Next Article in Journal
Vitamin D Supplementation and Blood Pressure in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Next Article in Special Issue
Quantifying Diet Intake and Its Association with Cardiometabolic Risk in the UK Airwave Health Monitoring Study: A Data-Driven Approach
Previous Article in Journal
Efficacy of Lactobacillus paracasei HA-196 and Bifidobacterium longum R0175 in Alleviating Symptoms of Irritable Bowel Syndrome (IBS): A Randomized, Placebo-Controlled Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Moderate Weight Loss Modifies Leptin and Ghrelin Synthesis Rhythms but Not the Subjective Sensations of Appetite in Obesity Patients
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Hot, Tired and Hungry: The Snacking Behaviour and Food Cravings of Firefighters during Multi-Day Simulated Wildfire Suppression

1
School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, Central Queensland University, Adelaide 5034, Australia
2
Institute of Physical Activity and Nutrition (IPAN), School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Deakin University, Geelong 3216, Australia
3
Faculty of Health, University of Technology, Sydney 2007, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2020, 12(4), 1160; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12041160
Submission received: 20 March 2020 / Revised: 18 April 2020 / Accepted: 19 April 2020 / Published: 21 April 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Circadian Misalignment, Nutrition, and Cardiometabolic Health)

Abstract

:
Firefighters are exposed to numerous stressors during wildfire suppression, including working in hot temperatures and sleep restricted conditions. Research has shown that when sleep restricted, individuals choose foods higher in carbohydrates, fat, and sugar, and have increased cravings for calorie dense foods. However, there is currently no research on the combined effect of heat and sleep restriction on snacking behaviour. Conducting secondary analyses from a larger study, the current study aimed to investigate the impact of heat and sleep restriction on snacking behaviour and food cravings. Sixty-six firefighters completed three days of simulated physically demanding firefighting work and were randomly allocated to either the control (n = 18, CON; 19 °C, 8 h sleep opportunity), sleep restricted (n = 16, SR; 19 °C, 4-h sleep opportunity), hot (n = 18, HOT; 33 °C, 8 h sleep opportunity), or hot and sleep restricted (n = 14 HOT + SR; 33 °C, 4-h sleep opportunity) condition. During rest periods firefighters were able to self-select sweet, savoury, or healthy snacks from a ration pack and were asked to rate their hunger, fullness, and cravings every two hours (eating block). Mixed model analyses revealed no difference in total energy intake between conditions, however there was a significant interaction between eating block and condition, with those in the CON, HOT, and HOT + SR condition consuming significantly more energy between 1230 and 1430 compared to the SR condition (p = 0.002). Sleep restriction and heat did not impact feelings of hunger and fullness across the day, and did not lead to greater cravings for snacks, with no differences between conditions. These findings suggest that under various simulated firefighting conditions, it is not the amount of food that differs but the timing of food intake, with those that are required to work in hot conditions while sleep restricted more likely to consume food between 1230 and 1430. This has potential implications for the time of day in which a greater amount of food should be available for firefighters.

1. Introduction

Wildland fires pose a significant threat to people and property, and frequently occur in Australia, North America, and Southern Europe [1,2]. Australia faces some of the most severe wildland fires, and consequently have some of the world’s largest fire and rescue agencies, consisting of many volunteer rural firefighters [3,4]. During wildfire suppression, firefighters can work extended hours over multiple days, and during this time experience a myriad of physical and emotional stressors, including extreme temperatures reaching 45 °C [5], rough terrain, sleep restriction, limited food availability, and smoke [4,6,7]. Of these stressors, heat and sleep restriction are particularly prevalent.
During wildfire suppression, temperatures can range from mild to extreme 18–45 °C [5]. Performing physical work in hot conditions (30 °C) is linked to increased levels of acute stress, with studies showing increases in cortisol levels [8,9] and acute inflammatory responses [10,11]. The majority of research shows that the physical performance of firefighters is largely unaffected in hot conditions [12]. However, cognitive performance is negatively affected under hot conditions [13,14,15]. This is concerning given firefighters are often tasked with making safety critical decisions [4]. For example, firefighters must assess, react, and respond to hazardous situations, and impaired decision making can impact not only their own safety, but that of their crew members and civilians.
During wildfire operations, firefighters are also required to work extended shifts, over multiple days [16], and during wildfire suppression this can result in restricted sleep [17]. Due to a number of factors such as shift start time, shift length, and sleeping in hot, smoky, and noisy conditions, some research has shown that firefighters can sleep on average for as little as 3–4 h per day [6,18]. While this level of sleep restriction can impair cognitive performance [13,14], it is also associated with greater risk of long-term health effects [19,20], with cardiometabolic disease prevalent among firefighters [21,22]. Further, the additive effect of sleep restriction in hot conditions has a synergistic effect on cognitive performance when compared with performance in hot or sleep restricted conditions alone [13,14], which has the potential to influence long-term health.
While the aforementioned body of research highlights some of the impacts of hot and sleep restricted conditions, what is unknown, is the influence of heat and sleep restriction on the food intake of firefighters during wildfire suppression. This is important to understand given that previous research in laboratory settings have found that food intake can influence on-shift cognitive performance [23] and can influence sleep quality and quantity [24]. For example, recent findings suggest that a large meal can negatively impact reaction time and sustained attention, two critical cognitive processes during wildfire suppression, compared to a smaller snack [25]. In addition, from a long-term health perspective, increased food intake on-shift, particularly at night, is linked to obesity due to consuming food at times when the body is not primed for digestion [23,26]. This can lead to chronic health conditions such as Type 2 diabetes [27] and cardiovascular disease [28].
Restricted sleep can impact food intake due to altered rhythms of hunger and appetite hormones [29]. Decreased levels of leptin and increased levels of ghrelin have been found following restricted sleep, leading to increased hunger and cravings for food during the day [30,31]. Further, the type of food consumed on-shift can also be influenced by sleep restriction. For example, shiftworkers have reported consuming fewer main meals and increased snacks when sleep restricted [23,25,32]. In addition, research examining food choices when sleep restricted have noted an increase in the consumption of carbohydrates [31,32,33], fat [34], and sugar [35]. Taken together, these findings suggest that, in sleep restricted conditions, workers may self-select snacks high in carbohydrates, fat and sugar.
Compared to cold temperature, food intake may be less following physically demanding tasks in hot temperatures [36]. For example, studies have found suppressed appetite in hot temperatures compared to cooler temperatures [37,38,39]. In one study, there was no difference between energy intake post-exercise between hot and neutral temperatures [40]. This suggests that perhaps exercise impacts the relationship between heat and appetite. However, it is yet to be determined what the effect on appetite would be in hot temperatures after longer periods of physical activity, such as the activity that firefighters perform on-shift [16,41]. In addition to appetite, energy expenditure is increased in conditions of heat [42,43] and sleep restriction [44,45], and this is thought to impact energy intake [46]. Therefore, it is possible that the combined effect of heat and sleep restriction on energy expenditure may influence food choice for wildland firefighters.
The first step in understanding the impact of food on firefighters’ health and safety is to understand the food intake patterns of these firefighters, particularly when working in unique shift conditions. In a study of metropolitan-based Australian firefighters, food intake was largely determined by shift schedules, attitudes of co-workers, time, accessibility, and health [47]. However, these firefighters were not deployed in wildfire conditions, and sleep restriction and heat were not considered as potential influences on hunger and food choice. The individual and combined effect of sleep restriction and heat on food intake, hunger, and the type of food that firefighters are craving is yet to be determined. Knowledge of such interactions may help firefighting agencies to be better prepared to supply firefighters with ration packs suitable to different wildfire suppression conditions. Therefore, the aim of this study was to conduct secondary analyses investigating the impact of sleep restricted to 4-h, and hot conditions, on food and macronutrient intake during snack opportunities, and the subjective ratings of hunger, fullness, and cravings of firefighters during multi-day simulated wildfire suppression.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Volunteer and salaried rural firefighters (n = 66; 56 males) were recruited from state fire agencies across the southern states/territories of Australia (Victoria, South Australia, New South Wales, Australian Capital Territory, and Tasmania). Firefighters from northern states/territories were excluded due to their exposure to hotter average temperatures. Firefighters were also excluded if they were under 18 years of age, had a diagnosed sleep disorder, condition limiting their usual work (e.g., injury, illness, or pregnancy), or any contraindications to exercise.
To limit the variation between the condition groups, firefighters were matched by age, sex, and body mass index (BMI) and then randomly allocated to one of four conditions; control (CON), sleep restricted (SR), hot (HOT), or hot and sleep restricted (HOT + SR). Participant demographics are presented in Table 1. There were no statistically significant differences between age or BMI between conditions [48], and the sample is broadly representative of wildland firefighter populations [18]. Data collection occurred from March to August 2012–2013 (autumn and winter months in Australia that are characterised by temperate to cool weather). All firefighters completed a medical questionnaire to ensure they were physically able to participate and subsequently provided written informed consent. Ethical approval was obtained from the Central Queensland University (H12/01-016) Ethics Committee and Deakin University (2010-170) Human Research Ethics Committee.
Data are presented as mean ± SD. CON = Control; SR = Sleep restricted; HOT = Hot; HOT + SR = Hot and sleep restricted.

2.2. Study Design

The current study was part of a large multidisciplinary project that investigated the impact of temperature, sleep, hydration, and stress on firefighters’ physical and cognitive performance during simulated wildfire scenarios. Details on methodology (including the pre-experimental protocol) and previously published research findings can be found here [8,12,48]. The methodology and measures specific to the current study are presented below.
Simulated wildfire scenarios took place in South Australia, Victoria, and Canberra. The study occurred across five consecutive days including an adaptation day, three experimental days, and a recovery day. Each trial was conducted in groups of 3–5 firefighters. To simulate a wildfire scenario, firefighters were required to ‘live in’ the laboratory for the duration of the study (except for showering and using the bathroom). Experimental conditions (e.g., ambient day and night temperatures and sleep opportunities) for each condition are shown in Figure 1.

2.3. Experimental Procedures

Upon arrival to the laboratory, firefighters’ heights, measured with a stadiometer (Fitness Assist, Wrexham, UK), and semi-nude body mass, measured with calibrated electronic scale (A and D, Japan), were collected. During all trials, firefighters were required to wear their personal protective clothing to reflect real-life situations (i.e., two-piece jacket and trouser set, suspenders, boots, gloves, and helmet) which amounted to roughly 5 kg in weight, and were provided with a ration pack (3085 kJ) in addition to their daily meals.
The experimental protocol is provided in Figure 1. The first day of the experiment was the adaptation day, to familiarise the firefighters to study procedures. During the experiment days, firefighters followed a strict schedule for their meals, 2-h work circuits, and sleep opportunities. During the 2-h work circuits six physical firefighting tasks were performed. These tasks are used within real-world wildfire suppression (charged hose advance, blackout hose work, hose rolling, lateral repositioning rake, and static hold [48]), and are considered to be the most physically demanding and operationally important of the tasks performed during wildfire suppression work [49]. Previously published data indicate that there were no differences in physical tasks performed between conditions [12,17,18,48,49]. The details of these components of the 2-h work circuit, including descriptions of all tasks and work to rest ratios are reported elsewhere [13,17,18,49]. For Day 1 of the experiment, firefighters completed three 2-h work circuits (6h total). For Day 2 and 3, five 2-h work circuits occurred (10 h total). The final day was for recovery and included one more night of sleep and one 2-h work circuit, of which data is not reported.
In the CON and SR conditions, ambient temperature during the day (0600-1800) was 19 °C (40% relative humidity; RH) and during the night, temperature was 18 °C (40% RH). For the HOT and HOT + SR conditions ambient temperature during the day was 33 °C (40% RH), and during the night temperature was 23 °C (40% RH). Ambient air temperature was maintained throughout the simulated work shifts through the use of split cycle air-conditioners (Daikin Industries Ltd., Osaka, Japan) and portable ceramic disk heaters (Micro Furnace, Sunbeam, Adelaide, Australia). Both were measured and monitored continuously using a wireless temperature and humidity data logger, data receiver, and associated software (HOBO® Pro Software, One Temp Pty Ltd., Marleston, Australia).
Smoking and caffeine consumption were restricted to the rest periods, while firefighters’ fluid consumption and snacking was ad libitum during rest periods. Post-dinner, firefighters engaged in free-time activity (e.g., read a book, watched a movie) until the sleep opportunity commenced. At all times, participants were monitored by researchers to ensure they did not fall asleep or perform physical work at nonspecified times. Participants slept on camp beds to replicate the sleeping conditions during a wildfire suppression deployment [48]. All sleep periods were recorded using standard polysomnography and for each period, total sleep time (min) was calculated. Sleep data is presented elsewhere [50,51]. Participants also wore activity monitors (Actical MiniMitter/Respironics, Bend, OR, USA) to measure sleep for the two days leading into the study. For further information on the methodology see [4,8].

2.4. Measures

2.4.1. Snack Amount and Type

On each experimental day firefighter’s food and drink intake was recorded at each meal and during rest periods. Similar to a fireground setting, during rest periods participants ate ad libitum from a ration pack. This ration pack was 3085 kJ in total and contained a range of snacks identified by subject matter experts as similar to those available during a wildfire situation. The specific snacks provided are outlined in Table 2. Firefighters received a new ration pack for each day, and any leftover snacks were taken away (i.e., snacks did not accumulate). The snacking data was entered into FoodWorks 7 nutrition software (2012 Xyris Software Pty Ltd., High Gate Hill, QLD, Australia). Once the data was compiled in FoodWorks, the type and number of snacks ingested were extracted to identify the total energy intake from snacking, and then specifically for sweet, savoury, and healthy snacks. Macronutrient intake was also extracted from FoodWorks, including protein (g), total fat (g), and carbohydrate (g).

2.4.2. Hunger

Firefighters hunger ratings were obtained using a hunger and cravings questionnaire that was developed for use in a similar study [35]. Firefighters completed this questionnaire as part of each cognitive performance testing battery (13 times total across the three experimental days), prior to eating any snacks. Firefighters were asked to identify their hunger and satisfaction levels on a 100mm point Visual Analogue Scale (VAS). Scores were integers between 0 and 100 (i.e., no decimal place values). Firefighters were asked four questions. (1) How hungry they felt on a scale of “I am not hungry at all” to “I have never been more hungry”, (2) how satisfied they felt on a scale of “I am completely empty” to “I cannot eat another bite”, (3) how full they felt on a scale of “not at all full” to “totally full”, and 4) how much they thought they could eat, “nothing at all” to “a lot”.

2.4.3. Craving, Craving Type, and Craving Intensity

The questionnaire also assessed firefighters’ cravings during each 2-h work circuit (13 times total across the three experimental days). Firefighters were asked an initial question to determine whether they were currently experiencing any desires/urges to eat specific foods. If yes was answered, firefighters were then asked to identify the type of food they were craving from a list; sweet carbohydrate/fat, savoury carbohydrate/fat, protein, or carbohydrate. Finally, firefighters were asked one question to rate the general intensity of their craving on a 100mm VAS scale, 0 (“very weak”) to 100 (“very strong”). The VAS score was an integer between 0 and 100 (i.e., no decimal place values), with a higher score representing a stronger craving intensity.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Data analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 25 (Armonk, New York). Prior to performing analyses, the data were screened for outliers, and no data were excluded based on this. There was missing data (n = 17) for the total intake data resulting in a final sample of n = 49 (CON n = 14; SR n = 16; HOT n = 18; HOT + SR n = 14). There was no missing data for the hunger and cravings data resulting in a final sample of n = 66 (CON n = 14; SR n = 16; HOT n = 11; HOT + SR n = 8). Linear mixed-effects analysis of variance (ANOVA) were used to determine if there were differences in total intake per day for total energy (kJ), carbohydrate (g), total fat (g), and protein (g), with fixed effects of condition (CON, SR, HOT, and HOT + SR), day (2 or 3), and condition × day, and a random effect of participant ID. Mixed effects ANOVAs were also used to determine if there were differences in total intake per food opportunity for total energy (kJ), carbohydrate (g), total fat (g), and protein (g), with fixed effects of condition (CON, SR, HOT, and HOT + SR), day (2 or 3), food opportunity block (1–5), condition × day, condition × block, and day × block, and a random effect of participant ID. Differences in hunger, fullness, and craving intensity were also analysed using mixed effect ANOVAs with fixed effects of condition (CON, SR, HOT, and HOT + SR), day (2 or 3), food opportunity block (1–5), condition × day, condition × block, and day × block, and a random effect of participant ID. Participant BMI was included as a covariate in all models. Due to the relative importance of the two-way interactions, the three-way interaction effects were dropped from the models.
As cravings and cravings type were binary variables, a generalised estimating equation (GEE) was used with a binary logistic regression, with predictors of day (2 or 3), block (1-5), condition (CON, SR, HOT, and HOT + SR), condition × day, and condition × block reported (via Wald’s χ2). This analysis accounts for binary data with repeated measurements for each participant and adjusts for the effects of clustered sampling.
Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05 and post hoc pairwise comparisons were performed using a Bonferroni correction. The data analysed in the current study were secondary to the primary aims of the study, with primary outcomes published elsewhere [8,12,13,14,15,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57].

3. Results

3.1. Total Intake Per Day from Snack Opportunities

As seen in Table 3 there were no significant condition × day interactions for total intake per day on total energy, (kJ), carbohydrate (g), total fat (g), or protein (g).
There were also no significant main effects of condition or day for total intake per day on total energy (kJ), carbohydrate (g), total fat (g), or protein (g; Table 4).

3.2. Total Intake Per Food Opportunity

A significant condition × block interaction was found for total energy intake (kJ; p = 0.002; Table 3 and Figure 2). In block 1 participants in the SR condition consumed less than participants in the CON condition. In block 3 participants in the SR condition consumed less than participants in the CON, HOT, and HOT + SR conditions. In block 4 participants in the HOT condition consumed more than participants in the SR and HOT + SR conditions. There was also a significant condition × block interaction for carbohydrate (g; p = 0.003; Table 3 and Figure 2). In block 1 participants in the SR condition consumed less than participants in the CON condition. In block 3 participants in the SR condition consumed less than participants in the CON, HOT, and HOT + SR conditions. In block 4 participants in the HOT condition consumed more than participants in the CON, SR, and HOT + SR conditions. There were no significant condition × block interactions for total fat (g) or protein (g; Table 3 and Figure 3). There were also no significant condition × day or condition × block interactions for total energy (kJ), carbohydrate (g), total fat (g), or protein (g; Table 3).
There was a significant main effect of the condition for total intake per food opportunity (Table 3) for total energy (kJ; p < 0.001), carbohydrate (g; p < 0.001), and total fat (g; p = 0.019). There was also a significant main effect of food opportunity block (Table 3) for total energy (kJ; p < 0.001), carbohydrate (g; p < 0.001), total fat (g; p < 0.001), and protein (g; p = 0.001). There was not a significant main effect of day (Table 3).

3.3. Ratings of Hunger, Fullness, and Craving Intensity

As seen in Table 3, there were no significant condition × day, condition × block (Figure 3), or day × block interactions for hunger, fullness, or craving intensity. There was a significant main effect of day for craving intensity (p = 0.015), but there was no significant main effect of block for hunger or fullness (Table 3). Significant main effects for food opportunity block were seen for hunger (p < 0.001), fullness (p < 0.001), and craving intensity (p < 0.001; Table 3). There were no significant main effects of condition for hunger, fullness, or craving intensity (Table 3).

3.4. Ratings of Craving and Craving Type

Significant interactions between condition × block were not found for craving (Wald’s χ2 = 16.792, df = 12, p = 0.16), sweet cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 13.95, df = 12, p = 0.30), savoury cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 21.89, df = 12, p = 0.05), protein cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 14.20, df = 12, p = 0.29), or carbohydrate cravings (Wald’s χ2 =14.67, df = 12, p = 0.20). Likewise, no significant interactions were found between condition × day for craving (Wald’s χ2 = 1.96, df = 3, p = 0.58), sweet cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 1.67, df = 3, p = 0.65), savoury cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 3.55, df = 3, p = 0.31), protein cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 3.83, df = 3, p = 0.28), or carbohydrate cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 0.18, df = 3, p = 0.98). There was a significant main effect of block for sweet craving (Wald’s χ2 = 31.03 df = 4, p < 0.001), savoury cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 12.27, df = 4, p = 0.02), protein cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 31.36, df = 4, p < 0 0.01), and carbohydrate cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 12.25, df = 4, p = 0.02), with greater likelihood of reporting cravings in block 5. There was a significant main effect of the condition on sweet cravings (Wald’s χ2 = 8.92, df = 3, p = 0.03), with a greater likelihood of reporting sweet cravings in the control condition. No other main effect was significant.

4. Discussion

This is the first study to investigate the impact of sleep restricted to 4-h, and hot conditions, on food and macronutrient intake during snack opportunities, and the subjective ratings of hunger, fullness, and cravings of firefighters during multi-day simulated wildfire suppression. The findings suggest the timing of food intake differs under these conditions, rather than the total amount of food consumed. Specifically, firefighters consume more kilojoules and more carbohydrates between 1230–1430 when working in hot conditions after 4-h of sleep restriction, and only hot conditions; compared to working in only sleep restricted conditions and in control conditions. Further, sleep restriction and hot conditions did not alter subjective ratings of hunger, fullness, or craving intensity across the day.
There was no difference in total energy intake or total macronutrient intake from snacking between those that were sleep restricted, working in hot conditions, or sleep restricted and working in hot conditions in the current study. However, the timing of food intake differed across the day. These findings are consistent with multiple studies that report that the timing of food intake differed, but not the amount of food consumed, across multiple shift-types [25,58,59]. Of note, the current study is the first to suggest that this pattern of altered meal timing can be found within the same shift-type when worked under different environmental conditions.
The most kilojoules were consumed during the food opportunity between 1230–1430 by those in the hot condition, the hot and sleep restricted condition, and the control condition, while those in the sleep restricted condition consumed the least number of kilojoules. This suggests that sleep restriction alone may not be enough to influence the food intake of firefighters during wildfire suppression, but hot conditions and the additive effect of hot conditions and sleep restriction has an influence on the amount of food eaten. An explanation for this finding may be energy expenditure. Although energy expenditure increases with sleep restriction leading to greater intake [45], physical activity can act as an appetite suppressant [60,61]. Our findings suggest that, for wildland firefighters, physical work may lead to decreased energy intake in sleep restricted conditions, compared to working in sleep restricted conditions with the addition of heat. The additive effect of heat on sleep restriction appears to increase energy intake, perhaps due to increased energy expenditure in hot conditions [42]. Further, that time of day (1230–1430) corresponds to the secondary window of circadian low when pressure for sleep is increased, and this biological sleep pressure would have been further exacerbated by sleep restriction [62,63]. Literature has shown an increase in snacking behaviour with increased sleepiness from sleep restriction [32,34,35] and with increased energy expenditure from sleep restriction [45,46]. Therefore, we may have expected that those in the sleep restricted conditions were more likely to consume the most kilojoules during this snacking period. Previously published results from this study have shown no difference in sleep quality between conditions, and confirmed that total sleep time was less in the sleep restricted condition [49]. Perhaps with a greater number of consecutive days of sleep restriction, or sleep deprivation (e.g. over 24-h awake), which are not uncommon for firefighters [17], we may see this pattern of greater food intake in the sleep restricted condition, although this remains speculative.
Previous laboratory-based studies have shown increased snack consumption, including greater consumption of carbohydrates, when sleep-restricted [35,64]. Methodological differences may explain why this relationship was not observed in the current study. For example, previous laboratory studies that have explored food consumption in sleep restricted individuals often use shiftwork naïve participants [34,35] who may not be as familiar with sleep restriction as the experienced firefighters in the current study. For instance, those more familiar with sleep restriction may experience less of an acute effect of sleep restriction on food choice. This has implications for shiftworkers, as our knowledge about increased food consumption with sleep restriction from laboratory studies may only apply to those with limited shiftwork experience. Further, studies investigating food consumption after sleep restriction have not included physically demanding tasks and/or physical activity. In nonsleep restricted conditions, acute exercise does not influence immediate energy intake [37], but increased energy intake has been found 120 mins post-exercise, corresponding with increased ghrelin concentrations [65]. Current findings suggest this effect may be blunted with sleep restriction. This is important for understanding any changes in food intake in future research, as differences in food intake on-shift are not only impacted by prior sleep, but physical activity on-shift.
Carbohydrate was the only macronutrient to differ based on condition, with higher carbohydrate consumption in the control condition, hot condition, and hot and sleep restricted condition compared to the sleep restricted condition between 1230–1430. Indeed, carbohydrate intake was lower during several blocks in the sleep restricted condition compared to other conditions. As such, the additive effect of the hot conditions and sleep restriction appears to influence carbohydrate intake. This may be due to increased stress levels and decreased alertness in response to these conditions, as in previous shiftwork literature, workers have reported increased carbohydrate consumption to increase alertness and feelings of comfort [25,66]. Further, previous findings have demonstrated increased carbohydrate utilisation during physical activity, particularly in hot conditions, and this has been linked to reduced fatigue [67]. In the current study, when participants were required to be physically active in hot conditions, while also sleep restricted, there may have been a greater drive for carbohydrates, compared to when they were physically active in only hot or sleep restricted conditions, or when they were in a control condition with no sleep restriction and a comfortable temperature. It should be noted that overall the ration pack items were lower in protein and fat than carbohydrates, which may have influenced the detection of between-group differences in protein and fat consumption across the study.
Ratings of hunger and fullness showed the expected pattern based on the standard timing of food intake in Western cultures with increased hunger in the morning before lunch, decreased hunger after lunch, and increased hunger in the afternoon and evening [68]. This pattern also aligns with the circadian patterns of leptin and ghrelin, with leptin decreasing in the afternoon and evening, and ghrelin increasing before lunch and dinner times [30,31,69]. Interestingly, there were no differences in ratings of hunger and fullness between conditions, with participants in all conditions reporting similar levels across the day. Previous research has shown that sleep restricted individuals may report greater hunger during the day due to changes in levels of leptin and ghrelin [31,70]. However, the present results show no difference in the pattern of hunger in those who are sleep restricted. Participants in the current study in the sleep restricted and the hot and sleep restricted conditions had a 4-h sleep opportunity, and this is consistent with the sleep timing reported by firefighters [17]. We would expect to see sleep-related changes based on this level of sleep restriction. It is possible that leptin and ghrelin levels were altered by this sleep restriction but that this did not alter subjective levels of hunger, however this was not measured in the current study.
Of note, the increase in energy intake and carbohydrate intake from 1230–1430 did not correspond to an increase in ratings of hunger, and there were no differences in hydration between the conditions [49]. This suggests that this increase in food intake was not motivated by hunger, but perhaps by alertness, comfort, habit, or boredom. All factors that have previously been reported as influencing food intake on-shift in industries such as healthcare [71,72], factory work [73], transport [74], and aviation [75]. In addition, in one study of the eating behaviours of firefighters, a social eating culture was reported, with ‘communal cook-ups’ on-shift [47]. In the real-world, this social motivation for eating may be a greater influence on eating patterns than alertness and hunger. Taken together, these findings suggest that eating for reasons other than hunger is common amongst many shiftworking industries with unique shift demands and conditions, including firefighters.
Overall there was no effect of sleep restriction or ambient temperature on cravings for food. While cravings increased towards the end of the day, following the pattern of hunger increasing and fullness decreasing at this time, this pattern occurred for all conditions and the intensity of these cravings were small. While the ration packs contained snacks that are commonly provided on-site for firefighters, perhaps these foods were not what the firefighters craved. In addition, this provides support for the argument that firefighters may be consuming food on-shift for reasons other than a craving to eat, for example for comfort or out of habit, as they were consuming food at these times, but were not experiencing strong cravings.
This study is the first to demonstrate a difference in food intake for firefighters working under different conditions of sleep and heat, however there are some limitations. While firefighters were completing fireground tasks during the study, which were representative of actual fire suppression work, and were wearing their personal protective clothing, aspects of the laboratory environment may have limited external validity. For example, aspects of an outdoor environment such as wind, daylight, smoke, and sound, are likely to increase stress levels and potentially effect food intake in the real-world. Further, the participants were familiar with completing the fireground tasks under sleep restricted and hot conditions, and therefore may be more resilient to the effects of these conditions compared to workers in other industries. However, this can also be viewed as a strength of the study, as firefighters are under-represented in the shiftwork literature, and as this group face unique shift constraints, such as extreme temperatures, understanding the impact on food intake is important. Further investigation of the impacts of these conditions in a larger sample, and between males and females may also highlight individual differences in eating patterns. There are also some limitations to the nutritional data. The snacks in the ration packs were not replaced in each eating block but were replaced at the end of each day, and it is possible that there was a ceiling effect from certain snacks and if more of certain snacks were available then more may have been consumed. While the content of the breakfast, lunch, and dinner meals were representative, it is still possible that macro and micronutrient intake could vary. It should also be noted that sample size calculations for the larger study were based on the primary outcome measures which are published elsewhere [13,14,16,48,52].
To build on these findings there are several recommendations for future research. Measuring metabolic factors such as blood glucose, cholesterol, and serum hormone levels would be beneficial in understanding the physiological response to sleep restriction and hot conditions. Similarly, rigorous measures of energy expenditure under the different conditions (heat and sleep restriction) should be considered in future research, as this may impact snacking behaviour and cravings. Further, food intake motivation questionnaires have been used to understand why workers are choosing to eat at certain times [25]. Future research could utilise this questionnaire with firefighters to understand the motivations for eating during the day, as this is important for understanding what is driving eating behavior on-shift. The health effects of different snack choices, for example healthier snacks vs. high fat snacks, could also be investigated in future research in order to reduce the long-term health conditions reported by wildland firefighters [21,22]. Given firefighters are likely to be working into the night [17], replicating the current study during the night is an important next step. Completing the fire-suppression tasks at night, when the drive for sleepiness is greater [62] may lead to altered food timing and altered patterns of hunger compared to during the day, and provide important insights into the potential long-term health risks of eating at night. Understanding the sleepiness of participants pre- and post-bed would also be important for understanding how snack timing influences their mood. Future research could also measure drinking preference, for example are wildland firefighters likely to preference sweet beverages in different conditions.
Firefighters are required to work long shifts in hot temperatures, often with restricted sleep, and understanding how this may impact food intake is important for ensuring that food can be supplied that is suitable to the conditions. Findings from this study suggest that it is not the amount of food that is altered in different conditions, but the timing of food intake. While future research is important to build on these results, this study has demonstrated the importance of understanding the food intake patterns of firefighters during wildfire suppression, and the need for more understanding of what motivates workers to eat on-shift, particularly with unique shift conditions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, G.E.V., S.A.F., B.A. and M.S.; Data curation, C.C.G., M.D., S.E.C. and M.S.; Formal analysis, C.C.G. and M.D.; Funding acquisition, S.A.F. and B.A.; Methodology, G.E.V., S.A.F., B.A. and M.S.; Project administration, G.E.V., S.A.F. and B.A.; Supervision, S.A.F. and B.A.; Writing—original draft, C.C.G., G.E.V., M.D., S.E.C., H.H.K.F., J.H.G. and S.K.; Writing—review & editing, C.C.G., G.E.V., S.A.F., B.A., M.D., S.E.C., H.H.K.F., J.H.G. and S.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was funded by the Bushfire Cooperative Research Centre.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the rural fire fighting agencies including the Country Fire Authority (CFA), Tasmania Fire Service, New South Wales Fire Service, Country Fire Service (CFS), and Australian Central Territory Fire Service for assistance with recruitment and providing volunteers to participate in the study. We thank and gratefully acknowledge the contribution of staff and students from the Central Queensland University (Appleton Institute), and the School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences at Deakin University. The project was funded by the Bushfire Cooperative Research Centre.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Phillips, M.; Payne, W.; Lord, C.; Netto, K.; Nichols, D.; Aisbett, B. Identification of physically demanding tasks performed during bushfire suppression by Australian rural firefighters. Appl. Ergon. 2012, 43, 435–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Van den Berg, M.J.; Wu, L.J.; Gander, P.H. Subjective Measurements of In-Flight Sleep, Circadian Variation, and Their Relationship with Fatigue. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 2016, 87, 869–875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Taylor, N.A.; Dodd, M.J.; Taylor, E.A.; Donohoe, A.M. A retrospective evaluation of injuries to Australian urban firefighters (2003 to 2012). J. Occup. Envion. Med. 2015, 57, 757–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Aisbett, B.; Wolkow, A.; Sprajcer, M.; Ferguson, S.A. “Awake, smoky, and hot”: Providing an evidence-base for managing the risks associated with occupational stressors encountered by wildland firefighters. Appl. Ergon. 2012, 43, 916–925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Raines, J.; Snow, R.; Petersen, A.; Harvey, J.; Nichols, D.; Aisbett, B. Pre-shift fluid intake: Effect on physiology, work and drinking during emergency wildfire fighting. Appl. Ergon. 2012, 43, 532–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Cater, H.; Clancy, D.; Duffy, K.; Holgate, A.; Wilison, B.; Wood, J. Fatigue on the fireground: The DPI experience. In Proceedings of the Australasian Fire Authorities Council/Bushfire Co-Operative Research Centre Annual Conference (2007), Hobart, Tasmania, 18–20 July 2007. [Google Scholar]
  7. Cuddy, J.S.; Gaskill, S.E.; Sharkey, B.J.; Harger, S.G.; Ruby, B.C. Supplemental feedings increase self-selected work output during wildfire suppression. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2007, 39, 1004–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Wolkow, A.; Ferguson, S.A.; Vincent, G.E.; Larsen, B.; Aisbett, B.; Main, L.C. The Impact of Sleep Restriction and Simulated Physical Firefighting Work on Acute Inflammatory Stress Responses. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0138128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Wolkow, A.; Aisbett, B.; Reynolds, J.; Ferguson, S.A.; Main, L.C. The impact of sleep restriction while performing simulated physical firefighting work on cortisol and heart rate responses. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2016, 89, 461–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wolkow, A.; Aisbett, B.; Jefferies, S.; Main, L.C. Effect of Heat Exposure and Simulated Physical Firefighting Work on Acute Inflammatory and Cortisol Responses. Ann. Work Expo. Health 2017, 61, 600–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Guy, J.H.; Vincent, G.E.J.S. Nutrition and supplementation considerations to limit endotoxemia when exercising in the heat. Sports (Basel) 2018, 6, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Larsen, B.; Snow, R.; Vincent, G.; Tran, J.; Wolkow, A.; Aisbett, B. Multiple Days of Heat Exposure on Firefighters’ Work Performance and Physiology. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0136413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Smith, B.P.; Browne, M.; Armstrong, T.A.; Ferguson, S.A. The accuracy of subjective measures for assessing fatigue related decrements in multi-stressor environments. Saf. Sci. 2016, 86, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ferguson, S.A.; Smith, B.P.; Browne, M.; Rockloff, M.J. Fatigue in Emergency Services Operations: Assessment of the Optimal Objective and Subjective Measures Using a Simulated Wildfire Deployment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Cvirn, M.A.; Dorrian, J.; Smith, B.P.; Vincent, G.E.; Jay, S.M.; Roach, G.D.; Charli, S.; Brianna, L.; Brad Aisbett, S.A.F. The effects of hydration on cognitive performance during a simulated wildfire suppression shift in temperate and hot conditions. Appl. Ergon. 2019, 77, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Vincent, G.E.; Aisbett, B.; Hall, S.J.; Ferguson, S.A. Sleep quantity and quality is not compromised during planned burn shifts of less than 12 h. Chronobiol. Int. 2016, 33, 657–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Vincent, G. Fighting Fires and Fatigue: Sleep, Physical Activity, and Physical Task Performance. Ph.D. Thesis, Deakin Univeristy, Melbourne, VIC, Australia, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  18. Vincent, G.E.; Aisbett, B.; Wolkow, A.; Jay, S.M.; Ridgers, N.D.; Ferguson, S.A. Sleep in wildland firefighters: What do we know and why does it matter? Int. J. Wildland Fire 2018, 27, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Cappuccio, F.P.; Miller, M.A. Sleep and cardio-metabolic disease. Curr. Cardiol. Rep. 2017, 19, 110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Rajaratnam, S.M.; Howard, M.E.; Grunstein, R.R. Sleep loss and circadian disruption in shift work: Health burden and management. Med. J. Aust. 2013, 199, S11-5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Achmat, G.; Leach, L.; Onagbiye, S.O. Prevalence of the risk factors for cardiometabolic disease among firefighters in the Western Cape province of South Africa. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fit. 2019, 59, 1577–1583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Soteriades, E.S.; Smith, D.L.; Tsismenakis, A.J.; Baur, D.M.; Kales, S.N. Cardiovascular disease in US firefighters: A systematic review. Cardiol Rev. 2011, 19, 202–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Lowden, A.; Moreno, C.; Holmbäck, U.; Lennernäs, M.; Tucker, P. Eating and shift work—effects on habits, metabolism, and performance. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2010, 36, 150–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Binks, H.; Vincent, G.E.; Gupta, C.; Irwin, C.; Khalesi, S. Effects of Diet on Sleep: A Narrative Review. Nutrients 2020, 12, 936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Gupta, C.C.; Coates, A.M.; Dorrian, J.; Banks, S. The factors influencing the eating behaviour of shiftworkers: What, when, where and why. Ind. Health 2019, 57, 419–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Bes-Rastrollo, M.; Sanchez-Villegas, A.; Basterra-Gortari, F.J.; Nunez-Cordoba, J.M.; Toledo, E.; Serrano-Martinez, M. Prospective study of self-reported usual snacking and weight gain in a Mediterranean cohort: The SUN project. Clin Nutr. 2010, 29, 323–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Nyamdorj, R. Obesity and Type 2 Diabetes. In Epidemiology of Type 2 Diabetes, 1st ed.; Qiao, Q., Ed.; Bentham Science Publishers Ltd.: Sharjah, UAE, 2012; pp. 39–64. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lavie, C.J.; Milani, R.V.; Ventura, H.O. Obesity and cardiovascular disease: Risk factor, paradox, and impact of weight loss. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2009, 53, 1925–1932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Scheer, F.A.; Morris, C.J.; Shea, S.A. The internal circadian clock increases hunger and appetite in the evening independent of food intake and other behaviors. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2013, 21, 421–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Schmid, S.M.; Hallschmid, M.; Jauch-Chara, K.; Born, J.; Schultes, B. A single night of sleep deprivation increases ghrelin levels and feelings of hunger in normal-weight healthy men. J. Sleep Res. 2008, 17, 331–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Spiegel, K.; Tasali, E.; Penev, P.; Van Cauter, E. Brief communication: Sleep curtailment in healthy young men is associated with decreased leptin levels, elevated ghrelin levels, and increased hunger and appetite. Ann. Intern. Med. 2004, 141, 846–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Reinberg, A.; Migraine, C.; Apfelbaum, M.; Brigant, L.; Ghata, J.; Vieux, N.; Laporte, A.; Nicolai. Circadian and ultradian rhythms in the feeding behaviour and nutrient intakes of oil refinery operators with shift-work every 3–4 days. Diabete Metab. 1979, 5, 33–41. [Google Scholar]
  33. Nedeltcheva, A.V.; Kilkus, J.M.; Imperial, J.; Kasza, K.; Schoeller, D.A.; Penev, P.D. Sleep curtailment is accompanied by increased intake of calories from snacks. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009, 89, 126–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Cain, S.W.; Filtness, A.J.; Phillips, C.L.; Anderson, C. Enhanced preference for high-fat foods following a simulated night shift. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2015, 41, 288–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Heath, G.; Roach, G.D.; Dorrian, J.; Ferguson, S.A.; Darwent, D.; Sargent, C. The effect of sleep restriction on snacking behaviour during a week of simulated shiftwork. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012, 45, 62–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Johnson, R.E.; Kark, R.M. Environment and food intake in man. Science 1947, 105, 378–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Mandic, I.; Ahmed, M.; Rhind, S.; Goodman, L.; L’Abbe, M.; Jacobs, I. The effects of exercise and ambient temperature on dietary intake, appetite sensation, and appetite regulating hormone concentrations. Nutr. Metab. (Lond.) 2019, 16, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Zheng, G.; Li, K.; Wang, Y. The effects of high-temperature weather on human sleep quality and appetite. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Wasse, L.K.; King, J.A.; Stensel, D.J.; Sunderland, C. Effect of ambient temperature during acute aerobic exercise on short-term appetite, energy intake, and plasma acylated ghrelin in recreationally active males. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2013, 38, 905–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Shorten, A.L.; Wallman, K.E.; Guelfi, K.J. Acute effect of environmental temperature during exercise on subsequent energy intake in active men. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009, 90, 1215–1221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Chappel, S.E.; Aisbett, B.; Vincent, G.E.; Ridgers, N.D. Firefighters’ physical activity across multiple shifts of planned burn work. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Gold, A.J.; Zornitzer, A.; Samueloff, S. Influence of season and heat on energy expenditure during rest and exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 1969, 27, 9–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mudambo, K.S.; Scrimgeour, C.M.; Rennie, M.J. Adequacy of food rations in soldiers during exercise in hot, day-time conditions assessed by doubly labelled water and energy balance methods. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1997, 76, 346–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Barf, R.; Van Dijk, G.; Scheurink, A.; Hoffmann, K.; Novati, A.; Hulshof, H.; Fuchs, E.; Meerlo, P. Metabolic consequences of chronic sleep restriction in rats: Changes in body weight regulation and energy expenditure. Physiol. Behav. 2012, 107, 322–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. McNeil, J.; Doucet, É.; Brunet, J.-F.; Hintze, L.J.; Chaumont, I.; Langlois, É.; Maitland, R.; Riopel, A.; Forest, G. The effects of sleep restriction and altered sleep timing on energy intake and energy expenditure. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 164, 157–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Blundell, J.E.; Caudwell, P.; Gibbons, C.; Hopkins, M.; Naslund, E.; King, N.; Finlayson, G. Role of resting metabolic rate and energy expenditure in hunger and appetite control: A new formulation. Dis. Model. Mech. 2012, 5, 608–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Bonnell, E.K.; Huggins, C.E.; Huggins, C.T.; McCaffrey, T.A.; Palermo, C.; Bonham, M.P.J.N. Influences on dietary choices during day versus night shift in shift workers: A mixed methods study. Nutrients 2017, 9, 193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Vincent, G.; Ferguson, S.A.; Tran, J.; Larsen, B.; Wolkow, A.; Aisbett, B. Sleep restriction during simulated wildfire suppression: Effect on physical task performance. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0115329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Vincent, G.E.; Aisbett, B.; Larsen, B.; Ridgers, N.D.; Snow, R.; Ferguson, S.A. The impact of heat exposure and sleep restriction on firefighters’ work performance and physiology during simulated wildfire suppression. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Cvirn, M.; Smith, B.; Jay, S.; Vincent, G.; Ferguson, S. (Eds.) The impact of temperature on the sleep characteristics of volunteer firefighters during a wildland fireground tour simulation. In Proceedings of the 14th Annual Scientific Meeting of the Australasian Chronobiology, Waiheke Island, New Zealand, 23–24 October 2017. [Google Scholar]
  51. Cvirn, M.A.; Dorrian, J.; Smith, B.P.; Jay, S.M.; Vincent, G.E.; Ferguson, S.A. The sleep architecture of Australian volunteer firefighters during a multi-day simulated wildfire suppression: Impact of sleep restriction and temperature. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 99, 389–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Larsen, B.; Snow, R.; Aisbett, B. Effect of heat on firefighters' work performance and physiology. J. Therm. Biol. 2015, 53, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Wolkow, A.; Aisbett, B.; Ferguson, S.A.; Reynolds, J.; Main, L.C. Psychophysiological relationships between a multi-component self-report measure of mood, stress and behavioural signs and symptoms, and physiological stress responses during a simulated firefighting deployment. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2016, 110, 109–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Wolkow, A.; Aisbett, B.; Reynolds, J.; Ferguson, S.A.; Main, L.C. Acute psychophysiological relationships between mood, inflammatory and cortisol changes in response to simulated physical firefighting work and sleep restriction. Appl. Psychophysiol Biofeedback 2016, 41, 165–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Wolkow, A.; Aisbett, B.; Reynolds, J.; Ferguson, S.A.; Main, L.C. Relationships between inflammatory cytokine and cortisol responses in firefighters exposed to simulated wildfire suppression work and sleep restriction. Physiol. Rep. 2015, 3, e12604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Wolkow, A.; Ferguson, S.; Aisbett, B.; Main, L.J. Effects of work-related sleep restriction on acute physiological and psychological stress responses and their interactions: A review among emergency service personnel. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 2015, 28, 183–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Vincent, G.E.; Ferguson, S.; Larsen, B.; Ridgers, N.D.; Snow, R.; Aisbett, B. Adding sleep restriction to the equation: Impact on wildland firefighters’ work performance and physiology in hot conditions. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2018, 91, 601–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Bonham, M.P.; Bonnell, E.K.; Huggins, C.E. Energy intake of shift workers compared to fixed day workers: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Chronobiol. Int. 2016, 33, 1086–1100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kosmadopoulos, A.; Kervezee, L.; Boudreau, P.; Gonzales-Aste, F.; Vujovic, N.; Scheer, F.A.J.L.; Boivin, D.B. Effects of Shift Work on the Eating Behavior of Police Officers on Patrol. Nutrients 2020, 12, 999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Blundell, J.E.; Stubbs, R.J.; Hughes, D.A.; Whybrow, S.; King, N.A. Cross talk between physical activity and appetite control: Does physical activity stimulate appetite? Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2003, 62, 651–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Hubert, P.; King, N.; Blundell, J.J.A. Uncoupling the effects of energy expenditure and energy intake: Appetite response to short-term energy deficit induced by meal omission and physical activity. Appetite 1998, 31, 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Akerstedt, T. Work hours, sleepiness and the underlying mechanisms. J. Sleep Res. 1995, 4, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Åkerstedt, T.; Gillberg, M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 1990, 52, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Sargent, C.; Zhou, X.; Matthews, R.W.; Darwent, D.; Roach, G.D. Daily rhythms of hunger and satiety in healthy men during one week of sleep restriction and circadian misalignment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Erdmann, J.; Tahbaz, R.; Lippl, F.; Wagenpfeil, S.; Schusdziarra, V. Plasma ghrelin levels during exercise—effects of intensity and duration. Regul. Pept. 2007, 143, 127–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Haus, E.; Reinberg, A.; Mauvieux, B.; Le Floc’h, N.; Sackett-Lundeen, L.; Touitou, Y. Risk of obesity in male shift workers: A chronophysiological approach. Chronobiol. Int. 2016, 33, 1018–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Jentjens, R.L.; Wagenmakers, A.J.; Jeukendrup, A.E. Heat stress increases muscle glycogen use but reduces the oxidation of ingested carbohydrates during exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 2002, 92, 1562–1572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Fayet-Moore, F.; McConnell, A.; Cassettari, T.; Tuck, K.; Petocz, P.; Kim, J. Discretionary intake among Australian adults: Prevalence of intake, top food groups, time of consumption and its association with sociodemographic, lifestyle and adiposity measures. Public Health Nutr. 2019, 22, 1576–1589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Reynolds, A.C.; Dorrian, J.; Liu, P.Y.; Van Dongen, H.P.; Wittert, G.A.; Harmer, L.J.; Banks, S. Impact of five nights of sleep restriction on glucose metabolism, leptin and testosterone in young adult men. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e41218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Omisade, A.; Buxton, O.M.; Rusak, B. Impact of acute sleep restriction on cortisol and leptin levels in young women. Physiol. Behav. 2010, 99, 651–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Torquati, L.; Kolbe-Alexander, T.; Pavey, T.; Persson, C.; Leveritt, M. Diet and physical activity behaviour in nurses: A qualitative study. Int. J. Health Promot. Educ. 2016, 54, 268–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Novak, R.D.; Auvil-Novak, S.E. Focus group evaluation of night nurse shiftwork difficulties and coping strategies. Chronobiol. Int. 1996, 13, 457–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Morikawa, Y.; Miura, K.; Sasaki, S.; Yoshita, K.; Yoneyama, S.; Sakurai, M.; Nakagawa, H. Evaluation of the effects of shift work on nutrient intake: A cross-sectional study. J. Occup. Health 2008, 50, 270–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Jack, F.R.; Piacentini, M.G.; Schröder, M.J. Perception and role of fruit in the workday diets of Scottish lorry drivers. Appetite 1998, 30, 139–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Perrin, S.L.; Dorrian, J.; Gupta, C.; Centofanti, S.; Coates, A.; Marx, L.; Beyne, K.; Banks, S. Timing of australian flight attendant food and beverage while crewing: A preliminary investigation. Ind. Health 2019, 57, 547–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Experimental Protocol. CON = Control; SR = Sleep restricted; HOT = Hot; HOT + SR = Hot and sleep restricted.
Figure 1. Experimental Protocol. CON = Control; SR = Sleep restricted; HOT = Hot; HOT + SR = Hot and sleep restricted.
Nutrients 12 01160 g001
Figure 2. Total food intake per food opportunity across food opportunity blocks (1–5). A: total energy (kJ); B: carbohydrate (g); C: total fat (g); D: protein (g). CON = control condition. SR = sleep restricted condition. HOT = hot condition. HOT + SR = hot + sleep restricted condition. Block 1: 0800–1000; block 2: 1000–1200; block 3: 1230–1430; block 4: 1430–1630: block 5: 1630–1830. Lunch was provided at 1200 (after block 2) and dinner was provided at 1900 (after block 5). Error bars indicate standard error. Asterisk indicates a significant difference between conditions (p < 0.05).
Figure 2. Total food intake per food opportunity across food opportunity blocks (1–5). A: total energy (kJ); B: carbohydrate (g); C: total fat (g); D: protein (g). CON = control condition. SR = sleep restricted condition. HOT = hot condition. HOT + SR = hot + sleep restricted condition. Block 1: 0800–1000; block 2: 1000–1200; block 3: 1230–1430; block 4: 1430–1630: block 5: 1630–1830. Lunch was provided at 1200 (after block 2) and dinner was provided at 1900 (after block 5). Error bars indicate standard error. Asterisk indicates a significant difference between conditions (p < 0.05).
Nutrients 12 01160 g002
Figure 3. Ratings of hunger, fullness, and craving intensity across food opportunity blocks (1–5; n = 66). (A): hunger; (B): fullness; (C): craving intensity. CON = control condition. SR = sleep restricted condition. HOT = hot condition. HOT + SR = hot + sleep restricted condition. Block 1: 0800–1000; block 2: 1000–1200; block 3: 1230–1430; block 4: 1430–1630: block 5: 1630–1830. Lunch was provided at 1200 (after block 2) and dinner was provided at 1900 (after block 5). The dotted line represents the midline of the scale. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3. Ratings of hunger, fullness, and craving intensity across food opportunity blocks (1–5; n = 66). (A): hunger; (B): fullness; (C): craving intensity. CON = control condition. SR = sleep restricted condition. HOT = hot condition. HOT + SR = hot + sleep restricted condition. Block 1: 0800–1000; block 2: 1000–1200; block 3: 1230–1430; block 4: 1430–1630: block 5: 1630–1830. Lunch was provided at 1200 (after block 2) and dinner was provided at 1900 (after block 5). The dotted line represents the midline of the scale. Error bars indicate standard error.
Nutrients 12 01160 g003
Table 1. Participant demographics by study condition.
Table 1. Participant demographics by study condition.
Condition
CON
(n = 18)
SR
(n = 16)
HO
T(n = 18)
HOT + SR
(n = 14)
Age (y)39 ± 1639 ± 1536 ± 1341 ± 17
Male:Female15:315:214:412:1
Body mass (kg)85 ± 1894 ± 2088 ± 1884 ± 14
Height (cm)178 ± 8178 ± 7178 ± 9176 ± 4
Body mass index (kg·m2)27 ± 530 ± 628 ± 427 ± 4
Data are presented as mean ± SD. CON = Control; SR = Sleep restricted; HOT = Hot; HOT + SR = Hot and sleep restricted.
Table 2. Contents of firefighters’ ration pack during simulated wildfire suppression.
Table 2. Contents of firefighters’ ration pack during simulated wildfire suppression.
Snack (Quantity)Total Energy (kJ) Per ServingTotal Protein (g) Per ServingTotal Fat (g) Per ServingTotal Carbohydrate (g) Per Serving
Granny smith apple (1)309.00.5016.1
Packet of Sunbeam sultanas (1)532.01.00.427
Jatz crackers (3)216.00.71.97.1
Packet of barbeque Shapes (1)530.01.95.615.8
Packet of vegemite (1)34.01.40.10.6
Arnott’s scotch finger biscuit (1)380.01.13.811.7
Arnott’s nice biscuit (1)216.00.71.78.8
Natural Confectionary Company snakes (5)351.02.10.647.2
Uncle Toby’s raspberry yogurt top muesli bar (1)517.02519.5
Set meals were provided at 0630 (breakfast), 1200 (lunch), and 1900 (dinner), with the same quantities of food items available to all participants. For main meals, participants ate ad libitum from the available foods. Food items in the meals (breakfast, lunch and dinner) were based on the food items that would be available to firefighters on the fireground [47]. These meals were representative in terms of food type, quantity, and variety, and included a hot breakfast of toast and eggs, sandwiches for lunch, and a hot dinner that included pasta, meat, and vegetables [52].
Table 3. Results of mixed effect ANOVAs, showing the main effects of condition, day, and food opportunity block, and the interaction between condition × day, condition × block, and condition × day for total intake per day from snack opportunities and total intake per food opportunity.
Table 3. Results of mixed effect ANOVAs, showing the main effects of condition, day, and food opportunity block, and the interaction between condition × day, condition × block, and condition × day for total intake per day from snack opportunities and total intake per food opportunity.
ConditionDayBlockCondition × DayCondition × BlockDay × Block
F(df)pF(df)pF(df)pF(df)pF(df)pF(df)p
Total Intake Per Day from Snack Opportunities (n = 50)
Total Energy (kJ)1.16(3, 50.86)0.3330.14(1, 41.23)0.710--2.21(3, 43.65)0.100----
Carbohydrate (g)1.42(3, 51.40)0.2480.05(1, 41.05)0.842--1.93(3, 43.31)0.140----
Total Fat (g)0.98(3, 50.96)0.4090.02(1, 46.04)0.897--0.66(3, 49.91)0.583----
Protein (g)1.21(3, 85.00)0.3131.35(1, 85.00)0.248--1.23(3, 85.00)0.303----
Total Intake Per Snack Opportunity (n = 50)
Total Energy (kJ)11.52(3, 435.00)< 0.001 *1.08(1, 435.00)0.30018.57(4, 435.00)< 0.001 *0.34(3, 435.00)0.7972.55(12, 435.00)0.003 *0.22(4, 435.00)0.130
Carbohydrate (g)11.89(3, 435.00)< 0.001 *1.23(1, 435.00)0.26818.273(4, 435.00)< 0.001 *0.26(3, 435.00)0.8542.40(12, 435.00)0.01 *0.32(4, 435.00)0.866
Total Fat (g)3.15(3, 47.93) 0.033 * 2.03(1, 408.89)0.1558.67(4, 392.63)< 0.001 *0.31(3, 407.50)0.8191.59(12, 392.63)0.0930.39(4, 392.63)0.819
Protein (g)1.77(3, 47.81)0.1651.46(1, 406.50)0.2284.29(4, 391.98) 0.002 * 0.51(3, 405.17)0.671.42(12, 4391.89)0.1540.33(4, 391.89)0.984
Ratings of Hunger, Fullness, and Craving Intensity (n = 66)
Hunger0.43(3, 83.14)0.7322.09(1, 739.80)0.14979.07(4, 738.21)< 0.001 *0.39(3, 739.78)0.7621.67(12, 738.21)0.0691.55(4, 738.21)0.186
Fullness0.79(3, 82.83)0.5020.11(1, 739.85)0.74571.30(4, 737.92)< 0.001 *0.55(3, 739.83)0.6511.64(12, 737.92)0.0762.15(4. 737.92)0.073
Craving Intensity0.02(3, 83.09)0.9966.00(1, 731.28)0.015*12.23(4, 730.21)< 0.001 *2.10(3, 731.27)0.0990.88(12, 730.21)0.5681.17(4, 730.26)0.322
Note. * p < 0.05.
Table 4. Total intake per day from snacking opportunities (n = 50) for energy (kJ), carbohydrate, total fat, and protein.
Table 4. Total intake per day from snacking opportunities (n = 50) for energy (kJ), carbohydrate, total fat, and protein.
Total Energy (kJ)Carbohydrate (g)% of Total EnergyTotal Fat (g)% of Total EnergyProtein (g)% of Total Energy
CON
Day 2 1935.37 ± 208.1782.12 ± 8.4971.0113.76 ± 3.3711.909.08 ± 3.0517.67
Day 3 1552.73 ± 208.1568.43 ± 8.4973.7616.53 ± 3.3717.8112.98 ± 3.0531.4
SR
Day 2 1558.73 ± 208.1556.10 ± 7.6460.238.04 ± 3.038.634.71 ± 2.7411.38
Day 3 1733.72 ± 200.0170.12 ± 8.1567.6911.44 ± 3.2411.046.44 ± 2.9313.99
HOT
Day 2 1333.46 ± 187.5062.32 ± 9.6678.2210.86 ± 3.8413.635.17 ± 3.4714.60
Day 3 1733.72 ± 200.0173.19 ± 9.6570.6512.58 ± 3.8412.147.03 ± 3.4715.27
HOT + SR
Day 2 1469.06 ± 250.6860.64 ± 10.2269.0812.95 ± 4.0414.756.28 ± 3.6616.10
Day 3 1039.09 ± 92.0144.77 ± 10.7372.116.43 ± 4.2910.363.86 ± 3.8813.99
Note. CON = control condition. SR = sleep restricted condition. HOT = hot condition. HOT + SR = hot + sleep restricted condition. All results presented are estimated marginal means ± standard error.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gupta, C.C.; Ferguson, S.A.; Aisbett, B.; Dominiak, M.; Chappel, S.E.; Sprajcer, M.; Fullagar, H.H.K.; Khalesi, S.; Guy, J.H.; Vincent, G.E. Hot, Tired and Hungry: The Snacking Behaviour and Food Cravings of Firefighters during Multi-Day Simulated Wildfire Suppression. Nutrients 2020, 12, 1160. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12041160

AMA Style

Gupta CC, Ferguson SA, Aisbett B, Dominiak M, Chappel SE, Sprajcer M, Fullagar HHK, Khalesi S, Guy JH, Vincent GE. Hot, Tired and Hungry: The Snacking Behaviour and Food Cravings of Firefighters during Multi-Day Simulated Wildfire Suppression. Nutrients. 2020; 12(4):1160. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12041160

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gupta, Charlotte C., Sally A. Ferguson, Brad Aisbett, Michelle Dominiak, Stephanie E. Chappel, Madeline Sprajcer, Hugh H. K. Fullagar, Saman Khalesi, Joshua H. Guy, and Grace E. Vincent. 2020. "Hot, Tired and Hungry: The Snacking Behaviour and Food Cravings of Firefighters during Multi-Day Simulated Wildfire Suppression" Nutrients 12, no. 4: 1160. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12041160

APA Style

Gupta, C. C., Ferguson, S. A., Aisbett, B., Dominiak, M., Chappel, S. E., Sprajcer, M., Fullagar, H. H. K., Khalesi, S., Guy, J. H., & Vincent, G. E. (2020). Hot, Tired and Hungry: The Snacking Behaviour and Food Cravings of Firefighters during Multi-Day Simulated Wildfire Suppression. Nutrients, 12(4), 1160. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12041160

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop