Next Article in Journal
Distributions and Trends of the Global Burden of Colorectal Cancer Attributable to Dietary Risk Factors over the Past 30 Years
Next Article in Special Issue
Effects of Acute Ingestion of Caffeine Capsules on Muscle Strength and Muscle Endurance: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Awareness and Availability of Low Sodium Iodized Salt: Results from Formative Research of Promoting Uptake of Low SodiUm Iodized Salt by Rural and Urban HousehoLds in India—The PLURAL Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effects of Acute Caffeine Ingestion on Cognitive Performance before and after Repeated Small-Sided Games in Professional Soccer Players: A Placebo-Controlled, Randomized Crossover Trial
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Association between Caffeine Consumption from Coffee and Tea and Sleep Health in Male and Female Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study

by
Mette van der Linden
1,*,
Margreet R. Olthof
2 and
Hanneke A. H. Wijnhoven
2
1
Department of Health Sciences, Faculty of Science, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
2
Department of Health Sciences, Faculty of Science, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2024, 16(1), 131; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010131
Submission received: 29 November 2023 / Revised: 22 December 2023 / Accepted: 27 December 2023 / Published: 30 December 2023
(This article belongs to the Collection Coffee and Caffeine Consumption for Health and Performance)

Abstract

:
Poor sleep health is common in older adults and is associated with negative health outcomes. However, the relationship between caffeine consumption and sleep health at an older age is poorly understood. This study investigated the association between caffeine consumption and sleep health in community-dwelling older males and females in The Netherlands. Cross-sectional analyses were performed using data from 1256 participants aged 61–101 years from the Longitudinal Ageing Study Amsterdam. Self-reported questions assessed sleep disturbances (including sleep latency, continuity, and early awakening), sleep duration, and perceived sleep quality. Caffeine consumption was determined with questions about frequency, quantity, and type of coffee and tea consumption. Logistic and linear regression models were used, controlling for potential confounders, and interaction by sex and age was tested. Caffeine consumption showed significant interactions with sex (p < 0.005) in association with sleep health outcomes. Older females who abstained from caffeine consumption reported more sleep disturbances (β = 0.64 [95%CI 0.13; 1.15]) and had greater odds of short sleep duration (<7 h/day) (OR = 2.26 [95% CI 1.22; 4.20]) compared to those who consumed caffeine. No associations were observed for long sleep duration (>8 h/day) and perceived sleep quality. No associations were observed in older males. Caffeine abstinence was associated with more sleep disturbances and short sleep duration in older females, but not in males. The observed association in older females may reflect reverse causation, suggesting that females may have different motivations for discontinuing caffeine consumption than males.

1. Introduction

Good sleep health is fundamental to human functioning, health, and well-being [1,2]. Its importance becomes even more evident during ageing as the prevalence of sleep disturbances and disorders increases with age [3,4,5]. Approximately one-fourth of older adults (≥65 years) do not meet the recommended sleep duration of 7 to 8 h per night, with symptoms of insomnia such as difficulty with sleep onset, sleep maintenance, and early morning awakening being particularly common [6]. Poor sleep quality and inadequate sleep duration in older adults are associated with adverse physical and mental health outcomes, including impaired cognitive function [7,8,9,10,11,12,13], reduced functional capacity [14,15,16], disability [17,18], reduced quality of life [19], and increased risk of all-cause mortality [10,20,21,22]. The causes of poor sleep health in older adults are multifactorial and can largely be attributed to age-related factors, including a higher prevalence of medical and psychiatric conditions, the use of multiple medications, and psychosocial or behavioural factors such as loneliness and depression [23]. In addition, nutrition—a modifiable lifestyle factor—may play a role [24].
Caffeine is a commonly used psychoactive substance found in a variety of foods and beverages [25]. In Europe, including The Netherlands, coffee and tea are the main sources of caffeine for adults [26,27]. While caffeine consumption has been associated with beneficial health outcomes, including a reduced risk of all-cause mortality, cardiovascular disease, and type 2 diabetes [28,29], it may also adversely affect sleep health. Caffeine is known to promote wakefulness by antagonizing adenosine A1 and A2 receptors in the brain [30,31]. Adenosine is a naturally occurring chemical in the human body that plays an important role in sleep regulation, primarily through its sleep-promoting effects when binding to its receptors [32]. Additionally, caffeine from coffee may also disrupt sleep by reducing the secretion of melatonin, a hormone responsible for regulating sleep patterns [33]. Controlled laboratory experiments and observational studies in adults have indicated that caffeine consumption can prolong sleep latency (the time it takes to fall asleep after going to bed) and reduce sleep duration, efficiency (defined as the ratio of total sleep time to time spent in bed), and perceived sleep quality [34,35]. However, few studies have examined the relationship between caffeine consumption and sleep health in older adults [36,37,38,39,40], despite the high prevalence of sleep disturbances and disorders in this population.
Previous laboratory experiments have shown that caffeine has a greater acute effect on sleep quality and duration in middle-aged adults compared to younger adults [41,42], particularly at a higher doses of 400 mg [41]. In addition, a large cross-sectional study found that self-reported insomnia attributed to caffeine consumption increased with age [43], suggesting that older adults may be more sensitive to the effects of caffeine than younger adults. However, the results of studies in older adults are inconsistent and inconclusive. Two cross-sectional studies found an association between higher caffeine consumption and shorter self-reported sleep duration [36,37]. In contrast, another cross-sectional study of 337 middle-aged and older adults found no association between caffeine consumption and sleep duration as measured using actigraphy [38]. The association between caffeine consumption and sleep quality in older adults is also inconsistent. One cross-sectional study of hospitalized older adults found that caffeine abstinence was associated with poorer sleep quality [39], whereas another cross-sectional study found that caffeine consumption was associated with better sleep quality in community-dwelling older adults and with poorer sleep quality in hospitalized older adults [40]. Another study of 428 older females found no association between caffeine consumption and sleep quality [37].
The present study aims to investigate the association between caffeine consumption from coffee and tea and sleep health in a representative sample of community-dwelling Dutch older adults, using data from the Longitudinal Ageing Study Amsterdam (LASA). In addition, this study aims to investigate whether associations differ by sex. The importance of considering sex differences in health research is increasingly recognized [44,45]. However, sex differences in the association between caffeine consumption and sleep have received little attention.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design and Participants

For the current study, existing data from LASA were used. LASA is an ongoing cohort study of older adults in The Netherlands that focuses on the determinants, trajectories, and consequences of physical, cognitive, emotional, and social functioning [46]. Data were first collected in 1992–1993 from a cohort of 3107 participants aged 55 to 85 years. Follow-up measurement waves were scheduled roughly every three years thereafter. The sample was derived from a variety of municipalities across three culturally distinct regions in The Netherlands, making it nationally representative. From the same sampling frame, an additional second and third cohort of respondents aged 55 to 64 years were added to the original sample in 2002–2003 and 2012–2013, respectively. More information on sampling and data collection procedures can be found elsewhere [46,47,48]. For this cross-sectional study, we used data collected in 2018–2019 of 1701 participants aged 61 to 101 years from the first, second and third cohort. Data collection consisted of a structured main interview, a structured medical interview with clinical measurements, and a self-administered questionnaire. Participants who did not complete the self-administered questionnaire or whose data on caffeine consumption were missing or invalid were excluded from the analysis. All included participants provided written informed consent. The LASA study protocol was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and received approval from the medical ethics committee of the VU University Medical Centre (IRB numbers: 92/138, 2002/141, 2012/361, and 2016.301).

2.2. Sleep Disturbances, Sleep Duration, and Perceived Sleep Quality

A self-administered questionnaire assessed participants’ sleep health. The presence and frequency of sleep disturbances was measured using three categorical questions about having difficulties with sleep onset, sleep continuity, and early morning awakening. Questions were formulated as follows: ‘Do you experience difficulties falling asleep?’, ‘Do you experience interruptions in your sleep?’, and ‘Do you wake up too early?’. Response options to each of the three questions were ‘almost never’, ‘sometimes’, ‘frequently’, and ‘almost always’. Response options were assigned scores of one, two, three, and four, respectively. These scores were summed to compute a scale ranging from 3 (no sleep disturbances) to 12 (many sleep disturbances). A scale was only computed if participants answered all three questions. Sleep duration was measured with a single open-ended question about the number of hours participants usually sleep each night, registered as total minutes of sleep within 24 h. According to the recommend sleep duration for adults aged ≥ 65 years [49], the variable was categorized into three groups: recommended sleep duration (7–8 h/day), short sleep duration (<7 h/day), and long sleep duration (>8 h/day). Lastly, perceived sleep quality was measured with the question: ‘When you think about the past month, how would you rate your sleep quality?’ (good; somewhat good; poor; very poor). The variable was dichotomized into ‘good’ (good; somewhat good) and ‘poor’ (somewhat poor; very poor).

2.3. Caffeine Consumption from Coffee and Tea

The self-reported questionnaire assessed participants’ coffee and tea consumption over the past month. Participants were asked about their weekly coffee and tea consumption, specifying the frequency in days per week (none; <1; 1; 2; 3; 4; 5; 6; 7 days per week) and the quantity in cups per day (1; 2; 3; 4; 5; 6; 7; 8; 9; ≥10 cups) for each type of coffee (caffeinated; decaffeinated) and tea (black; green; rooibos; herbal; other namely …) on days they consumed these beverages. Teas reported under ‘other, namely …’ were subdivided into the existing response categories if appropriate. Furthermore, cup size was asked separately for coffee and tea. Response options were ‘small (about 125 mL)’, ‘medium (about 165 mL)’, ‘large (about 225 mL)’, ‘other, namely … mL’, and ‘don’t know’. In case of ‘other, namely … mL’, participants could provide a numerical response.
Average daily caffeine consumption from coffee and tea, in milligrams per day, was calculated for each participant as follows. First, the total volume in millilitres on days of consumption was calculated for caffeinated coffee, black tea, and green tea separately. This involved multiplying the reported number of cups a day by the corresponding cup size. If cup size was unknown (don’t know) or missing, this value was imputed by the mean cup volumes within the sample—160 mL for coffee and 178 mL for tea. Additionally, if a participant reported consuming 10 or more cups a day, a total of 10 cups was attributed. Subsequently, an average daily consumption in millilitres was calculated for each beverage by multiplying the total daily volume by the weekly consumption (frequency in days per week) and dividing it by seven (days). For participants indicating a consumption frequency of less than one day per week, a value of 0.5 days per week was assigned. To ascertain the average daily caffeine consumption in milligrams per day, the average daily consumption in millilitres for each beverage was multiplied by the respective caffeine content per 100 mL: 44.5 mg for coffee, 22.0 mg for black tea, and 15.1 mg for green tea [26]. Finally, the total daily caffeine consumption from both coffee and tea was calculated by summing caffeine consumption from each source per participant. As an example, if a participant reported drinking coffee five days a week, with a daily consumption of 2 cups of 125 mL, the calculation is as follows: ((2 cups × 125 mL) × 5 days)/7 days = 179 mL coffee on average per day. This corresponds to an average caffeine intake of 80 mg per day, calculated as follows: 179 mL × (44.5 mg/100 mL). In cases where a participant lacked information on caffeine consumption from one source (coffee or tea), while information on caffeine consumption from the other source was present, missing values were recoded to reflect zero caffeine consumption from that specific missing source (n = 161). For the analyses, caffeine consumption was divided into five categories. The first category included participants who abstained from caffeine consumption (0 mg/day). The subsequent four categories were defined based on quartiles for participants who reported caffeine consumption (>0 mg/day), yielding the following categories: low (≤173 mg/day), moderate (174–272 mg/day), high (273–367 mg/day), and very high (>367 mg/day).

2.4. Other Measurements

Potential sociodemographic (age, sex, level of attained education, partner status, and level of urbanization), health-related (number of chronic diseases, depressive symptoms, body mass index (BMI), and subjective pain), and behavioural (smoking status, alcohol use, and physical activity) confounders were selected a priori based on data availability, prior research, and reported associations with both exposure and outcome. During the main interview, participants were questioned about their sex at birth, age, and level of attained education. Response categories for level of attained education were combined into three categories: low (elementary not completed; elementary education; lower vocational education), intermediate (general intermediate education; intermediate vocational education; general secondary education), and high (higher vocational education; college education; university education). The level of urbanization—defined as the average number of addresses per square kilometre within a one-kilometre radius circle [50]—was measured using a zip code classification system designed by Statistics Netherlands (CBS, Heerlen/Voorburg, The Netherlands), which classifies zip codes into five urbanization levels: not (<500), little (500–1000), somewhat (1000–1500), highly (1500–2500), and very highly (≥2500). These groups were categorized into ‘sparsely populated (<1000)’ and ‘densely populated (≥1000)’. Information on partner status was self-administered and categorized as ‘living alone’ and ‘living with partner’. Subjective pain was assessed using a subscale based on the Nottingham Health Profile [51]. Participants were questioned about experiencing constant pain and experiencing pain when changing position, sitting, or walking. Data on health-related factors, alcohol consumption, and cigarette smoking were accumulated during the medical interview. Participants were questioned about the presence of chronic nonspecific lung disease, cardiac disease, peripheral arterial disease, stroke, diabetes mellitus, rheumatoid arthritis, and malignancies. Additionally, participants could report a maximum of two other chronic diseases for which symptoms or treatment had been present for at least three months. Depressive symptoms were measured with the Centre for Epidemiologic studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [52]. BMI was calculated as weight (kilograms)/height (meters)2. Weight was measured using a calibrated bathroom scale, and height was measured using a stadiometer. In the absence of measured weight or height, self-reported values were obtained. To define alcohol use, a standard developed by the Netherlands Economic Institute (NEI) was used, which categorizes alcohol use into four groups (no use, moderate use, grey area, and excessive use), corrected for sex [53]. The ‘excessive use’ group was merged with the ‘grey area’ group, creating a combined category labelled as ‘above moderate use’. Based on questions about current (yes/no) and past smoking (yes/no), participants were categorized as never smoked, former smoker, or current smoker. Physical activity was measured using the validated LASA Physical Activity Questionnaire (LAPAQ), an interviewer-administered questionnaire that estimates the frequency and duration of participation in activities over the past 2 weeks [54]. All activities in the LAPAQ were assigned a MET score based on previously published MET score lists [55,56] and interviews with activity experts. From this, a total amount of MET hours per week was calculated.

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics (version 28, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). A total of 445 participants were excluded from the analyses due to the non-completion of the self-administered questionnaire (n = 422) or because of missing or invalid data on caffeine consumption (n = 23), resulting in a final analytical sample of 1256 participants. Sample characteristics of included and excluded eligible participants were quantified using descriptive statistics. Continuous variables were presented as means with standard deviations (SD) if normally distributed. If not normally distributed, variables were presented as medians with interquartile ranges (IQR). Categorical variables were presented as proportions. Linear regression analyses were performed to assess the association between caffeine consumption and sleep disturbances. Multinominal logistic regression analyses were performed to assess the association between caffeine consumption and short (<7 h) and long (>8 h) sleep duration (with recommended sleep duration (7–8 h) as the reference group), and logistic regression analyses were performed to assess the association between caffeine consumption and perceived sleep quality. In each model, ‘low caffeine consumption (≤173 mg/day)’ was set as the reference group. Analyses were also performed with a binary caffeine classification: no caffeine consumption (0 mg/day) and caffeine consumption (>0 mg/day), with ‘caffeine consumption (>0 mg/day)’ set as the reference group. Both crude and adjusted models were fitted to account for potential confounding variables. Potential confounders were adjusted for by adding them to the regression models. Effect modification by sex and age was tested by adding interaction terms to the crude regression models and evaluating statistical significance of the interaction term (p < 0.05). If effect modification was present, stratified results were presented; otherwise, variables were included as potential confounders. To preserve sample size and increase the precision of the estimates, all covariates with missing values were imputed using the multiple imputation procedure in SPSS Statistics. This was performed despite all confounders having less than 5% missing values. A total of five datasets were imputed. Variables included in the imputation procedure were age, sex, level of attained education, partner status, level of urbanization, number of chronic diseases, depressive symptoms, BMI, subjective pain, smoking status, alcohol use, and physical activity.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

A flowchart of the participants included in the analytical samples is shown in Figure 1. Compared to included participants, excluded eligible participants (n = 445) were somewhat older (mean age of 76 years versus 73 years), more often female (58% versus 53%), lower educated (45% versus 31% lower education), and had more chronic diseases (mean of 2.8 versus 2.2) (Table 1).
Sample characteristics of included participants (n = 1256) are presented in Table 1, stratified by sex and caffeine consumption (yes/no). The sample consisted of 587 (46.7%) males and 669 females. Seventeen percent of males and 26.3% of females had short sleep duration (<7 h/night), 25.7% of males and 43.4% of females experienced several to many sleep disturbances, and 12.8% of males and 22.5% of females reported poor perceived sleep quality. Compared to males, females had a lower mean caffeine intake (244 mg/day versus 286 mg/day in males), consumed less caffeine from coffee (median of 167 mg/day versus 220 mg/day in males) and more caffeine from tea (median of 50 mg/day versus 26 mg/day in males). The proportion of females who did not consume caffeine (9.1%) exceeded that of males (6.6%). Males and females who did not consume caffeine were on average older, lower educated, had more chronic diseases and depressive symptoms, and were physically less active.

3.2. Association between Caffeine Consumption and Sleep Health Outcomes

Statistically significant interactions were observed between sex and caffeine consumption in the association with sleep disturbances (p ≤ 0.002), short sleep duration (p ≤ 0.001), and perceived sleep quality (p = 0.005). No interactions were found for age. In both males and females, there was no association between the categories of caffeine consumption and poorer sleep health. After adjusting for confounders, females who did not consume caffeine reported more sleep disturbances (β = 0.64 [95%CI 0.13; 1.15]) compared to females who did consume caffeine (Table 2). In males, the direction of the association between caffeine consumption and sleep disturbances was similar to that in females but did not reach statistical significance. Females who did not consume caffeine had significantly higher odds of short sleep duration (OR = 2.26 [95% CI 1.22; 4.20]) compared to those who did consume caffeine (Table 3). No association was found between caffeine consumption and sleep duration in males. In both sexes, there was no statistically significant association between caffeine consumption and long sleep duration (Table 3), and no statistically significant association between caffeine consumption and perceived sleep quality (Table 4).

4. Discussion

Interaction between caffeine consumption and sex was statistically significant for each sleep outcome. Older females who did not consume caffeine reported more sleep disturbances and had greater odds of short sleep duration (<7 h/day) compared to those who did consume caffeine, whereas no associations were observed in males. In both sexes, after adjustment for confounders, there was no association between caffeine consumption and perceived sleep quality. Overall, a higher caffeine consumption was not associated with a poorer sleep health.
Although caffeine consumption has been linked to sleep disturbances, shorter sleep duration, and poorer perceived sleep quality in healthy adults [34], we did not observe this association in a representative sample of community-dwelling older males and females in The Netherlands. Our results are consistent with a previous observational study in middle-aged and older adults that found no association between self-reported caffeine consumption and sleep duration measured using actigraphy [38]. Most other studies evaluated caffeine intake from coffee consumption only [36,37,39], which may have led to a misclassification of subjects and an underestimation of associations. A large observational study (n = 8091) of adults aged 55 to 101 years in Europe did find an association between coffee consumption (i.e., drinking more than six cups of coffee per day) and shorter sleep duration [36]. Consumption of more than six cups of coffee per day corresponds to the ‘high’ to ‘very high’ caffeine consumption categories in the present study. Similarly, a cross-sectional study of 428 older Nigerian females found that habitual coffee consumption (yes versus no coffee) was associated with shorter sleep duration [37], contradicting our findings in older females. Furthermore, our observation that older females who abstained from caffeine consumption reported more sleep disturbances and shorter sleep duration than those who did consume caffeine is in line with the results of two observational studies of hospitalized older adults (59.3% female) [39] and community-dwelling older adults (43.1% female) [40]. In the latter study, caffeine consumption was measured using a questionnaire that assessed the intake of several caffeinated substances, and plasma caffeine concentrations were also measured in the late afternoon.
The results of our observational study contradict results from controlled experiments in laboratory settings, wherein the administration of ≥200 mg of caffeine resulted in prolonged sleep latency (difficulties falling asleep), reduced sleep efficiency (ratio of total sleep time to time in bed), and shorter sleep duration in young and middle-aged adults [35,41,42]. A key difference between controlled laboratory experiments and the present study is that caffeine exposure in the present study represents the participants’ habitual caffeine consumption. It has been hypothesized that habitual caffeine consumption may lead to the development of tolerance to caffeine, potentially reducing its effects on sleep in real-life circumstances [57,58]. A randomized experiment by Hindmarch et al. [59], for example, found that caffeine had a greater adverse effect on sleep health in adults with a lower habitual caffeine consumption compared to adults with a higher habitual caffeine consumption.
An alternative explanation could be that people with lower habitual caffeine consumption and people who abstain from caffeine consumption are more sensitive to the effects of caffeine and therefore consciously limit their caffeine consumption or adjust the timing of their consumption. This may explain the absence of an association between categories of caffeine consumption and sleep health in the present study. Indeed, there are significant interpersonal differences in caffeine sensitivity, which can be partially attributed to genetic factors. Studies have shown that certain genes—such as those involved in the metabolism of caffeine—may determine an individual’s physiological response to the stimulant as well as their consumption patterns [60,61]. The timing of caffeine consumption may impact the magnitude of its effect on sleep as the potential for sleep disturbance increases when caffeine is consumed closer to habitual bedtime [59,62]. Our observation that female older adults who abstained from caffeine consumption reported more sleep disturbances and shorter sleep duration compared to those who did consume caffeine may be explained by the possibility that females who experience sleep disturbances are aware of the stimulatory effects of caffeine and therefore refrain from its consumption. This presumed reverse causation and the observed sex differences therein are supported by evidence that females more often limit caffeinated coffee consumption than males or do so because they experience sleep disturbances [63]. Yet, our study suggests that sleep disturbances remain present despite refraining from caffeine consumption.
The present study has several strengths, including the use of a large, nationally representative sample of community-dwelling older adults, increasing the generalizability of the results. Additionally, detailed information on coffee and tea consumption was gathered to assess caffeine consumption (i.e., type of coffee/tea and cup volume). Furthermore, relevant potential confounding variables were accounted for in the analyses, although residual confounding cannot be fully disregarded. In addition to these strengths, this study has some limitations that need to be taken into account when interpreting the results. One limitation is that the timing of caffeine consumption was not taken into account. Another limitation is the reliance on self-reported measures of caffeine consumption and sleep health, which increases the likelihood of misclassification and bias due to subjectivity. Disparities in subjective and objective measurements of sleep have frequently been described. Observational evidence indicates that subjective report of habitual sleep duration may be biased by systematic overreporting, a consistent tendency of individuals to report longer sleep duration [64]. Discrepancies between subjective and objective findings may also result from discreet abnormalities in sleep quality that are difficult for individuals to notice but can be detected through the use of objective measurement tools such as polysomnography or actigraphy. Additionally, it has been suggested that individuals adjust their perception of what constitutes a good sleep as they age [65,66], making older adults generally more satisfied with their sleep, despite apparent disruptions in objective sleep quality [67,68,69]. Overall, the use of subjective measurements could potentially have led to random misclassification, resulting in an underestimation of the associations in the expected direction. Furthermore, the use of retrospective questions to assess coffee and tea consumption induces a potential for recall bias, especially given the higher prevalence of age-related cognitive and memory decline in older adults [70]. However, it is expected that coffee and tea consumption are well-recalled as habitual or frequently consumed foods are more accurately recalled than foods consumed less frequently or without a pattern [71,72]. Another limitation is the method used to calculate caffeine consumption from coffee and tea. Although a distinction was made between caffeinated and decaffeinated coffee, as well as the different types of tea, no information was available on the type of caffeinated coffee and tea, including the method of preparation, brand, or strength of the coffee or tea brew, which may determine the actual caffeine content [26]. Additionally, caffeine consumption was estimated based on coffee and tea consumption only, not taking into account caffeine intake through other dietary sources. However, this is unlikely to have led to a significant underestimation of caffeine consumption as coffee and tea are the primary sources of caffeine for adults in The Netherlands [27]. Lastly, this study is limited by the absence of a validated measurement instrument to assess sleep health. However, validated instruments such as the PSQI [73] and the Insomnia Severity Index [74] contain similar questions to assess sleep health; thus, we do not expect the results to be different.
The results of this study and previous research provide conflicting evidence. Therefore, the relationship between caffeine consumption and sleep health in older adults remains unclear. Given the high prevalence of sleep disturbances in older adults and the debilitating consequences associated with poor sleep health, further research is warranted. Epidemiological studies could help to better understand the relationship between caffeine and sleep in real-life settings. Such studies could consider the use of diaries to collect information on caffeine consumption and sleep health and may benefit from investigating the timing of caffeine consumption and the motivations behind older adults’ decisions to limit or abstain from caffeine consumption as this may confirm the potential reverse causation in older females. In addition, studies should consider the potential sex differences in the effects of caffeine on sleep as well as sex differences in caffeine-related behaviours.

5. Conclusions

Community-dwelling older females who did not consume caffeine reported more sleep disturbances and had higher odds of short sleep duration (<7 h/day) compared to females who did consume caffeine. In older males, no association was found between caffeine consumption and sleep health. The association found in older females may reflect reverse causation, suggesting that females may have different motivations for discontinuing caffeine use than males. In both sexes, there was no association between higher caffeine consumption and sleep health.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the design of the study and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. M.v.d.L. performed the statistical analysis and drafted the original paper. All authors approved the final version of the article, including the authorship list. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

All included participants provided written informed consent. The LASA study protocol was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and received approval from the medical ethics committee of the VU University Medical Centre (IRB numbers: 92/138, 2002/141, 2012/361, and 2016.301).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy restrictions.

Acknowledgments

We thank all LASA participants for their valued contributions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  1. Luyster, F.S.; Strollo, P.J., Jr.; Zee, P.C.; Walsh, J.K. Boards of Directors of the American Academy of Sleep M, the Sleep Research S. Sleep: A health imperative. Sleep 2012, 35, 727–734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Watson, N.F.; Badr, M.S.; Belenky, G.; Bliwise, D.L.; Buxton, O.M.; Buysse, D.; Dinges, D.F.; Gangwisch, J.; Grandner, M.A.; Kushida, C. Joint Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society on the Recommended Amount of Sleep for a Healthy Adult: Methodology and Discussion. Sleep 2015, 38, 1161–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Foley, D.; Ancoli-Israel, S.; Britz, P.; Walsh, J. Sleep disturbances and chronic disease in older adults: Results of the 2003 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Survey. J. Psychosom. Res. 2004, 56, 497–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Gulia, K.K.; Kumar, V.M. Sleep disorders in the elderly: A growing challenge. Psychogeriatrics 2018, 18, 155–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Ohayon, M.M. Epidemiology of insomnia: What we know and what we still need to learn. Sleep. Med. Rev. 2002, 6, 97–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Kocevska, D.; Lysen, T.S.; Dotinga, A.; Koopman-Verhoef, M.E.; Luijk, M.P.; Antypa, N.; Biermasz, N.; Blokstra, A.; Brug, J.; Burk, W.J.; et al. Sleep characteristics across the lifespan in 1.1 million people from the Netherlands, United Kingdom and United States: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 113–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Lee, J.E.; Ju, Y.J.; Park, E.-C.; Lee, S.Y. Effect of poor sleep quality on subjective cognitive decline (SCD) or SCD-related functional difficulties: Results from 220,000 nationwide general populations without dementia. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 260, 32–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Hokett, E.; Arunmozhi, A.; Campbell, J.; Verhaeghen, P.; Duarte, A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of individual differences in naturalistic sleep quality and episodic memory performance in young and older adults. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021, 127, 675–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Li, M.; Wang, N.; Dupre, M.E. Association between the self-reported duration and quality of sleep and cognitive function among middle-aged and older adults in China. J. Affect. Disord. 2022, 304, 20–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Robbins, R.; Quan, S.F.; Weaver, M.D.; Bormes, G.; Barger, L.K.; Czeisler, C.A. Examining sleep deficiency and disturbance and their risk for incident dementia and all-cause mortality in older adults across 5 years in the United States. Aging 2021, 13, 3254–3268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Casagrande, M.; Forte, G.; Favieri, F.; Corbo, I. Sleep Quality and Aging: A Systematic Review on Healthy Older People, Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 8457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Shi, L.; Chen, S.J.; Ma, M.Y.; Bao, Y.P.; Han, Y.; Wang, Y.M.; Shi, J.; Vitiello, M.V.; Lu, L. Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2018, 40, 4–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Sabia, S.; Fayosse, A.; Dumurgier, J.; van Hees, V.T.; Paquet, C.; Sommerlad, A.; Kivimäki, M.; Dugravot, A.; Singh-Manoux, A. Association of sleep duration in middle and old age with incidence of dementia. Nat. Commun. 2021, 12, 2289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Denison, H.J.; Jameson, K.A.; Sayer, A.A.; Patel, H.P.; Edwards, M.H.; Arora, T.; Dennison, E.M.; Cooper, C.; Baird, J. Poor sleep quality and physical performance in older adults. Sleep Health 2021, 7, 205–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Sun, X.; Ma, T.; Yao, S.; Chen, Z.; Xu, W.-D.; Jiang, X.-Y.; Wang, X.-F. Associations of sleep quality and sleep duration with frailty and pre-frailty in an elderly population Rugao longevity and ageing study. BMC Geriatr. 2020, 20, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Wai, J.L.; Yu, D.S. The relationship between sleep-wake disturbances and frailty among older adults: A systematic review. J. Adv. Nurs. 2020, 76, 96–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Chien, M.Y.; Chen, H.C. Poor sleep quality is independently associated with physical disability in older adults. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2015, 11, 225–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Sagayadevan, V.; Abdin, E.; Binte Shafie, S.; Jeyagurunathan, A.; Sambasivam, R.; Zhang, Y.; Picco, L.; Vaingankar, J.; Chong, S.A.; Subramaniam, M. Prevalence and correlates of sleep problems among elderly Singaporeans. Psychogeriatrics 2017, 17, 43–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lo, C.M.; Lee, P.H. Prevalence and impacts of poor sleep on quality of life and associated factors of good sleepers in a sample of older Chinese adults. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 2012, 10, 72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. da Silva, A.A.; de Mello, R.G.; Schaan, C.W.; Fuchs, F.D.; Redline, S.; Fuchs, S.C. Sleep duration and mortality in the elderly: A systematic review with meta-analysis. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e008119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Zawisza, K.; Tobiasz–Adamczyk, B.; Gałaś, A.; Brzyska, M. Sleep duration and mortality among older adults in a 22-year follow-up study: An analysis of possible effect modifiers. Eur. J. Ageing 2014, 12, 119–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Hublin, C.; Partinen, M.; Koskenvuo, M.J.; Kaprio, J.A. Sleep and mortality: A population-based 22-year follow-up study. Sleep 2007, 30, 1245–1253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Pappas, J.A.; Miner, B. Sleep Deficiency in the Elderly. Clin. Chest Med. 2022, 43, 273–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Gupta, C.; Irwin, C.; Vincent, G.; Khaleesi, S. P047 Sleep and diet in older adults: What do we know and what do we need to find out? Sleep Adv. 2021, 2 (Suppl. S1), A36–A37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Reyes, C.M.; Cornelis, M.C. Caffeine in the Diet: Country-Level Consumption and Guidelines. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. EFSA Panel on Dietetic Products, Nutrition and Allergies (NDA). Scientific Opinion on the safety of caffeine. EFSA J. 2015, 13, 4102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Breedveld, B.C.; Peters, J.A.C.; Netherlands Nutrition Centre. Factsheet Caffeine. Available online: www.voedingscentrum.nl (accessed on 23 November 2023).
  28. Tsujimoto, T.; Kajio, H.; Sugiyama, T. Association between Caffeine Intake and All-Cause and Cause-Specific Mortality: A Population-Based Prospective Cohort Study. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2017, 92, 1190–1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Grosso, G.; Godos, J.; Galvano, F.; Giovannucci, E.L. Coffee, Caffeine, and Health Outcomes: An Umbrella Review. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2017, 37, 131–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ferre, S. An update on the mechanisms of the psychostimulant effects of caffeine. J. Neurochem. 2008, 105, 1067–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Fisone, G.; Borgkvist, A.; Usiello, A. Caffeine as a psychomotor stimulant: Mechanism of action. Cell Mol. Life Sci. 2004, 61, 857–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Landolt, H.P. Sleep homeostasis: A role for adenosine in humans? Biochem. Pharmacol. 2008, 75, 2070–2079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Shilo, L.; Sabbah, H.; Hadari, R.; Kovatz, S.; Weinberg, U.; Dolev, S.; Dagan, Y.; Shenkman, L. The effects of coffee consumption on sleep and melatonin secretion. Sleep Med. 2002, 3, 271–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Clark, I.; Landolt, H.P. Coffee, caffeine, and sleep: A systematic review of epidemiological studies and randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med. Rev. 2017, 31, 70–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Gardiner, C.; Weakley, J.; Burke, L.M.; Roach, G.D.; Sargent, C.; Maniar, N.; Townshend, A.; Halson, S.L. The effect of caffeine on subsequent sleep: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2023, 69, 101764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Ohayon, M.M. Interactions between sleep normative data and sociocultural characteristics in the elderly. J. Psychosom. Res. 2004, 56, 479–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Fawale, M.B.; Ismaila, I.A.; Mustapha, A.F.; Komolafe, M.A.; Ibigbami, O. Correlates of sleep quality and sleep duration in a sample of urban-dwelling elderly Nigerian women. Sleep Health 2017, 3, 257–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hu, Y.; Stephenson, K.; Klare, D. The dynamic relationship between daily caffeine intake and sleep duration in middle-aged and older adults. J. Sleep Res. 2020, 29, e12996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Park, M.J.; Kim, K.H. What affects the subjective sleep quality of hospitalized elderly patients? Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2017, 17, 471–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Curless, R.; French, J.M.; James, O.F.W.; Wynne, H.A. Is Caffeine a Factor in Subjective Insomnia of Elderly People? Age Ageing 1993, 22, 41–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Robillard, R.; Bouchard, M.; Cartier, A.; Nicolau, L.; Carrier, J. Sleep is more sensitive to high doses of caffeine in the middle years of life. J. Psychopharmacol. 2015, 29, 688–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Carrier, J.; Paquet, J.; Fernandez-Bolanos, M.; Girouard, L.; Roy, J.; Selmaoui, B.; Filipini, D. Effects of caffeine on daytime recovery sleep: A double challenge to the sleep-wake cycle in aging. Sleep Med. 2009, 10, 1016–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Frozi, J.; de Carvalho, H.W.; Ottoni, G.L.; Cunha, R.A.; Lara, D.R. Distinct sensitivity to caffeine-induced insomnia related to age. J. Psychopharmacol. 2018, 32, 89–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Gahagan, J.; Gray, K.; Whynacht, A. Sex and gender matter in health research: Addressing health inequities in health research reporting. Int. J. Equity Health 2015, 14, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Day, S.; Mason, R.; Lagosky, S.; Rochon, P.A. Integrating and evaluating sex and gender in health research. Health Res. Policy Syst. 2016, 14, 75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Huisman, M.; Poppelaars, J.; van der Horst, M.; Beekman, A.T.; Brug, J.; van Tilburg, T.G.; Deeg, D.J. Cohort Profile: The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2011, 40, 868–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Hoogendijk, E.O.; Deeg, D.J.H.; de Breij, S.; Klokgieters, S.S.; Kok, A.A.L.; Stringa, N.; Timmermans, E.J.; van Schoor, N.M.; van Zutphen, E.M.; van der Horst, M.; et al. The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam: Cohort update 2019 and additional data collections. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2020, 35, 61–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Hoogendijk, E.O.; Deeg, D.J.; Poppelaars, J.; van der Horst, M.; Broese van Groenou, M.I.; Comijs, H.C.; Pasman, H.R.; van Schoor, N.M.; Suanet, B.; Thomése, F.; et al. The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam: Cohort update 2016 and major findings. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2016, 31, 927–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Hirshkowitz, M.; Whiton, K.; Albert, S.M.; Alessi, C.; Bruni, O.; DonCarlos, L.; Hazen, N.; Herman, J.; Hillard, P.J.; Katz, E.S.; et al. National Sleep Foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations: Final report. Sleep Health 2015, 1, 233–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Den Dulk, C.J.; Van De Stadt, H.; Vliegen, J.M. A new measure for degree of urbanization: The address density of the surrounding area. Maandstat. Bevolk. 1992, 40, 14–27. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hunt, S.M.; McEwen, J.; McKenna, S.P. Measuring health status: A new tool for clinicians and epidemiologists. J. R. Coll. Gen. Pract. 1985, 35, 185–188. [Google Scholar]
  52. Radloff, L.S. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Reinhard, O.P.M.; Rood-Bakker, D.S. Alcoholgebruik in Beeld. Standaardmeetlat; Nederlands Economisch Instituut: Rotterdam, The Netherlands, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  54. Stel, V.S.; Smit, J.H.; Pluijm, S.M.; Visser, M.; Deeg, D.J.; Lips, P. Comparison of the LASA Physical Activity Questionnaire with a 7-day diary and pedometer. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2004, 57, 252–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Ainsworth, B.E.; Haskell, W.L.; Leon, A.S.; Jacobs, D.R., Jr.; Montoye, H.J.; Sallis, J.F.; Paffenbarger, R.S., Jr. Compendium of physical activities: Classification of energy costs of human physical activities. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1993, 25, 71–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Caspersen, C.J.; Bloemberg, B.P.; Saris, W.H.; Merritt, R.K.; Kromhout, D. The prevalence of selected physical activities and their relation with coronary heart disease risk factors in elderly men: The Zutphen Study, 1985. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1991, 133, 1078–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Dager, S.R.; Layton, M.E.; Strauss, W.; Richards, T.L.; Heide, A.; Friedman, S.D.; Artru, A.A.; Hayes, C.E.; Posse, S. Human brain metabolic response to caffeine and the effects of tolerance. Am. J. Psychiatry 1999, 156, 229–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Lovallo, W.R.; Whitsett, T.L.; Al’absi, M.; Sung, B.H.; Vincent, A.S.; Wilson, M.F. Caffeine stimulation of cortisol secretion across the waking hours in relation to caffeine intake levels. Psychosom. Med. 2005, 67, 734–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hindmarch, I.; Rigney, U.; Stanley, N.; Quinlan, P.; Rycroft, J.; Lane, J. A naturalistic investigation of the effects of day-long consumption of tea, coffee and water on alertness, sleep onset and sleep quality. Psychopharmacology 2000, 149, 203–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Yang, A.; Palmer, A.A.; de Wit, H. Genetics of caffeine consumption and responses to caffeine. Psychopharmacology 2010, 211, 245–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Retey, J.V.; Adam, M.; Khatami, R.; Luhmann, U.F.; Jung, H.H.; Berger, W.; Landolt, H.P. A genetic variation in the adenosine A2A receptor gene (ADORA2A) contributes to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 2007, 81, 692–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Drake, C.; Roehrs, T.; Shambroom, J.; Roth, T. Caffeine effects on sleep taken 0, 3, or 6 hours before going to bed. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2013, 9, 1195–1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Soroko, S.; Chang, J.; Barrett-Connor, E. Reasons for changing caffeinated coffee consumption: The Rancho Bernardo Study. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 1996, 15, 97–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Lauderdale, D.S.; Knutson, K.L.; Yan, L.L.; Liu, K.; Rathouz, P.J. Self-Reported and Measured Sleep Duration: How Similar Are They? Epidemiology 2008, 19, 838–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Vitiello, M.V.; Larsen, L.H.; Moe, K.E. Age-related sleep change: Gender and estrogen effects on the subjective-objective sleep quality relationships of healthy, noncomplaining older men and women. J. Psychosom. Res. 2004, 56, 503–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Danker-Hopfe, H.; Hornung, O.; Regen, F.; Hansen, M.L.; Albrecht, N.; Heuser, I. Subjective sleep quality in noncomplaining elderly subjects: Results of a follow-up study. Anthr. Anz. 2006, 64, 369–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Zilli, I.; Ficca, G.; Salzarulo, P. Factors involved in sleep satisfaction in the elderly. Sleep Med. 2009, 10, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Buysse, D.J.; Reynolds, C.F.; 3rd Monk, T.H.; Hoch, C.C.; Yeager, A.L.; Kupfer, D.J. Quantification of subjective sleep quality in healthy elderly men and women using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). Sleep 1991, 14, 331–338. [Google Scholar]
  69. Åkerstedt, T.; Schwarz, J.; Gruber, G.; Lindberg, E.; Theorell-Haglöw, J. The relation between polysomnography and subjective sleep and its dependence on age—Poor sleep may become good sleep. J. Sleep Res. 2016, 25, 565–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Schönknecht, P.; Pantel, J.; Kruse, A.; Schröder, J. Prevalence and natural course of aging-associated cognitive decline in a population-based sample of young-old subjects. Am. J. Psychiatry 2005, 162, 2071–2077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Ambrosini, G.L.; van Roosbroeck, S.A.; Mackerras, D.; Fritschi, L.; de Klerk, N.H.; Musk, A.W. The reliability of ten-year dietary recall: Implications for cancer research. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 2663–2668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Krall, E.A.; Dwyer, J.T.; Ann Coleman, K. Factors influencing accuracy of dietary recall. Nutr. Res. 1988, 8, 829–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Buysse, D.J.; Reynolds, C.F.; 3rd Monk, T.H.; Berman, S.R.; Kupfer, D.J. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989, 28, 193–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Bastien, C.H.; Vallières, A.; Morin, C.M. Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep. Med. 2001, 2, 297–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flowchart of participants included in the analytic samples for the association between caffeine consumption from coffee and tea and sleep health in older adults from the Longitudinal Ageing Study Amsterdam (LASA).
Figure 1. Flowchart of participants included in the analytic samples for the association between caffeine consumption from coffee and tea and sleep health in older adults from the Longitudinal Ageing Study Amsterdam (LASA).
Nutrients 16 00131 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of the excluded and analytical sample (n = 1256), stratified by sex and by caffeine consumption (mg/day).
Table 1. Characteristics of the excluded and analytical sample (n = 1256), stratified by sex and by caffeine consumption (mg/day).
MalesFemales
CharacteristicsExcluded Cases
(n = 455)
Analytical Sample
(n = 1256)
Total
(n = 587)
No Caffeine
(n = 39)
Caffeine
(n = 548)
Total
(n = 669)
No Caffeine
(n = 61)
Caffeine
(n = 608)
Caffeine consumption
Total caffeine (mg/day)
 Mean ± SD-263 ± 167286 ± 175-306 ± 164244 ± 156-268 ± 143
Categories of caffeine, N (%) a
 None-100 (8.0)39 (6.6)39 (100)-61 (9.1)61 (100)-
 Low-289 (23.0)120 (20.4)-120 (21.9)169 (25.3)-169 (27.8)
 Moderate-289 (23.0)126 (21.5)-126 (23.0)163 (24.4)-163 (26.8)
 High-300 (23.9)153 (26.1)-152 (27.9)147 (22.0)-147 (24.2)
 Very high-278 (22.1)149 (25.4)-149 (27.2)129 (19.3)-129 (21.2)
Caffeine from coffee (mg/day) b
 Median (IQR)-220 (194) 220 (187)-223 (220)167 (220)-200 (182)
Caffeine from tea (mg/day) c
 Median (IQR)-36 (102)26 (75)-33.6 (78)50 (116)-62 (123)
Demographics
Age in yearsn = 445
 Mean ± SD76.3 ± 9.972.8 ± 7.572.5 ± 7.377.3 ± 10.272.2 ± 6.973.1 ± 7.775.9 ± 8.872.8 ± 7.5
Level of attained education, N (%)n = 445
 Low199 (44.7)386 (30.7)159 (27.1)13 (33.3)146 (26.6)227 (33.9)28 (45.9)199 (32.7)
 Intermediate162 (36.4)487 (38.8)202 (34.4)16 (41.0)186 (33.9)285 (42.6)23 (37.7)262 (43.1)
 High84 (18.9)383 (30.5)226 (38.5)10 (25.6)216 (39.4)157 (23.5)10 (16.4)147 (24.2)
Partner status, N (%)n = 443
 Living alone 212 (47.9)408 (32.5) 125 (21.3)8 (20.5)117 (21.4)283 (42.3)29 (47.5)254 (41.8)
 Living with partner231 (52.1)848 (67.5)462 (78.7)31 (79.5)431 (78.6)386 (57.7)32 (52.5)354 (58.2)
Level of urbanization, N (%) *n = 441
 Sparsely populated (<1000)173 (39.2)520 (41.5)240 (41.0)22 (56.4)218 (39.9)280 (41.9)29 (47.5)251 (41.4)
 Densely populated (≥1000)268 (60.8)733 (58.5)345 (59.0)17 (43.6)328 (60.1)388 (58.1)32 (52.5)356 (58.6)
Health related factors
Number of chronic diseases dn = 137
 Mean ± SD2.8 ± 1.72.2 ± 1.42.1 ± 1.42.4 ± 1.62.1 ± 1.42.3 ± 1.42.5 ± 1.52.3 ± 1.4
Depressive symptoms (CES-D) *n = 137
 Median (IQR)10 (12)5.0 (8.0)5.0 (7.0)5.0 (6.0)4.0 (7.0)6.0 (8.0)8.0 (10)6.0 (8.0)
Body mass index (kg/m2) *n = 103
 Mean ± SD27.2 ± 4.727.3 ± 4.727.4 ± 4.227.0 ± 5.227.5 ± 4.127.2 ± 5.228.3 ± 5.827.1 ± 5.1
Subjective pain *
 Mean ± SD-5.8 ± 1.45.6 ± 1.25.8 ± 1.25.6 ± 1.26.0 ± 1.66.4 ± 1.96.0 ± 1.5
Behavioural factors
Smoking status, N (%) *n = 107
 Never smoked26 (24.3)304 (24.9)116 (20.3)7 (18.4)109 (20.5)188 (28.9)20 (33.3)168 (28.5)
 Former smoker66 (61.7)812 (66.5)410 (71.8)30 (78.9)380 (71.3)402 (61.8)35 (58.3)367 (62.6)
 Current smoker15 (14.0)105 (8.6)45 (7.9)1 (2.6)44 (8.3)60 (9.2)5 (8.3)55 (9.3)
Alcohol use, N (%) *n = 107
 No use35 (32.7)172 (14.1)56 (9.8)7 (18.4)49 (9.2)116 (17.8)19 (31.7)97 (16.4)
 Moderate use67 (62.6)925 (75.8)467 (81.8)31 (81.6)436 (81.8)458 (70.5)38 (63.3)420 (71.2)
 Above moderate use5 (4.7)124 (10.2)48 (8.4)-48 (9.0)76 (11.7)3 (5.0)73 (12.4)
Physical activity (MET-hrs./wk.) *n = 136
 Median (IQR)35.7 (49.1)52.0 (45.3)45.2 (40.6)37.3 (43.2)45.6 (40.2)58.6 (45.2) 51.0 (41.1)59.3 (45.5)
Sleep parameters
Sleep duration(h/day), N (%) *
 Mean ± SD-7.4 ± 1.17.5 ± 1.17.8 ± 1.07.5 ± 1.17.3 ± 1.16.9 ± 1.37.3 ± 1.1
 Recommended (7–8 h) -795 (64.2)402 (69.2)27 (71.1)375 (69.1)393 (59.8)24 (42.9)369 (61.4)
 Short (< 7 h)-272 (22.0)99 (17.0)5 (13.2)94 (17.3)173 (26.3)25 (44.6)148 (24.6)
 Long (>8 h)-171 (13.8)80 (13.8)6 (15.8)74 (13.6)91 (13.9) 7 (12.5) 84 (14.0)
Sleep disturbances, N (%) e *
 Median (IQR)-6.0 (3.0)5.0 (3.0)6.0 (2.0)5.0 (3.0)6.0 (3.0)7.0 (3.0)6.0 (3.0)
 No disturbances (3)-154 (12.4)101 (17.4)2 (5.1)99 (18.3)53 (8.0)2 (3.3)51 (8.4)
 Some disturbances (4–6)-652 (52.4)329 (56.8)25 (64.1)304 (56.3)323 (48.6)23 (37.7)300 (49.7)
 Several disturbances (7–9)-376 (30.2)131 (22.6)12 (30.8)119 (22.0) 245 (36.8)29 (47.5)216 (35.8)
 Many disturbances (10–12)-62 (5.0)18 (3.1)-18 (3.3)44 (6.6)7 (11.5) 37 (6.1)
Perceived sleep quality, N (%) *
 Good-1028 (82.0)511 (87.2)36 (92.3)475 (86.8)517 (77.5)44 (72.1)473 (78.1)
 Poor-225 (18.0)75 (12.8)3 (7.7)72 (13.2)150 (22.5)17 (27.9)133 (21.9)
n = number of participants, SD = standard deviation, IQR = interquartile range. Sample characteristics are based on non-imputed data. Variables with missing data (<5%) are denoted with an asterisk (*). a None: 0 mg/day, low: ≤ 173 mg/day, moderate: 174–272 mg/day, high: 273–367 mg/day, and very high: >367 mg/day, b n = 1223, c n = 1128, d. Including nonspecific lung disease, cardiac disease, peripheral arterial disease, stroke, diabetes mellitus, rheumatoid arthritis, and malignancies as well as up to two other self-reported chronic diseases, e Measured on a 3 to 12 scale based on three questions that assessed the frequency of experiencing difficulties with sleep latency, continuity, and early morning awakening. Higher scores indicate more sleep disturbances.
Table 2. Crude and adjusted linear regression models for the association between caffeine consumption (mg/day) and sleep disturbances (measured on a 3–12 scale b), stratified by sex (n = 1244).
Table 2. Crude and adjusted linear regression models for the association between caffeine consumption (mg/day) and sleep disturbances (measured on a 3–12 scale b), stratified by sex (n = 1244).
Males (n = 579)Females (n = 665)
β 95% CIp-Valueβ95% CIp-Value
Categories of caffeine a
 Crude model
  None0.12−0.58–0.820.7370.800.20–1.400.009
  Low (ref.)------
  Moderate−0.27−0.76–0.210.2670.06−0.38–0.510.776
  High−0.54−1.00–−0.070.024−0.23−0.68–0.230.326
  Very high−0.21−0.68–0.260.382−0.14−0.61–0.330.561
 Adjusted model c
  None0.23−0.44–0.900.5000.610.04–1.170.035
  Low (ref.)------
  Moderate−0.17−0.63–0.290.4670.08−0.34–0.500.713
  High−0.35−0.80–0.090.122−0.12−0.55–0.310.572
  Very high−0.12−0.58–0.330.597−0.12−0.57–0.320.587
Caffeine vs. no caffeine
 Crude model
  Caffeine (ref.)------
  No caffeine0.39−0.24–1.020.2220.870.33–1.410.002
 Adjusted model c
  Caffeine (ref.)------
  No caffeine0.39−0.21–1.000.2050.640.13–1.150.014
β = beta coefficient, CI = confidence interval, ref. = reference category. a None: 0 mg/day, low: ≤173 mg/day, moderate: 174–272 mg/day, high: 273–367 mg/day, and very high: >367 mg/day, b Higher scores indicate more sleep disturbances, c Adjusted for age, level of attained education, partner status, level of urbanization, number of chronic diseases, depressive symptoms (CES-D), BMI (kg/m2), subjective pain, smoking status, alcohol use, and physical activity (MET-hrs./wk.).
Table 3. Crude and adjusted multinomial logistic regression models for the association between caffeine consumption (mg/day) and sleep duration, stratified by sex (n = 1238).
Table 3. Crude and adjusted multinomial logistic regression models for the association between caffeine consumption (mg/day) and sleep duration, stratified by sex (n = 1238).
Males (n = 581)Females (n = 657)
OR b95% CIp-ValueOR95% CIp-Value
Short sleep duration (<7 h ) a
Categories of caffeine c
 Crude model
  None0.780.27–2.290.6512.451.26–4.750.008
  Low (ref.)------
  Moderate0.910.45–1.840.7910.930.55–1.550.767
  High1.200.63–2.290.587 1.00 0.59–1.690.997
  Very high1.080.56–2.080.812 0.830.48–1.450.511
 Adjusted model d
  None0.930.30–2.870.9032.181.09–4.370.028
  Low (ref.)------
  Moderate0.940.45–1.960.8670.970.56–1.660.899
  High1.240.62–2.470.5371.100.63–1.910.744
  Very high1.000.49–2.020.9890.800.45–1.440.454
Caffeine vs. no caffeine
 Crude model
  Caffeine (ref.)------
  No caffeine0.740.28–1.970.5452.601.44–4.690.002
 Adjusted model d
  Caffeine (ref.)------
  No caffeine0.900.32–2.480.8332.261.22–4.200.010
Long sleep duration (>8 h) a
Categories of caffeine c
 Crude model
  None0.940.34–2.590.8981.280.49–3.330.612
  Low (ref.)------
  Moderate0.810.40–1.670.5741.070.56–2.010.847
  High0.910.46–1.790.7811.060.55–2.050.862
  Very high0.640.31–1.330.2310.860.43–1.730.666
 Adjusted model d
  None0.730.24–2.170.5641.190.44–3.180.735
  Low (ref.)------
  Moderate0.890.42–1.900.7611.080.55–2.100.827
  High1.190.57–2.460.6461.170.59–2.340.657
  Very high0.820.38–1.780.6220.970.46–2.020.925
Caffeine vs. no caffeine
 Crude model
  Caffeine (ref.)------
  No caffeine1.130.45–2.820.8001.280.53–3.070.579
 Adjusted model d
  Caffeine (ref.)---- --
  No caffeine0.750.28–2.030.5671.130.46–2.800.788
OR = odds ratio, CI = confidence interval, ref. = reference category. a The reference category is the recommended sleep duration (7–8 h/day). b An OR > 1 indicates higher odds of short or long sleep duration, compared to the recommended sleep duration. c None: 0 mg/day, low: ≤173 mg/day, moderate: 174–272 mg/day, high: 273–367 mg/day, and very high: >367 mg/day, d Adjusted for age, level of attained education, partner status, level of urbanization, number of chronic diseases, depressive symptoms (CES-D), BMI (kg/m2), subjective pain, smoking status, alcohol use, and physical activity (MET-hrs/wk.).
Table 4. Crude and adjusted logistic regression models for the association between caffeine consumption (mg/day) and perceived sleep quality, stratified by sex (n = 1253).
Table 4. Crude and adjusted logistic regression models for the association between caffeine consumption (mg/day) and perceived sleep quality, stratified by sex (n = 1253).
Males (n = 586)Females (n = 667)
OR a95% CIp-ValueOR95% CIp-Value
Categories of caffeine b
 Crude model
  None0.580.16–2.130.4151.420.73–2.770.308
  Low (ref.)------
  Moderate0.960.45–2.050.9051.090.65–1.830.758
  High0.820.39–1.730.5980.900.52–1.560.710
  Very high1.480.75–2.940.2631.160.67–2.000.595
 Adjusted model c
  None0.860.22–3.470.8371.250.60–2.580.556
  Low (ref.)------
  Moderate1.180.50–2.770.7111.040.59–1.820.903
  High1.030.45–2.400.9380.970.54–1.760.917
  Very high1.970.89–4.390.0951.110.61–2.020.738
Caffeine vs. no caffeine
 Crude model
  Caffeine (ref.)------
  No caffeine0.550.17–1.830.3301.370.76–2.480.293
 Adjusted model c
  Caffeine (ref.)------
  No caffeine0.670.19–2.410.5431.210.64–2.320.558
OR = odds ratio, CI = confidence interval, ref. = reference category. a An OR > 1 indicates higher odds of poor perceived sleep quality. b None: 0 mg/day, low: ≤173 mg/day, moderate: 174–272 mg/day, high: 273–367 mg/day, and very high: >367 mg/day, c Adjusted for age, level of attained education, partner status, level of urbanization, number of chronic diseases, depressive symptoms (CES-D), BMI (kg/m2), subjective pain, smoking status, alcohol use, and physical activity (MET-hrs/wk.).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

van der Linden, M.; Olthof, M.R.; Wijnhoven, H.A.H. The Association between Caffeine Consumption from Coffee and Tea and Sleep Health in Male and Female Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study. Nutrients 2024, 16, 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010131

AMA Style

van der Linden M, Olthof MR, Wijnhoven HAH. The Association between Caffeine Consumption from Coffee and Tea and Sleep Health in Male and Female Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study. Nutrients. 2024; 16(1):131. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010131

Chicago/Turabian Style

van der Linden, Mette, Margreet R. Olthof, and Hanneke A. H. Wijnhoven. 2024. "The Association between Caffeine Consumption from Coffee and Tea and Sleep Health in Male and Female Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study" Nutrients 16, no. 1: 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010131

APA Style

van der Linden, M., Olthof, M. R., & Wijnhoven, H. A. H. (2024). The Association between Caffeine Consumption from Coffee and Tea and Sleep Health in Male and Female Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study. Nutrients, 16(1), 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16010131

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop