3.2. The Influence of Demographic and Clinical Variables on Social Support
The ‘gender’ variable differentiated the need for support between the studied women (M = 15.16) and men (M = 13.36) at a statistically significant level (p ≤ 0.01). There were no statistically significant differences in the remaining scales of support.
The ‘education’ variable did not affect the need for support in the studied patients.
The ‘place of residence’ variable did not show statistically significant differences, with the exception that respondents from large cities (M = 15.73) searched for support more than those from small towns (M = 14.09). Respondents from small towns (M = 3.76), on the other hand, had higher satisfaction with the support received (M = 3.57) than those living in large cities.
The ‘attitude to work’ variable (full-time work, disability pension, retirement pension) did not affect the need for support in the surveyed patients.
Patients who have previously undergone surgery sought support more intensely (M = 15.20) than those who had not had previous surgery (M = 13.97). The difference was statistically significant. Seeking support is associated with adopting a specific attitude that allows meeting the need for support.
On the other hand, respondents who had not had surgery showed a greater need for emotional support (M = 13.97) than those who had had previous surgery (M = 2.91), and the difference was statistically significant. Emotional support is treated as a kind of support—that is, the need for care from others in order to build a sense of security during the recovery process.
The number of previous surgeries statistically significantly differentiated patients on the scale of satisfaction with the support received. Patients after their second surgery were more satisfied (M = 3.67) with the support received than those after their first surgery (M = 3.88). There were no statistically significant differences in the remaining scales of support.
The ‘type of treatment’ variable differentiated patients at a statistically significant level (p ≤ 0.05) on the scale of the support currently received. The need for emotional support was lower for patients treated with chemotherapy than for those who had other types of treatment. There were no statistically significant differences in the remaining scales of support.
The duration of treatment also differentiated patients at a statistically significant level in the scale of the perceived information support. This concerns the relationship between patients treated for 1–3 months (M = 13.40) and for 4–12 months (M = 14.80, p ≤ 0.01); for 1–3 months (M = 13.40) and 1–2 years (M = 14.34; p ≤ 0.05); and for 4–12 months (M = 14.80) and 2–5 years (M = 13.91; p ≤ 0.05).
In the scale of the need for support, statistically significant correlations occurred in patients treated for 1–3 months (M = 11.79) and 2–5 years (M = 10.74; p ≤ 0.05); for 4–12 months (M = 12.55) and 2–5 years (M = 10.74; p ≤ 0.001); and for 1–2 years (M = 12.59) and 2–5 years (10.74; p ≤ 0.01).
In the scale of seeking support, statistically significant correlations occurred in patients treated for 1–3 months (M = 14.12) and 4–12 months (M = 15.42; p ≤ 0.05); for 4–12 months (M = 15.42) and 2–5 years (M = 13.26; p ≤ 0.05); and for 1–2 years (M = 15.38) and 2–5 years (M = 13.26; p ≤ 0.05).
In the scale of satisfaction with the support received, there was a significant correlation between people treated for 4–12 months (M = 3.82) and 2–5 years (M = 3.84; p ≤ 0.05).
The ‘duration of treatment’ variable in all the scales discussed above differentiated patients treated for 4–12 months, whose sense of support was higher than that of patients treated for 2–5 years.
Our study showed that the longer a patient was treated, the lower their sense of support was.
3.3. Analysis of Determinants of Social Support
The data obtained in our study were subjected to statistical analysis, and correlations with individual components of social support were calculated. The variables that were statistically significant in explaining the variables of the BSSS questionnaire subscales or showed high dependence coefficients are presented below.
The study showed that perceived emotional support was conditioned by the variables of the POS (β = 0.30) and the ‘exclusion variable’ (β = −0.19) on the SPT.2 scale (
Table 4). They were important for explaining the ‘perceived emotional support’ variable. The positive correlation obtained between the POS and perceived emotional support indicates that people who had a positive view about themselves and the world, and experienced life satisfaction despite various difficult situations, noticed and perceived the provided emotional support to a greater extent. On the other hand, people with a tendency to a negative assessment of a situation and themselves, who had no prospects for the future, would barely be aware of emotional support from relatives or medics.
Regression analysis showed that people with no sense of social exclusion were open-minded and perceived the emotional support they received from others.
The anger scale (β = −0.12) with a negative correlation was qualified to explain the variable of perceived emotional support in the studied patients, but it was not statistically significant.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient showed that the variables were correlated at a medium level (R = 0.46). The variables of the POS and exclusion (SPT.2) indicate that perceived emotional support was an important area of social support, explaining 21% of the variance (R2 = 0.21). The remaining percentage was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation.
Perceived instrumental support was conditioned by the POS (β = 0.38). The positive correlation obtained between the scales indicates that patients who were characterised by positive self-esteem and attached importance to the positive aspects of life despite various difficult situations would perceive and appreciate more the instrumental support provided to them in the form of concrete help. On the other hand, patients who were prone to a negative assessment of a situation or themselves had difficulty noticing instrumental support given to them. The exclusion (β = −0.14) and anger (β = −0.10) scales were additionally included in the explanation of the ‘perceived instrumental support’ variable in the studied patients, showing a negative correlation, which was, however, not statistically significant.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the analysed variables were related to each other at a medium level (R = 0.49). The ‘POS’ variable indicates that perceived instrumental support was an important area of social support, explaining 24% of the variance (R2 = 0.24). The remaining percentage was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation.
Our study showed that the need for support was statistically significantly conditioned by the need for help (β = 0.28) and anger (β = −0.29). The studied patients treated for cancer who demonstrated the need for help were characterised by an increased need for support, unlike the patients who did not show such a need. The negative correlation between the variables (β = −0.29) indicates that patients who did not experience negative emotions, such as anger, could be characterised by an increased need for support during cancer treatment. Experiencing negative emotions blocked both the need for and openness to support (
Table 5).
The scales of loss (β = 0.15) and self-esteem (β = 0.11) were included in the explanation of the ‘need for support’ variable in the studied patients, showing a positive correlation, which was, however, not statistically significant.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the analysed variables were related to each other at a medium level (R = 0.33). The ‘need for help’ and ‘anger’ variables indicate that the need for support was an important area of social support, explaining 11% of the variance (R2 = 0.11). The remaining percentage was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation.
The results in
Table 6 show that seeking support was conditioned by the need for help (β = 0.43), anger (β = −0.27) and extroversion (β = 0.19). The surveyed patients who signalled the need for help could be characterised by a higher commitment to seeking support than those who said that they did not feel such a need (
Table 6).
People who did not experience anger and irritation sought support more intensely during treatment than those experiencing negative emotions during this period (a negative correlation between the studied variables).
The positive relationship between extraversion and seeking support indicates that patients characterised by the ease of expressing thoughts, emotions or feelings, openness in establishing interpersonal contacts, and a strong need for action were open to and looked for support during treatment. On the other hand, patients prone to shyness, uncertainty, and withdrawal in interpersonal relations found it difficult to actively seek support during the treatment process.
To explain the ‘seeking support’ variable, openness to experience, emotional stability (showing a negative correlation), and self-compassion (showing a positive correlation) were additionally qualified. However, they were not statistically significant in the studied group.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the variables need for help, anger, and extraversion explained seeking support and were interrelated at a medium level (R = 0.40), explaining 16% of the variance (R
2 = 0.16). The remaining percentage was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation (
Table 6).
The next stage of the study involved analysis of the support currently received, which consisted of the following components: emotional, instrumental and information support, and satisfaction with the support received. The results are summarised in
Table 7,
Table 8,
Table 9,
Table 10 and
Table 11, followed by detailed analysis and interpretation.
The support currently received was conditioned by the POS (β = 0.22) and exclusion obtained from the SPT.2 questionnaire (β = −0.19) (
Table 7). A positive correlation with the POS means that the surveyed patients who experienced life satisfaction during treatment and had positive self-esteem more noticed the support currently provided in difficult situations. Patients who showed a negative assessment of the situation or themselves had difficulty noticing the support currently received. Respondents with no sense of social exclusion were able to perceive the support currently received.
The self-esteem scale, which showed a positive relationship, was additionally qualified to explain the variable of support currently received by the group of patients but was not statistically significant.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the analysed variables were correlated at a medium level (R = 0.39), explaining 15% of the variance (R
2 = 0.15). The remaining percentage was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation (
Table 7).
Our study shows that emotional support was conditioned by the SES variables (β = 0.16). The positive correlation obtained shows that people characterised by a positive and high assessment of themselves and their actions were open to emotional support from other people. Patients who evaluated themselves negatively were unable to accept emotional support (
Table 8).
In explaining the ‘emotional support’ variable, the ‘exclusion’ scale (SPT.2) was additionally distinguished, showing a negative relationship, but it was not statistically significant in the studied group.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the analysed variables were related to each other at a medium level (R = 0.24). SES variables indicated that emotional support was an important area of social support, explaining 6% of the variance (R2 = 0.06). The remaining percentage of variability was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation.
The study shows that exclusion (β = −0.29) and the POS (β = 0.16) determined instrumental support. Patients characterised by no sense of social exclusion and who were actively participating in activities and social life perceived the instrumental support provided to them. In turn, patients who felt withdrawn and alienated from social activities found it difficult to accept instrumental support during cancer treatment. Those characterised by high satisfaction with life and positive self-esteem during treatment were able to benefit from instrumental support. Patients who showed a negative assessment of the situation or themselves found it difficult to accept instrumental support (
Table 9).
The ‘agreeableness’ scale (β = 0.12), showing a positive correlation, qualified for the explanation of instrumental support, but was not statistically significant in the studied group.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the analysed variables were correlated at a medium level (R = 0.40) and that instrumental support was an important area of social support, explaining 16% of the variance (R2 = 0.16). The remaining percentage was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation.
The study shows that information support was conditioned by the POS (β = 0.30). The positive correlation between the scales indicates that patients characterised by life satisfaction, attaching importance to the positive aspects of life, and having positive self-esteem during the treatment process were able to accept information support. Patients who did not attach importance to the positive aspects of life or did not notice them found it difficult to accept information support (
Table 10).
The explanation of the ‘information support’ variable was additionally supplemented by the following scales: anger (β = −0.10), showing a negative relationship; SES (β = 0.12), showing a positive correlation; anxiety (β = −0.10), showing a negative relationship; and stress (β = 0.26), showing a positive relationship. They were not statistically significant in the studied group of patients.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the analysed variables were correlated with each other at a medium level (R = 0.42) and explained 18% of the variance (R
2 = 0.18). The remaining percentage of variability was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation (
Table 10).
The results in
Table 11 show that satisfaction with the support received was determined by the exclusion scale (SPT.2) (β = −0.28). The negative correlation obtained indicates that patients who, despite their disease, actively participated in activities and social life and could be characterised by no sense of social exclusion seen and were able to appreciate and be satisfied with support in the treatment process. Patients who felt withdrawn and alienated from social activities during the treatment process found it difficult to be satisfied with and show gratitude for the support provided to them.
The POS (β = 0.10), showing a positive relationship, was additionally included in the explanation of the ‘satisfaction with the support received’ variable, but was not statistically significant (
Table 11).
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the analysed variables correlated with each other at a medium level (R = 0.32). The ‘exclusion’ variable (SPT.2) indicates that satisfaction with the support received was an important area of social support, explaining 10% of the variance (R2 = 0.10). The remaining percentage of variability was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation.
Our study shows that protective buffering support was conditioned by exclusion (SPT.2) (β = 0.16) and openness to experience (O) (β = −0.16).
A positive correlation with exclusion means that patients who were characterised by no involvement and activity in life did not join in activities and social life and experienced a sense of social exclusion and showed a special need for protective buffering support. This type of support was not needed by patients who were actively involved in their individual and social lives and did not feel alienated or excluded from society.
On the other hand, patients who were not open to experiences and accepted difficult situations in life with fear and uncertainty showed the need for protective buffering support. In turn, patients who were open-minded and coped with new life situations during cancer treatment did not need such support.
The SCSS (β = −0.13), showing a negative correlation, was additionally used to explain the ‘protective buffering support’ variable in the group of patients, but it was not statistically significant.
The analysis of the correlation coefficient shows that the analysed variables were correlated at a medium level (R = 0.32) and that protective buffering support was an important area of social support, explaining 10% of the variance (R
2 = 0.10). The remaining percentage of variability was due to the influence of independent variables that were not part of the regression equation (
Table 12).