Is Face Age Mapped Asymmetrically onto Space? Insights from a SNARC-like Task
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Stimuli and Apparatus
2.3. Apparatus
2.4. Experimental Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Data Handling
3.2. Analyses of the SNARC-like Effect
3.3. Analyses of the Distance Effect
3.4. Relationship between Handedness and the SNARC-like Effect
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Dehaene, S.; Bossini, S.; Giraux, P. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1993, 122, 371–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moyer, R.S.; Landauer, T.K. Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature 1967, 215, 1519–1520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood, G.; Willmes, K.; Nuerk, H.-C.; Fischer, M.H. On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 50, 489–525. [Google Scholar]
- Shaki, S.; Fischer, M.H.; Petrusic, W.M. Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2009, 16, 328–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- de Hevia, M.D.; Veggiotti, L.; Streri, A.; Bonn, C.D. At birth, humans associate “few” with left and “many” with right. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, 3879–3884.e2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rugani, R.; Vallortigara, G.; Priftis, K.; Regolin, L. Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line. Science 2015, 347, 534–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Felisatti, A.; Laubrock, J.; Shaki, S.; Fischer, M.H. A biological foundation for spatial-numerical associations: The brain’s asymmetric frequency tuning. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2020, 1477, 44–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vallortigara, G. Comparative cognition of number and space: The case of geometry and of the mental number line. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2018, 373, 20170120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Hevia, M.D.; Girelli, L.; Addabbo, M.; Cassia, V.M. Human infants’ preference for left-to-right oriented increasing numerical sequences. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e96412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Davidson, R. Well-being and affective style: Neural substrates and biobehavioural correlates. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2004, 359, 1395–1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Walsh, V. A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 483–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proctor, R.W.; Cho, Y.S. Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 132, 416–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Myachykov, A.; Scheepers, C.; Fischer, M.H.; Kessler, K. TEST: A tropic, embodied, and situated theory of cognition. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2013, 6, 442–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vallesi, A.; Binns, M.A.; Shallice, T. An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition 2008, 107, 501–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ren, P.; Nicholls, M.; Ma, Y.-Y.; Chen, L. Size matters: Non-numerical magnitude affects the spatial coding of response. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e23553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Dalmaso, M.; Vicovaro, M. Evidence of SQUARC and distance effects in a weight comparison task. Cogn. Process. 2019, 20, 163–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- D’Argembeau, A.; Van der Linden, M. Facial expressions of emotion influence memory for facial identity in an automatic way. Emotion 2007, 7, 507–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pecchinenda, A.; Pes, M.; Ferlazzo, F.; Zoccolotti, P. The combined effect of gaze direction and facial expression on cueing spatial attention. Emotion 2008, 8, 628–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fantoni, C.; Baldassi, G.; Rigutti, S.; Prpic, V.; Murgia, M.; Agostini, T. Emotional semantic congruency based on stimulus driven comparative judgements. Cognition 2019, 190, 20–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holmes, K.J.; Lourenco, S.F. Common spatial organization of number and emotional expression: A mental magnitude line. Brain Cogn. 2011, 77, 315–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prete, G. Spatializing emotions besides magnitudes: Is there a left-to-right valence or intensity mapping? Symmetry 2020, 12, 775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pitt, B.; Casasanto, D. Spatializing emotion: No evidence for a domain-general magnitude system. Cogn. Sci. 2017, 42, 2150–2180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Blanco, I.; Nieto, I.; Vazquez, C. Emotional SNARC: Emotional faces affect the impact of number magnitude on gaze patterns. Psychol. Res. 2021, 85, 1885–1893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández, S.R.; Rahona, J.J.; Hervás, G.; Vázquez, C.; Ulrich, R. Number magnitude determines gaze direction: Spatial-numerical association in a free-choice task. Cortex 2011, 47, 617–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tatler, B.W.; Wade, N.J.; Kwan, H.; Findlay, J.M.; Velichkovsky, B.M. Yarbus, eye movements, and vision. i-Perception 2010, 1, 7–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Little, A.C.; Jones, B.; DeBruine, L. The many faces of research on face perception. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 366, 1634–1637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rhodes, M.G.; Anastasi, J.S. The own-age bias in face recognition: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 146–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalmaso, M.; Castelli, L.; Galfano, G. Social modulators of gaze-mediated orienting of attention: A review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2020, 27, 833–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, M.; Stevens, M. Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. J. Cogn. 2018, 1, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Veale, J.F. Edinburgh handedness inventory—Short form: A revised version based on confirmatory factor analysis. Laterality 2013, 19, 164–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ebner, N.C.; Riediger, M.; Lindenberger, U. FACES—A database of facial expressions in young, middle-aged, and older women and men: Development and validation. Behav. Res. Methods 2010, 42, 351–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuznetsova, A.; Brockhoff, P.B.; Christensen, R.H.B. ImerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 82, 38433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brunyé, T.T.; Gardony, A.; Mahoney, C.R.; Taylor, H.A. Body-specific representations of spatial location. Cognition 2012, 123, 229–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- MacNamara, A.; Loetscher, T.; Keage, H.A.D. Mapping of non-numerical domains on space: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Exp. Brain Res. 2017, 236, 335–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramon, M.; Gobbini, M.I. Familiarity matters: A review on prioritized processing of personally familiar faces. Vis. Cogn. 2017, 26, 179–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boroditsky, L.; Fuhrman, O.; McCormick, K. Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition 2011, 118, 123–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maurer, D.; Le Grand, R.; Mondloch, C.J. The many faces of configural processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002, 6, 255–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giuliani, F.; Manippa, V.; Brancucci, A.; Palumbo, R.; Tommasi, L.; Pietroni, D. How emotional is a banknote? The affective basis of money perception. Psychol. Res. 2021, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Root, J.C.; Wong, P.S.; Kinsbourne, M. Left hemisphere specialization for response to positive emotional expressions: A divided output methodology. Emotion 2006, 6, 473–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Casasanto, D. Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right and left-handers. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2009, 138, 351–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- North, M.S.; Fiske, S.T. Modern attitudes toward older adults in the aging world: A cross-cultural meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 993–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chopik, W.J.; Giasson, H. Age differences in explicit and implicit age attitudes across the life span. Gerontologist 2017, 57, S169–S177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnston, R.A.; Kanazawa, M.; Kato, T.; Oda, M. Exploring the structure of multidimensional face-space: The effects of age and gender. Vis. Cogn. 1997, 4, 39–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Craig, B.M.; Lipp, O.V. Facial age cues and emotional expression interact asymmetrically: Age cues moderate emotion categorisation. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 32, 350–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Craig, B.M.; Lipp, O.V. The relationship between visual search and categorization of own and other-age faces. Br. J. Psychol. 2018, 109, 736–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Levin, D.T. Classifying faces by race: The structure of face categories. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1996, 22, 1364–1382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valentine, T.; Endo, M. Towards an exemplar model of face processing: The effects of race and distinctiveness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 1992, 44, 671–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hugenberg, K.; Young, S.G.; Bernstein, M.J.; Sacco, D.F. The categorization-individuation model: An integrative account of the other-race recognition deficit. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 1168–1187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schwarz, W.; Keus, I.M. Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Percept. Psychophys. 2004, 66, 651–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vicovaro, M.; Dalmaso, M. Is ‘heavy’ up or down? Testing the vertical spatial representation of weight. Psychol. Res. 2020, 85, 1183–1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rusconi, E.; Kwan, B.; Giordano, B.L.; Umiltà, C.; Butterworth, B. Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition 2006, 99, 113–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dalmaso, M.; Castelli, L.; Scatturin, P.; Galfano, G. Trajectories of social vision: Eye contact increases saccadic curvature. Vis. Cogn. 2017, 25, 358–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalmaso, M.; Castelli, L.; Galfano, G. Attention holding elicited by direct-gaze faces is reflected in saccadic peak velocity. Exp. Brain Res. 2017, 235, 3319–3332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dalmaso, M.; Castelli, L.; Galfano, G. Early saccade planning cannot override oculomotor interference elicited by gaze and arrow distractors. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2020, 27, 990–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fan, X.; Wang, F.; Shao, H.; Zhang, P.; He, S. The bottom-up and top-down processing of faces in the human occipitotemporal cortex. eLife 2020, 9, e48764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yeo, D.; Wilkey, E.; Price, G.R. The search for the number form area: A functional neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 78, 145–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Demaree, H.; Everhart, D.; Youngstrom, E.; Harrison, D.W. Brain lateralization of emotional processing: Historical roots and a future incorporating “dominance”. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2005, 4, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Dalmaso, M.; Vicovaro, M. Is Face Age Mapped Asymmetrically onto Space? Insights from a SNARC-like Task. Symmetry 2021, 13, 1617. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym13091617
Dalmaso M, Vicovaro M. Is Face Age Mapped Asymmetrically onto Space? Insights from a SNARC-like Task. Symmetry. 2021; 13(9):1617. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym13091617
Chicago/Turabian StyleDalmaso, Mario, and Michele Vicovaro. 2021. "Is Face Age Mapped Asymmetrically onto Space? Insights from a SNARC-like Task" Symmetry 13, no. 9: 1617. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym13091617
APA StyleDalmaso, M., & Vicovaro, M. (2021). Is Face Age Mapped Asymmetrically onto Space? Insights from a SNARC-like Task. Symmetry, 13(9), 1617. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym13091617