and singles for both sex (Figure 6). Females with a partners reported less spatial activity during the previous 7 days of quarantine (F = 7.35; p = 0.007; N (co-habitation with a partner) = 158, with Mean = 27.69, SD = 2.2; CI 95% [23.34; 32.04], compared to females, living alone: N = 104, Mean = 38.99 (min distance), SD = 3.9, CI 95% [31.26; 46.72]; while males with a partner reported significantly higher level of spatial activity for the same period (F = 15.84, p = 0.018; N (co-habitation with a partner) = 120, with Mean = 38.58, SD = 3.89; CI 95% [30.88; 46.28], compared to singles N = 99, Mean = 27.87 (min distance), SD = 2.26, CI 95% [23.38; 32.35].
The observed differences in mobility patterns in males and females in the context of the current (SARS-CoV-2) COVID-19 pandemic may be caused by the division of roles when partners of the opposite sex are cohabiting in the same house. Sex differences in spatial activity do increase once families have offspring. Figure 7 demonstrates the potential differences in activity levels between the individuals who have children younger than 12 years old and those who have not. The key differences were demonstrated for males (Figure 7). In households with children, males showed a significantly higher level of spatial activity.
In both sexes, no significant association between the presence of elderly people in the household and the level of activity was found.

3.4. Economic Losses and Their Impact on the Level of Worry in Men and Women

One of the sufficient parameters affecting the level of wariness could have sociological and economic issues: an economic losses and potential negative effect on the economic stability of the household could directly affect the general level of wariness and anxiety, which became more obvious in the males’ sample (Male: Parameter Estimate = 0.267, P(>|z|| < 0.001, z-value = 4.037); Female: Parameter Estimate = 0.132, P(>|z| = 0.002, z-value = 2.270).
Figure 8 represents a growing trend of the marginal means values of the subjective level of worry in a context of COVID-19 for males’ and females’ samples that positively corresponds to the individual assessment of the negative effect on the economy.

4. Discussion

In this paper, we examined a link between several critical biosocial and socioeconomic parameters and personal mobility or spatial activity of Russian-speaking Internet users in the current COVID-19 pandemic context. We tested the effect of sex, age, household size and structure, economic losses, perceived virus threat, and wariness associated with the new COVID-19 virus. We did not find any association between the level of activity and the presence of elderly people in the households neither in men nor in women. Hence, our data supports the paternal investment hypothesis: altruistic intentions and a growth in spatial activity in adult males mostly took place in cases of children presence in the households, rather than the elderly people—who are the most vulnerable part of society.
The important finding is that the general level of spatial activity was sex-specific: males reported a significantly higher mobility level during the pandemic than females. These data are in agreement with previous studies on the sex differences in spatial activity and mobility, particularly, the overall higher level of general spatial activity in males [26,47]. These sex differences seem to be robust, assuming that men are utilizing larger territories than women across a broad spectrum of geographical and subsistence contexts [14,48,49]. Besides this, males outperform women in some spatial and navigational tasks [15,50]. Although the sex differences in spatial perception have been small, around half a standard deviation [51,52], they were demonstrated cross-culturally and have been found also in post-industrial societies. It is assumed that sexual dimorphism in spatial activity in contemporary societies may be a marker of social and economic inequality [26], biological predispositions, and sex-specific reproductive strategies [12,13,47].
In this paper, we highlighted that observed sex differences in spatial behavior potentially had been predefined by increasing females’ avoidance of any risks associated with social contacts within a dangerous environment in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Several studies where domain-specific risk has been investigated confirmed that females were less likely to engage in risky behavior with stronger reaction on a threat, especially in the health/safety context of decision making [28,29,30,31]. Kruger with coauthors identified five risk-taking domains that reflect recurrent survival and reproductive challenges that humans have faced during our evolutionary history. This domain-specific scale includes the risks associated with environmental challenges, such as a significant risk of mortality from predators, accidents, and parasites. In all these evolutionarily important areas of risk-related decision making, men showed a significantly higher propensity to risk-taking decisions than women, except one environmental risk domain in one study (where female respondents were more likely to take risks to save their infants [33]). Slovic [30] examined sex differences in risk perception in the health/safety domain and found sex differences in the perception of all risk categories related to health and safety. Similar results were recently obtained for the Russian sample [53].
According to our study, women revealed a higher level of wariness associated with the new viral threat, as was anticipated. Numerous studies describe sex differences in the endocrine, behavioral, and neural aspects of stress response. Women are much more susceptible to anxiety disorders, including stress-related major depressive disorders [41,54,55,56,57,58,59]. McLean and Anderson revealed that women exhibit greater fear and are more likely to develop anxiety disorders than men [41]. Our findings are consistent with data obtained by other scholars [60,61,62,63]. Jungmann and Witthöft recently investigated the impact of the coronavirus pandemic on anxiety levels in the German sample, and demonstrated that women reported substantially higher viral anxiety [44].
In the pandemic context, a higher level of anxiety among females followed by a reduction in their mobility as a risk-avoiding behavior is consistent with the theoretical principles of evolutionary biology, predicting differences in natural selection strength imposed upon males and females. We suggest that the notable reduction of any form of spatial activity in females may be due to mechanisms associated with the increase in anxiety or wariness. A high level of anxiety and the avoidance of risky decisions that have been triggered by increased environmental threat may be highly adaptive for women in situations where men are acting as primary protectors and providers.
The sex differences found by us in spatial and risk-taking behavior may reflect evolutionary evolved adaptive strategies still relevant to modern post-industrial environments. Risky behavioral strategies of young men generally promote social status, resource control and marital competition, ultimately increasing reproductive success. At the same time, female risk-taking behavior reduces the chances of reproductive success [64,65].
On the other hand, the growing level of wariness and the concern about the threat posed by the virus in the males’ sample has been primarily associated with financial problems and potential economic loss. Our findings support the theoretical predictions of sexual dimorphism in parental and reproductive roles of male to female in family [21,66]. Specialization in family roles related to spatial activity and mobility originated in our evolutionary past, when males faced more significant navigational challenges for resource acquisition, namely, during hunting [15,67]. The potential differences between the males’ and the females’ samples in their responses to the economic challenges provoked by the new coronavirus COVID-19 could be due to a greater involvement of males in household provisioning and resource contribution for family subsistence, which is still being observed in current Russian reality. However, the studies conducted in other countries provided similar information. Similar to our findings, in New Zealand, economic loss and the wellbeing gap was larger among men than women; males rated higher on feeling of anger (9% gap between men who experienced economic loss during lockdown vs. men who did not, compared to a 1% gap among women), and feelings of loneliness (7% gap for men vs. 2% gap for women) [68].
According to our results, under the challenging conditions of the COVID-19 pandemic, the division of sexual roles within Russia’s families was highly consistent and robust. Men who reported that they cohabit with a partner demonstrated a greater level of weekly activity than single men. In contrast, females living with a spouse reported that the weekly activity level has been lower in comparison to females without a partner. The division of sex roles in terms of spatial activity was more evident in males with children under 12 years old. The general level of activity in males with kids was significantly higher when compared to childless males. Hence, we suggest that current male and female strategies are basically reflecting the evolutionary stable strategies of sex roles within a family, related to resource acquisition and economic investments [10,21,69,70,71].
Another important finding of the current study was the critical role of the health attitude, manifested through concern about the virus threat, in maintaining the observed variability of spatial activity and individual mobility. We found that higher confidence in the reality of the virus threat significantly reduces spatial activity. Along with this, an adequate health attitude and understanding of the virus threat were positively associated with the level of worry (anxiety) in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. The lower scores of wariness and anxiety were associated with neglecting the rules of self-isolation and denial of scientifically reliable information about the coronavirus (SARS-CoV-2) and its potential risks.
One of the main conclusions of this study is that a higher level of anxiety and a higher level of wariness may function as powerful psychological mechanism of prevention of virus spreading, due to a sufficient reduction of the individual spatial activity.
However, health concerns and anxiety associated with epidemics/pandemics may have broad negative psychological consequences (e.g., stress, depressive intrusive thoughts, avoidance, fear-increasing) and may be associated with ineffective or unfavorable preventive behavior and adverse longer-term consequences such as persistent pessimism [72,73,74,75,76]. In our study, more than a half of the respondents reported moderate to extreme wariness and anxiety associated with SARS-CoV-2. Other studies suggest that the current COVID-19 pandemic causes considerable psychological and physical stress, and provide comparative data: over 50% of respondents reported a significant growth of virus anxiety in recent months, especially among individuals with an elevated health anxiety trait [4,74,76,77,78].

5. Limitations and Future Agenda

Despite the enforced lockdown regime in late March 2020 in Russia, the phenomenon of the consistent and continued virus spreading highlighted the importance of studies investigating the individual motivations underlying permanent presence of the substantial level of spatial activity that facilitates interpersonal human contacts. The current survey was designed in a relatively short form and successfully introduced in March 2020. The initial goal of our survey was the identification and detection of the critical factors, potentially predisposing individual mobility and violation of enforced social restrictions. Along with the significant influence on spatial activity that the key biosocial attributes could have had such as sex, family structure, and age, the preliminary survey included a number of non-validated single-items: (1) a level of worry, (2) individual level of the economic anxiety (economic losses), and (3) a health attitude variable. As stated in the current paper, the final analyses revealed that these sociopsychological factors have been of unprecedented importance in canalizing human mobility patterns. However, dealing with single-item constructs, which have shown a profound relevance to the initial study’s goals, is an essential limitation of the current study. For example, the level of worry—a single-item variable—could be potentially expanded and, in the future, include a wider range of stress-related factors such as the sense of lack of freedom, etc. Undeniably, these three sociopsychological factors represent more complex constructs than the single-item measures introduced at the beginning of the present research. These points require additional consideration in future studies. We also admit that our survey does not encompass all the sociopsychological factors potentially influencing mobility level during a pandemic. For instance, sufficient impact of the attitude toward restrictions and fear of punishments by authorities on the individual spatial mobility level could be expected.
At the same time, the current results may be treated as successful and novel in outlining the key components influencing human mobility during the pandemic. Our study highlighted the future work agenda, which is supposed to implement a multidisciplinary approach and focus on analyzing the most consistent factor explaining mobility differences—the level of concern about the virus threat. It is important to keep in mind that the deviation of the health attitude variable to a great extent is laying beyond the scope of anxiety state and stress-related factors, which had been studied in this paper and well-documented within psychological and sociological literature in the past. The resulting single-item health attitude variable in this paper potentially embodies various psychological features such as coronavirus perceptions, subjective beliefs about pandemic risks, the understanding of the danger of a new virus, and even sustainability to the amplification of vast amounts of disinformation (misinformation) about COVID-19, in other words, to the “Infodemic” pressure. These questions stand beyond the tasks of current paper, represented evolutionary approach predominantly focused on sex differences.
The dramatic drop in offline field research availability during the lockdown regime and ultimately orientation on the online data collecting resulted in the essential shortage of the sample size. During the first phase of lockdown in Russia, 492 participants were engaged in the survey, which we regard as an insufficient and relatively small sample size. The latter is an essential limitation of our study resulting in several drawbacks. For instance, the latent three-items construct “Activity” representing the joint weights of manifested variables of actual spatial activity patterns has not shown sufficiently internal consistency according to the relatively low Cronbach’s Alpha. This lowering Cronbach’s Alpha value we attribute mostly to the small sample size. However, we claim that the construct “Activity” still is a reliable latent variable since, in the CFA procedure, we have based it on the solid hypothesis that a mobility manifested through the objective behavioral acts of spatial activity included in the current survey. We realize that the “Activity” construct’s essential limitation lies in the sphere of the subjectivity of self-reported spatial activity data. In future studies, the construct “Activity” reliability has to be enhanced by expanding the sample size, introducing repeated measuring into the survey, and/or by implementing the big data and the self-phone individual geopositioning data into the future research design.

Author Contributions

All authors have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work. Conceptualization: O.S., J.A. and M.B.; methodology: O.S., J.A. and M.B.; software: O.S.; validation: J.A.; formal analysis: O.S.; investigation: O.S., J.A. and M.B.; resources: O.S., J.A. and M.B.; data curation: O.S.; writing—original draft preparation: O.S., J.A.; writing—review and editing: O.S., J.A. and M.B.; visualization: O.S.; supervision: M.B.; project administration: O.S. and M.B.; funding acquisition: M.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The article was prepared in the framework of a research grant funded by the Ministry of Science and Higher Education of the Russian Federation: ID:075-15-2020-910.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Review Board of the Institute of Ethnology and Anthropology, Russian Academy of Sciences and in accordance the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board.

Informed Consent Statement

All subjects in this study gave written informed consent for participation in this study and publication of it’s results.

Data Availability Statement

The data is available on request from the authors.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all our respondents, participated in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Fenichel, E.P. Economic considerations for social distancing and behavioral based policies during an epidemic. J. Health Econ. 2013, 32, 440–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Torales, J.; O’Higgins, M.; Castaldelli-Maia, J.M.; Ventriglio, A. The outbreak of COVID-19 coronavirus and its impact on global mental health. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Bults, M.; Beaujean, D.J.M.A.; de Zwart, O.; Kok, G.; van Empelen, P.; van Steenbergen, J.E.; Richardus, J.H.; Voeten, H.A.C.M. Perceived risk, anxiety, and behavioural responses of the general public during the early phase of the Influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands: Results of three consecutive online surveys. BMC Public Health 2020, 11, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Jalloh, M.F.; Li, W.; Bunnell, R.E.; Ethier, K.A.; O’Leary, A.; Hageman, K.M.; Sengeh, P.; Jalloh, M.B.; Morgan, O.; Hersey, S.; et al. Impact of Ebola experiences and risk perceptions on mental health in Sierra Leone, July 2015. BMJ Glob. Health 2018, 3, e000471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Jones, J.H.; Salathe, M. Early assessment of anxiety and behavioral response to novel swine-origin influenza A (H1N1). PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e8032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Main, A.; Zhou, Q.; Ma, Y.; Luecken, L.J.; Liu, X. Relations of SARS-related stressors and coping to Chinese college students’ psychological adjustment during the 2003 Beijing SARS epidemic. J. Couns. Psychol. 2011, 58, 410–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Trenado, C.; Santillana, M.; Ruge, D. Stress as a meaningful variable in models of covid-19 spreading. PsyArXiv 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Clobert, J.; Baguette, M.; Benton, T.G.; Bullock, J.M. (Eds.) Dispersal Ecology and Evolution; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  9. Lawson Handley, L.J.; Perrin, N. Advances in our understanding of mammalian sex-biased dispersal. Mol. Ecol. 2007, 16, 1559–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Strassmann, B.I. Sexual selection, paternal care, and concealed ovulation in humans. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1981, 2, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Lovejoy, C.O. The origin of man. Science 1981, 211, 341–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Gavrilets, S. Human origins and the transition from promiscuity to pair-bonding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 9923–9928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Gaulin, S.J.C. Evolution of sex difference in spatial ability. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1992, 35, 125–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Gaulin, S.J.C.; Fitzgerald, R.W. Sexual selection for spatiallearning ability. Anim. Behav. 1989, 37, 322–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Gaulin, S.J.C.; Hoffman, H. Evolution and development of sex differences in spatial ability. In Human Reproductive Behaviour: A Darwinian Perspective; Betzig, L., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Turke, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1988; pp. 129–152. [Google Scholar]
  16. Eals, M.; Silverman, I. The hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: Proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of 410 object arrays. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1994, 15, 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Watson, N.V. Sex differences in throwing: Monkeys having a fling. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2001, 5, 98–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Draper, P.; Hames, R. Birth order, sibling investment, and fertility among Ju/’Hoansi (! Kung). Hum. Nat. 2000, 11, 117–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Hewlett, B.S. Demography and childcare in preindustrial societies. J. Anthropol. Res. 1991, 47, 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Hill, K.; Hurtado, A.M. The evolution of premature reproductive senescence and menopause in human females. Hum. Nat. 1991, 2, 313–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Irons, W. Human female reproductive strategies. Soc. Behav. Female Vertebr. 1983, 169–213. [Google Scholar]
  22. Marlowe, F. Paternal investment and the human mating system. Behav. Process. 2000, 51, 45–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Golledge, R.G.; Stimson, R.J. Spatial Behavior: A Geographic Perspective; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA; London, UK, 1996; p. 620. [Google Scholar]
  24. Xu, Y.; Shaw, S.L.; Zhao, Z.; Yin, L.; Lu, F.; Chen, J.; Fang, Z.; Li, Q. Another tale of two cities: Understanding human activity space using actively tracked cellphone location data. Ann. Am. Assoc. Geogr. 2016, 106, 489–502. [Google Scholar]
  25. Gao, H.; Tang, J.; Liu, H. Exploring social-historical ties on location-based social networks. In Proceedings of the 6th International AAAI Conference on Web and Social Media, Dublin, Ireland, 4–7 June 2012; Volume 6, p. 1. [Google Scholar]
  26. Zhang, F.; Wu, L.; Zhu, D.; Liu, Y. Social sensing from street-level imagery: A case study in learning spatio-temporal urban mobility patterns. ISPRS J. Photogramm. Remote Sens. 2019, 153, 48–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kang, C.; Gao, S.; Lin, X.; Xiao, Y.; Yuan, Y.; Liu, Y.; Ma, X. Analyzing and geo-visualizing individual human mobility patterns using mobile call records. In Proceedings of the 2010 18th International Conference on Geoinformatics, Beijing, China, 18–20 June 2010; pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  28. Byrnes, J.P.; Miller, D.C.; Schafer, W.D. Gender differences in risk-taking: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 1999, 125, 367–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Harris, C.R.; Jenkins, M.; Glaser, D. Gender differences in risk assessment: Why do women take fewer risks than men? Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2006, 1, 48–63. [Google Scholar]
  30. Slovic, P. Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. Risk Anal. 1999, 19, 689–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  31. Nicholson, N.; Soane, E.; Fenton-O’Creevy, M.; Willman, P. Personality and domain-specific risk taking. J. Risk Res. 2005, 8, 157–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Weber, E.U.; Blais, A.R.; Betz, N.E. A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2002, 15, 263–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kruger, D.J.; Wang, X.T.; Wilke, A. Towards the development of an evolutionarily valid domain-specific risk-taking scale. Evol. Psychol. 2007, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Giorgetta, C.; Grecucci, A.; Zuanon, S.; Perini, L.; Balestrieri, M.; Bonini, N.; Sanfey, A.G.; Brambilla, P. Reduced risk-taking behavior as a trait feature of anxiety. Emotion 2012, 12, 1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kashdan, T.B.; Collins, R.L.; Elhai, J.D. Social anxiety and positive outcome expectancies on risk-taking behaviors. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2006, 30, 749–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lerner, J.S.; Keltner, D. Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cogn. Emot. 2000, 14, 473–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Maner, J.K.; Schmidt, N.B. The role of risk avoidance in anxiety. Behav. Ther. 2006, 37, 181–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Maner, J.K.; Richey, J.A.; Cromer, K.; Mallott, M.; Lejuez, C.W.; Joiner, T.E.; Schmidt, N.B. Dispositional anxiety and risk-avoidant decision-making. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2007, 42, 665–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rodrigues, P.F.; Pandeirada, J.N.; Bem-Haja, P.; França, J. The Trait Anxiety Scale for Children: A validation study for European Portuguese children and adolescents. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 15, 472–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kessler, R.C.; Berglund, P.; Demler, O.; Jin, R.; Merikangas, K.R.; Walters, E.E. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2005, 62, 593–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. McLean, C.P.; Anderson, E.R. Brave men and timid women? A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 29, 496–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Seedat, S.; Scott, K.M.; Angermeyer, M.C.; Berglund, P.; Bromet, E.J.; Brugha, T.S.; Demyttenaere, K.; de Girolamo, G.; Haro, J.M.; Jin, R.; et al. Cross-national associations between gender and mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2009, 66, 785–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Greitmeyer, T.; Kastenmüller, A.; Fischer, P. Romantic motives and risk-taking: An evolutionary approach. J. Risk Res. 2013, 16, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Jungmann, S.M.; Witthöft, M. Health anxiety, cyberchondria, and coping in the current COVID-19 pandemic: Which factors are related to coronavirus anxiety? J. Anxiety Disord. 2020, 102239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Fetzer, T.; Hensel, L.; Hermle, J.; Roth, C. Coronavirus perceptions and economic anxiety. Rev. Econ. Stat. 2020, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Rosseel. Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5–12 (BETA). J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–36. [Google Scholar]
  47. Cashdan, E.; Marlowe, F.W.; Crittenden, A.; Porter, C.; Wood, B.M. Sex differences in spatial cognition among Hadza foragers. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2012, 33, 274–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. MacDonald, D.; Hewlett, B. Reproductive interests and forager mobility. Curr. Anthropol. 1999, 40, 501–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Ecuyer-Dab, I.; Robert, M. Spatial ability and home-range size: Ex415 amining the relationship in Western men and women (Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol. 2004, 118, 217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Lawton, C. Gender, Spatial Abilities, and Wayfinding. In Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology; Chrisler, J., McCreary, D., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2010; pp. 317–341. [Google Scholar]
  51. Linn, M.; Petersen, A. Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Dev. 1985, 56, 1479–1498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Voyer, D.; Voyer, S.; Bryden, M.P. Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 117, 250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Fedenok, Y.U.N.; Burkova, V.N. Social’noe distancirovanie kak al’truizm v usloviyah pandemii koronavirusa: Kross-kul’turnoe issledovanie (Social distancing as altruism in the context of the coronavirus pandemic: A cross-cultural study). Sib. Hist. Res. 2020, 2. [Google Scholar]
  54. Bekhbat, M.; Neigh, G.N. Sex differences in the neuro-immune consequences of stress: Focus on depression and anxiety. Brain Behav. Immun. 2018, 67, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Donner, N.C.; Lowry, C.A. Sex differences in anxiety and emotional behavior. Pflügers Arch. 2013, 465, 601–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Inslicht, S.S.; Metzler, T.J.; Garcia, N.M.; Pineles, S.L.; Milad, M.R.; Orr, S.P.; Marmar, C.R.; Neylan, T.C. Sex differences in fear conditioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013, 47, 64–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Jalnapurkar, I.; Allen, M.; Pigott, T. Sex differences in anxiety disorders: A review. J. Psychiatry Depress. Anxiety 2018, 4, 012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Kelly, M.M.; Tyrka, A.R.; Anderson, G.M.; Price, L.H.; Carpenter, L.L. Sex differences in emotional and physiological responses to the Trier Social Stress Test. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2008, 39, 87–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Leach, L.S.; Christensen, H.; Mackinnon, A.J.; Windsor, T.D.; Butterworth, P. Gender differences in depression and anxiety across the adult lifespan: The role of psychosocial mediators. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2008, 3, 983–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Ahmed, M.A.K.; Alansari, B.M. Gender differences in anxiety among undergraduates from ten Arab countries. Soc. Behav. Pers. 2004, 32, 649–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Feingold, A. Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 1994, 116, 429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Paro, H.B.M.S.; Silveira, P.S.P.; Perotta, B.; Gannam, S.; Enns, S.C.; Giaxa, R.R.B.; Bonito, R.F.; Martins, M.A.; Tempski, P.Z. Empathy among medical students: Is there a relation with quality of life and burnout? PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e94133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Burkova, V.N.; Butovskaya, M.L.; Dronova, D.A.; Apal’kova, Y.I. Empatiya, trevozhnost’ i agressiya u moskovskih studentov (Empathy, anxiety and aggression in Moscow students). Ethnogr. Rev. 2019, 5, 169–188. [Google Scholar]
  64. Wilson, M.; Daly, M. Lethal confrontational violence among young men. In Adolescent Risk Taking; Bell, N.J., Bell, R.W., Eds.; Sage Publications: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 2020; pp. 84–106. [Google Scholar]
  65. Baker, M.D., Jr.; Maner, J.K. Risk-taking as a situationally sensitive male mating strategy. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2008, 29, 391–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Kramer, K.; Boone, J. Why intensive agriculturalists have higher fertility: A household energy budget approach. Curr. Anthropol. 2002, 43, 511–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Silverman, I.; Eals, M. Sex differences in spatial abilities: Evolutionary theory and data. In The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture; Barkow, J.H., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 533–549. [Google Scholar]
  68. Prickett, K.C.; Fletcher, M.; Chapple, S.; Doan, N.; Smith, C. Life in Lockdown: The Economic and Social Effect of Lockdown during Alert Level 4 in New Zealand. Available online: https://researcharchive.vuw.ac.nz/xmlui/bitstream/handle/10063/9022/WP-20-03-covid-19-life-in-lockdown.pdf?sequence=1 (accessed on 19 January 2021).
  69. Westermarck, E. Marriage; Jonathan Cape and Harrison Smith: New York, NY, USA, 1929. [Google Scholar]
  70. Morris, D. The Naked Ape; Dell: New York, NY, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
  71. Lancaster, J.B.; Lancaster, C.S. Parental Investment: The Hominid Adaptation; Ortner, D., Ed.; Smithsonian Institute Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
  72. Gaygisiz, U.; Gaygisiz, E.; Ozkan, T.; Lajunen, T. Individual differences in be-havioral reactions to H1N1 during a later stage of the epidemic. J. Infect. Public Health 2012, 5, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Peng, E.Y.-C.; Lee, M.-B.; Tsai, S.-T.; Yang, C.-C.; Morisky, D.E.; Tsai, L.-T.; Weng, Y.-L.; Lyu, S.-Y. Population-based post-crisis psychological distress: An example from the SARS outbreak in Taiwan. J. Formos. Med. Assoc. 2010, 109, 524–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Qiu, J.; Shen, B.; Zhao, M.; Wang, Z.; Xie, B.; Xu, Y. A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: Implications and policy recommendations. Gen. Psychiatry 2020, 33, e100213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Wang, C.; Pan, R.; Wan, X.; Tan, Y.; Xu, L.; Ho, C.S.; Ho, R.C. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Cao, W.; Fang, Z.; Hou, G.; Han, M.; Xu, X.; Dong, J.; Zheng, J. The psychological impact of the COVID-19 epidemic on college students in China. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 112934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Huang, Y.; Zhao, N. Generalized anxiety disorder, depressive symptoms and sleep quality during COVID-19 outbreak in China: A web-based cross-sectional survey. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 288, 112954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Goulia, P.; Mantas, C.; Dimitroula, D.; Mantis, D.; Hyphantis, T. General hospital staff worries, perceived sufficiency of information and associated psychological distress during the A/H1N1 influenza pandemic. BMC Infect. Dis. 2010, 10, 322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. SEM. Individual activity in a pandemic. Note. The ellipse represents a factor “Spatial activity” with three indicators and is also called latent variable, constructs, or unobserved variable. Rectangles represent measured variables. A line with one arrow represents a hypothesized direct relationship between two variables. The strength of the association between variables is defined by arrow thickness. Negative relationships have pessimistic parameter estimates characterized by red arrows and positive relationships by green color.
Figure 1. SEM. Individual activity in a pandemic. Note. The ellipse represents a factor “Spatial activity” with three indicators and is also called latent variable, constructs, or unobserved variable. Rectangles represent measured variables. A line with one arrow represents a hypothesized direct relationship between two variables. The strength of the association between variables is defined by arrow thickness. Negative relationships have pessimistic parameter estimates characterized by red arrows and positive relationships by green color.
Sustainability 13 01110 g001
Figure 2. Number of inhabitants in a place of residence and the mean level of anxiety (z-score).
Figure 2. Number of inhabitants in a place of residence and the mean level of anxiety (z-score).
Sustainability 13 01110 g002
Figure 3. Panel A: SEM for males (N = 213).
Figure 3. Panel A: SEM for males (N = 213).
Sustainability 13 01110 g003
Figure 4. Panel B: SEM females (N = 252).
Figure 4. Panel B: SEM females (N = 252).
Sustainability 13 01110 g004
Figure 5. The level of worry in the sample: a distribution of answers for males and females.
Figure 5. The level of worry in the sample: a distribution of answers for males and females.
Sustainability 13 01110 g005
Figure 6. Difference in weekly spatial activity levels between the individuals with partners and singles (in males’ and females’ groups respectively) during the pandemic’s quarantine.
Figure 6. Difference in weekly spatial activity levels between the individuals with partners and singles (in males’ and females’ groups respectively) during the pandemic’s quarantine.
Sustainability 13 01110 g006
Figure 7. The difference in spatial activity levels (zscores) between individuals who have kids and who do not (for male and female groups).
Figure 7. The difference in spatial activity levels (zscores) between individuals who have kids and who do not (for male and female groups).
Sustainability 13 01110 g007
Figure 8. Negative economic outcomes and their impact on the level of anxiety for males and females, respectively. Both variables z-transformed.
Figure 8. Negative economic outcomes and their impact on the level of anxiety for males and females, respectively. Both variables z-transformed.
Sustainability 13 01110 g008
Table 1. Descriptive statistics (Males).
Table 1. Descriptive statistics (Males).
Descriptive Statistics Males
NRangeMinMaxMeanSD
Age22552176934.939.480
Family size225200202.411.893
Children in a family2253030.420.658
Level of worry22591105.232.596
Health attitude (virus concern)22591106.802.473
Economic losses level22591104.533.229
Daily distance in minutes221120012017.5922.540
Weekly activity level225100105.613.309
Weekly distance in minutes219240.00240.033.74035.3020
N217
Table 2. Descriptive statistics (Females).
Table 2. Descriptive statistics (Females).
Descriptive Statistics Females
NRangeMinMaxMeanSD
Age26551186936.0210.444
Family size2638082.601.324
Children in a family2633030.440.662
Level of worry26491106.192.338
Health attitude (virus concern)26491107.172.383
Economic losses level26591104.623.085
Daily distance in minutes262180018013.6922.048
Weekly activity level263100104.443.128
Weekly distance in minutes262180.00180.032.17833.3903
N257
Table 3. Correlations between tested traits in the study sample.
Table 3. Correlations between tested traits in the study sample.
Correlation Matrix
Activity
Arithmetic Mean
AgeHealth Attitude
(z-Score)
Level of Worry
(z-Score)
Economic Losses
(z-Score)
SexMarital StatusFamily SizeElderly PeopleChildren
Activity
Mean
1.000
Age−0.0261.000
Health attitude
(z-score)
−0.2680.0601.000
Level of worry (z-score)−0.1350.1340.5191.000
Economic
losses
(z-score)
−0.0110.0640.0660.2131.000
Sex−0.1260.0540.0750.1920.0141.000
Marital status0.0060.1550.0010.2080.1120.0491.000
Family size0.0380.1200.0380.1460.1100.0580.3001.000
Elderly people −0.0180.168−0.0280.030−0.137−0.007−0.1180.1941.000
Children in a family0.0780.0690.0190.0620.0510.0140.3220.513−0.0181.000
Note. Bolded numbers are statistically significant with p-values < 0.05; N = 490; Activity level (M = 0.0027, SD = 2.287) represents an arithmetic mean of three z-transformed parameters: (1) weekly activity level, where M = 33.18 (minutes), SD = 34.4857; (2) daily activity level, where M = 15.74 (minutes), SD = 22.816; (3) a number of trips out of home during last week with M = 5 times per week and SD = 3.2685; Age of respondent M = 35.49, SD = 10.03; Health attitude M = 6.9918, SD = 2.4395; Level of worry M = 5.7393, SD = 2.508; Economic losses M = 4.5691, SD = 3.147; Sex of respondent: males N = 225 and females N = 265; Marital status with two parameters (1) “Cohabit with a partner” (N = 286); “Single” (N = 206); Family size Min = 0, Max = 20 with M = 2.51, SD = 1.612; Presence of elderly people in the household with two parameters “Yes” (N = 63) and “No” (N = 427); Number of children in a family M = 0.43, SD = 0.659.
Table 4. SEM Individual activity in a pandemic.
Table 4. SEM Individual activity in a pandemic.
PathwayEstimateStd. EstimateSECritical Ratiop
Activity    →Weekly distance0.6880.7360.05911.5940.000 **
  →Weekly activity0.5080.5380.0539.5390.000 **
→Daily distance0.4870.5250.0529.4280.000 **
Activity                      ←Health attitude−0.355−0.3350.061−5.8170.000 **
Health attitude          ←Level of worry0.5130. 5100.04012.9120.000 **
Level of worry ←Economic losses0.1960.1970.0444.4750.000 **
  ←Sex0.3460.1730.0883.9450.000 **
                ←Family size0.0740.1210.0272.7400.006 **
Note. Model χ2 was 20.858, df = 20, p < 0.406. Significant paths with p-value ≥ 0.001 denotes by sign **. The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was 0.010; with Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = 0.998; Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) = 0.997. R-Square: Weekly distance = 0.54; Weekly activity = 0.289; Daily distance = 0.276; Health attitude = 0.260; Level of worry = 0.092; Spatial Activity = 0.112.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Semenova, O.; Apalkova, J.; Butovskaya, M. Sex Differences in Spatial Activity and Anxiety Levels in the COVID-19 Pandemic from Evolutionary Perspective. Sustainability 2021, 13, 1110. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13031110

AMA Style

Semenova O, Apalkova J, Butovskaya M. Sex Differences in Spatial Activity and Anxiety Levels in the COVID-19 Pandemic from Evolutionary Perspective. Sustainability. 2021; 13(3):1110. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13031110

Chicago/Turabian Style

Semenova, Olga, Julia Apalkova, and Marina Butovskaya. 2021. "Sex Differences in Spatial Activity and Anxiety Levels in the COVID-19 Pandemic from Evolutionary Perspective" Sustainability 13, no. 3: 1110. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13031110

APA Style

Semenova, O., Apalkova, J., & Butovskaya, M. (2021). Sex Differences in Spatial Activity and Anxiety Levels in the COVID-19 Pandemic from Evolutionary Perspective. Sustainability, 13(3), 1110. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13031110

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop