Next Article in Journal
White Blood Cells, COVID-19, and Mendelian Randomization
Previous Article in Journal
A NLP Pipeline for the Automatic Extraction of a Complete Microorganism’s Picture from Microbiological Notes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Application of Signals with Rippled Spectra as a Training Approach for Speech Intelligibility Improvements in Cochlear Implant Users

by
Dmitry Nechaev
1,*,
Marina Goykhburg
2,3,
Alexander Supin
1,
Vigen Bakhshinyan
2,3 and
George Tavartkiladze
2,3
1
A.N. Severtsov Institute of Ecology and Evolution, 119071 Moscow, Russia
2
National Research Centre for Audiology and Hearing Rehabilitation, 117513 Moscow, Russia
3
Russian Medical Academy of Continuing Professional Education, 125993 Moscow, Russia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Pers. Med. 2022, 12(9), 1426; https://doi.org/10.3390/jpm12091426
Submission received: 17 July 2022 / Revised: 19 August 2022 / Accepted: 30 August 2022 / Published: 31 August 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Personalized Therapy and Drug Delivery)

Abstract

:
In cochlear implant (CI) users, the discrimination of sound signals with rippled spectra correlates with speech discrimination. We suggest that rippled-spectrum signals could be a basis for training CI users to improve speech intelligibility. Fifteen CI users participated in the study. Ten of them used the software for training (the experimental group), and five did not (the control group). Software based on the phase reversal discrimination of rippled spectra was used. The experimental group was also tested for speech discrimination using phonetic material based on polysyllabic balanced speech material. An improvement in the discrimination of the rippled spectrum was observed in all CI users from the experimental group. There was no significant improvement in the control group. The result of the speech discrimination test showed that the percentage of recognized words increased after training in nine out of ten CI users. For five CI users who participated in the training program, the data on word recognition were also obtained earlier (at least eight months before training). The increase in the percentage of recognized words was greater after training compared to the period before training. The results allow the suggestion that sound signals with rippled spectra could be used not only for testing rehabilitation results after CI but also for training CI users to discriminate sounds with complex spectra.

1. Introduction

Signals with rippled spectra (rippled signals) are convenient tests for measuring the frequency resolution of hearing. Rippled spectra feature periodically alternating spectral peaks and troughs that form a sort of spectral grid. The resolvable ripple density (the number of ripples per frequency unit) may be considered a convenient measure of the frequency resolution of the auditory system. The resolvable ripple depth may be considered a measure of the spectral modulation resolution. Therefore, sound signals with rippled spectra have been applied to assess the frequency resolving power (FRP) of listeners with normal hearing [1,2], patients with hearing loss [3], and cochlear implant (CI) users [4,5,6]. The FRP for CI users is much lower than that for normal-hearing listeners and features considerable variation.
For ripple pattern resolution measurements, various discrimination tasks have been used in conjunction with the various versions of rippled-spectrum tests: (i) discrimination between ripple noise with constant position of ripples and ripple noise with ripple reversals (the spectral ripple discrimination test) [3,7,8]; (ii) discrimination between a flat and rippled spectrum with varying modulation depth (the spectral ripple detection) [9,10]; (iii) discrimination between the ripple spectrum with drifting ripple phase and constant ripple spectrum (the spectral-temporally modulated ripple test) [11,12,13].
The results of measurements using various rippled-spectrum tests correlated with speech recognition in quiet conditions [3,4,5,6,9,10] as well as in background noise [5,6,8]. The strength of the correlation depended on the test signal parameters and the type of deafness [14].
Rippled spectra tests have been suggested for utilization with respect to clinical goals. These tests may be useful as a nonlinguistic diagnostic tool to estimate the rehabilitation results after cochlear implantation and could predict speech recognition by CI users. Several tests were developed based on various tasks: the spectral discrimination test with constant stimuli [13,15] and the spectral modulation detection task (quick spectral modulation detection and easy quick spectral modulation detection) [9,10,16,17,18,19].
Apart from diagnostic application, rippled signals may be a tool for training CI users to extract information from stimuli generated by the CI. Previously, learning effects on solving ripple discrimination and detection tasks have been investigated [8,12,20,21]. The results were contradictory. For the ripple discrimination test in quiet conditions, there was no learning effect after 12 repeated runs, but a learning effect was observed in noise [8]. Drennan et al. [20] reported that results of a spectral discrimination test remained stable over time. Later, the same research group demonstrated that although spectral-ripple discrimination remained constant over the first year after implantation, 20% of the individuals showed a significant improvement in spectral-ripple discrimination [21]. Drennan et al. [22] reported a learning effect in normal-hearing listeners with CI simulation in noise. De Jong et al. [12] demonstrated a significant learning effect for the spectral-temporally modulated ripple test. The learning effect was observed between 2 and 6 weeks, although an instantaneous learning effect during sessions was not detected.
In the present study, we investigated whether CI users can experience improved ripple spectrum discrimination due to training.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

Training was performed by multiple repetitions of runs during which the listener had to distinguish between a test and a reference signal that differed from one another by the patterns of spectral ripples, with the ripple density approaching the resolution limit. Apart from the expected training effect, each run provided an estimate of the current ripple density resolution.

2.2. Listeners

Fifteen CI users with a diagnosis of bilateral sensorineural hearing loss participated in the study. Data for CI users are summarized in Table 1. All CI users had experience using CI for longer than one year. For all CI users, pure-tonal audiometry in free sound field resulted hearing thresholds of 25–30 dB hearing levels within a frequency range from 0.125 to 8 kHz. The speech development of all CI users corresponded to age norms with no disturbances in the lexical and grammatical structure of speech.
Ten of the CI users participated in the training procedure (experimental group, EG), and five CI users were in the control group (CG).

2.3. Rippled-Spectrum Signals

Rippled-spectrum sound signals were used both for testing ripple density resolution and as a signal for training. The bandwidth of the signals ranged from 0.1 to 8 kHz. Within the frequency band, the spectrum of signals featured several spectral maxima and minima (ripples). The ripple density was frequency proportional; i.e., the ripples looked uniform on a logarithmic frequency scale. The density of ripples was specified in ripples per octave (ripples/oct).
The principle of the ripple phase reversal test was to find the maximum ripple density at which a listener could detect the phase reversals of the spectrum ripples. In the test signal, every 400 ms, the rippled spectrum was replaced with a spectrum of the same parameters except for the opposite position of the spectral peaks and troughs on the frequency scale. The signal contained six segments of alternative ripple phases; thus, the overall signal duration was 2400 ms. The ripple phase in the reference signal was kept constant throughout the signal’s duration. The CI users perceived ripple reversals as a change in the signal timbre if the rippled pattern of the spectrum was resolvable. The highest density of spectral ripples at which the listeners were able to detect phase reversals was considered to be the ripple density’s resolution.

2.4. Procedure

For training, ripple-pattern discrimination runs were repeated. In each run, the adaptive variation of the ripple density was performed using a three-alternative force-choice procedure with feedback. Each trial included three stimuli: one with ripples phase reversals (the test) and two with constant ripples phase (the references). The order of the stimuli presentation was varied randomly, trial-by-trial. The task of the CI user was to identify the test signal that differs from the other two. The ripple density varied trial-by-trial adaptively, and a “two-up, one-down” version was used. After two successive correct detections of the test signal, the ripple density in the next trial increased by one step; after every mistake, the ripple density in the next trial decreased by one step. The ripple density varied stepwise using a pseudo-logarithmic scale: 0.7, 1, 1.5, 2, 3, 5, 7, and 10 ripples/oct. Each run continued until 10 turn points (transition from ripple density increase to decrease and back) were obtained. The geometric mean of these 10 points was taken as the threshold estimate for the run.

2.5. Speech Discrimination Test

Phonetic material based on polysyllabic balanced speech material was used (30 words in the group). The words contained all phonemes of the Russian language and were pronounced by a male’s voice in Russian. The CI user’s task was to replicate the words. For each group of 30 words, the percentage of correctly replicated words was determined. Correct answers were recorded only if the CI user accurately reproduced all phonemes of the heard word.
Before measurements, CI users had several training sessions intended to make sure that he/she understands the task of replication the words. Different sets of words were used for each test.

2.6. Control and Training

Before training, in the clinic, the ripple density resolution and speech discrimination were tested in all CI users in the EG and CG groups.
For EG, depending on the listeners’ personal circumstances, the training lasted from 4 to 16 weeks (mean 10 weeks), at one run per day. Every run provided an estimate of the current ripple density resolution. The training proceeded at CI users’ home.
After the training, in the clinic, the ripple density resolution and speech discrimination were retested for the EG and CG. For the CG, ripple density resolutions were retested after 16 weeks.
Apart from the speech-discrimination tests performed in the present study, the results of the before-study tests (more than eight months before the training) were available for five CI users of the EG (“long before” data).

2.7. Instrumentation

The LabView (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA) environment was used for software development.
For the ripple spectrum test, the digitally synthetized signals were digital-to-analog (D/A) converted by a USB 6212 data acquisition board (National Instruments) and played in a free sound field via an SP 90 loudspeaker (Interacoustics, Middelfart, Denmark) located at a distance of 1 m in front of the CI user.
For the speech discrimination test, signals were played with an AC-40 clinical audiometer (Interacoustics, Denmark) and SP 90 loudspeaker (Interacoustics, Denmark) located at a distance of 1 m in front of CI user. The average sound level of the signals was 65 dB SPL in the clinical test.
The training proceeded at CI users’ home using their PC, and digitally synthetized signals were D/A converted by the sound card of PC. During the training, the CI users could use a comfortable sound level by their choice.

2.8. Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using GraphPad Prism 9 software (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA).

3. Results

In the EG, multiple repetitions of ripple-pattern discrimination runs resulted in an increase in ripple density resolutions (Table 2, Figure 1). The resolution increased from 1.2 times (EG6) to 5.7 times (EG1). Within the EG group, the ripple density resolutions before training and after training significantly differed (p = 0.002, Wilcoxon matched pairs test, N = 10, W = 55). In the CG, the same test did not reveal a significant difference between the ripple density resolution of the test and retest (p > 0.9, N = 5, W = 1).
In majority of the CI users, the individual dynamics of the ripple density resolution demonstrated progressive improvement during training (Figure 2). The regression analysis revealed a significant positive slope of regression lines for all CI users, from 0.015 (ripples/oct)/day (EG6, p = 0.029) to 0.094 (ripples/oct)/day (EG3, p < 0.001). The only exception was EG9, which featured a negative slope of -0.43 (ripples/oct)/day; this negative slope was not statistically significant (p = 0.067).
The results of the word recognition test showed that the percentage of recognized words increased after training in 9 out of 10 EG CI users (Table 3, Figure 3). The Wilcoxon matched-pairs test showed a significant difference between the percentages of recognized words before training and after training (p = 0.0039, N = 10, W = 45). In the CG, the Wilcoxon matched-pairs test did not reveal a significant difference between the percentage of recognized words for the test and retest (p > 0.9, N = 5, W = 2).
We had “long before” (more than 8 months before training) data of word recognition for 5 out of 10 EG CI users. For two of these five CI users (EG 3 and 6), the increase in word recognition was observed only after training and was not observed during the preceding period of using the CI. In one CI user (EG2), the increase in the percentage of word recognition was 10% during 8 months before training and 10% further after 4 months of training. For EG5, the increase in percentage of word recognition was 5% during 18 months before training and 15% after 3 months of training.
It is notable that three patients (EG 1, 5, and 6) reported subjective improvements in speech discrimination after training.

4. Discussion

A weak point in the present study was the limited number of CI users. Because of that, EG and CG were rather small, EG and CG were unequal, and CI users have different types of CIs. We assume that there was no device difference that influenced both ripple spectrum resolutions and word recognition. Our assumption was based on the fact that in previous studies, no difference in speech intelligibility was found depending on the manufacturer of the CI system [23]. Lansberger et al. [24] assumed that the ACE (CI 512) processing strategy might in fact benefit spectral resolution by enhancing spectral contrast. The type of array may influence the resolution [14]; however, in the present study, all CI users had a perimodiolar array. This weakness was compensated by a comparison of the control and experimental periods in the same CI users. A comparison of “After training”, “Before training”, and (when available) “Long before” data showed that several (4 to 16) weeks of training for rippled spectra discrimination produced more prominent improvements in speech recognition than the period preceding CI use. Therefore, we suggest that the present study demonstrated significant improvements in both spectral ripple discrimination and speech recognition as a result of training. On average, the spectral ripple discrimination improved by approximately threefold. In previous studies [8,12,20,21], the learning effect has been detected for the spectral temporal modulation task, but those studies did not imply the purpose of training, and there were long intervals between repeated measurements. In the present study, CI users performed the task every day and that might have made the training program more effective.
The data on improvement of ripple pattern discrimination alone do not show that either auditory abilities improved or procedural learning occurred. Procedural learning might appear because EG listeners participated in more sessions than CG listeners. We suppose that the procedural learning for ripple spectra resolutions could not improve word recognition because of substantial differences in testing procedures. Additionally, before the speech test, CI users had training to make sure that they understood the task. We suggest that the improvement in spectral ripple discrimination ability and the percentage of recognized words indirectly indicate perceptual learning in complex spectral discrimination during training.
Based on the obtained results, we hypothesize that spectral ripple discrimination training may be useful for CI users’ learning to resolve separate spectral peaks in complex spectra. This, in turn, can help discriminate and recognize complex sound signals, including speech. Spectral discrimination training could be an addition to analytic and synthetic training approaches.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.N., M.G., A.S. and G.T.; methodology, A.S. and D.N.; software, A.S.; validation, G.T. and A.S.; formal analysis, D.N. and M.G.; investigation, M.G.; resources, G.T., V.B. and A.S.; data curation, A.S. and G.T.; writing—original draft preparation, D.N.; writing—review and editing, A.S., M.G., G.T. and V.B.; visualization, D.N.; supervision, A.S., G.T. and V.B.; project administration, G.T. and V.B.; funding acquisition, A.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by The Russian Foundation for Basic Research, Grant 20-22-00054.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of National Research Centre for Audiology and Hearing Rehabilitation at the meeting of 1 February 2022 (Protocol N2).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, Dmitry Nechaev, upon request.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the listeners for their dedicated efforts.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Milekhina, O.N.; Nechaev, D.I.; Supin, A.Y. Estimation of Frequency Resolving Power of Human Hearing by Different Methods: Roles of Sensory and Cognitive Factors. Hum. Physiol. 2018, 44, 481–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Supin, A.Y.; Popov, V.V.; Milekhina, O.N.; Tarakanov, M.B. Ripple density resolution for various rippled-noise patterns. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1998, 103, 2042–2050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Henry, B.A.; Turner, C.W.; Behrens, A. Spectral peak resolution and speech recognition in quit: Normal hearing, hearing impaired, and cochlear implant listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2005, 118, 1111–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Henry, B.A.; Turner, C.W. The resolution of complex spectral patterns by cochlear implant and normal-hearing listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 113, 2861–2873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Anderson, E.S.; Nelson, D.A.; Kreft, H.; Nelson, P.B.; Oxenham, A.J. Comparing spatial tuning curves, spectral ripple resolution, and speech perception in cochlea implant users. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 130, 364–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Jeon, E.K.; Turner, C.W.; Karsten, S.A.; Henry, B.A.; Gantz, B.J. Cochlear implant users’ spectral ripple resolution. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2015, 138, 2350–2358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Supin, A.Y.; Popov, V.; Milekhina, O.N.; Tarakanov, M.B. Frequency resolving power measured by rippled noise. Hear. Res. 1994, 78, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Won, J.H.; Drennan, W.R.; Rubinstain, J.T. Spectral-rippler resolution correlated with speech reception in noise in cochlear implant users. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2007, 8, 384–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Litvak, L.M.; Spahr, A.J.; Saoji, A.A.; Fridman, G.Y. Relationship between perception of spectral ripple and speech recognition in cochlear implant and vocoder listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007, 122, 982–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Saoji, A.A.; Litvak, L.; Spahr, A.J.; Eddins, D.A. Spectral modulation detection and vowel and consonant identifications in cochlear implant listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 126, 955–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Aronoff, J.M.; Landsberger, D.M. The development of a modified spectral ripple test. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2013, 134, EL217–EL222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. De Jong, M.A.; Briaire, J.J.; Frijns, H.M. Learning effects in psychophysical tests od spectral and temporal resolution. Ear Hear. 2018, 39, 475–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Landsberger, D.M.; Stupa, N.; Aronoff, J.M. SLRM: A nonlinguistic test for audiology clinics. Ear Hear. 2019, 40, 1253–1255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Nechaev, D.I.; Goykhburg, M.V.; Supin, A.Y.; Bakhshinyan, V.V.; Tavartkiladze, G.A. Discrimination of Rippled-Spectrum Signals by Prelingual and Postlingual Cochlear Implant Users. Hum. Physiol. 2020, 46, 119–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Drennan, W.R.; Anderson, E.S.; Won, J.H.; Rubinstein, J.T. Validation of a Clinical Assessment of Spectral-Ripple Resolution for Cochlear Implant Users. Ear Hear. 2014, 35, e92–e98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Noble, J.H.; Gifford, R.; Hedley-Williams, A.J.; Dawant, B.M.; Labadie, R.F. Clinical Evaluation of an Image-Guided Cochlear Implant Programming Strategy. Audiol. Neurotol. 2014, 19, 400–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Dwyer, R.T.; Spahr, T.; Agrawal, S.; Hetlinger, C.; Holder, J.T.; Gifford, R.H. Participant-generated cochlear implant programs: Speech recognition, sound quality and satisfaction. Otol. Neurotol. 2016, 37, e209–e216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gifford, R.H.; Hedley-Williams, A.; Spahr, A.J. Clinical assessment of spectral modulation detection for adult cochlear implant recipients: A non-language based measure of performance outcomes. Int. J. Audiol. 2014, 53, 159–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gifford, R.H.; Noble, J.H.; Camarata, S.M.; Sunderhaus, L.W.; Dwyer, R.T.; Dawant, B.M.; Dietrich, M.S.; Labadie, R.F. The Relationship Between Spectral Modulation Detection and Speech Recognition: Adult Versus Pediatric Cochlear Implant Recipients. Trends Hear. 2018, 22, 2331216518771176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Drennan, W.R.; Won, J.H.; Nie, K.; Jameyson, E.; Rubinstein, J.T. Sensitivity of psychophysical measures to signal processor modifications in cochlear implant users. Hear. Res. 2010, 262, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Drennan, W.R.; Won, J.H.; Timme, A.O.; Rubinstein, J.T. Nonlinguistic Outcome Measures in Adult Cochlear Implant Users Over the First Year of Implantation. Ear Hear. 2016, 37, 354–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Drennan, W.R.; Won, J.H.; Dasika, V.K.; Rubinstein, J.T. Effects of Temporal Fine Structure on the Lateralization of Speech and on Speech Understanding in Noise. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2007, 8, 373–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Taitelbaum-Swead, R.; Kishon-Rabin, L.; Kaplan-Neeman, R.; Muchnik, C.; Kronenberg, J.; Hildesheimer, M. Speech perception of children using Nucleus, Clarion or Med-El cochlear implants. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2005, 69, 1675–1683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Landsberger, D.M.; Padilla, M.; Martinez, A.S.; Eisenberg, L.S. Spectral-Temporal Modulated Ripple Discrimination by Children With Cochlear Implants. Ear Hear. 2018, 39, 60–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The ripple density resolution (ripples/oct) for the EG (before and after training) and CG (test/retest). Each line indicates the result of one CI user.
Figure 1. The ripple density resolution (ripples/oct) for the EG (before and after training) and CG (test/retest). Each line indicates the result of one CI user.
Jpm 12 01426 g001
Figure 2. The individual changes of ripple density resolution (ripples/oct) during training. The straight line is a regression line; the dotted lines show 95% confidence bands of the best-fit line.
Figure 2. The individual changes of ripple density resolution (ripples/oct) during training. The straight line is a regression line; the dotted lines show 95% confidence bands of the best-fit line.
Jpm 12 01426 g002
Figure 3. The percentage of recognized words for EG (long before, immediately before training, and after training) and for CG (test/retest). Each line indicates results for one CI user.
Figure 3. The percentage of recognized words for EG (long before, immediately before training, and after training) and for CG (test/retest). Each line indicates results for one CI user.
Jpm 12 01426 g003
Table 1. Basic data for the CI users.
Table 1. Basic data for the CI users.
IDAgeCI ModelImplantation DateTime of Training, Days
EG 147HiRes 90 K Advantage CI MS Electrode11 December 201984
EG 261HiRes 90 K Advantage CI MS Electrode30 May 2019112
EG 346HiRes 90 K HiFocus Helix electrode8 December 201441
EG 448HiRes 90 K HiFocus Helix electrode24 January 201348
EG 535HiRes 90 K HiFocus Helix electrode28 June 201284
EG 657CI 512 (CA)24 October 201870
EG 737HiRes 90 K HiFocus Helix electrode3 April 2012105
EG 828HiRes 90 K Advantage CI MS Electrode9 October 201928
EG 918HiRes 90 K HiFocus Helix electrode12 October 201282
EG 1010HiRes 90 K HiFocus Helix electrode21 March 201577
CG 146CI 512 (CA)20 June 2017-
CG 233CI 512 (CA)6 June 2019-
CG 333CI 512 (CA)6 June 2019-
CG 411Nucleus Freedom CI24RE(CA)22 June 2011-
CG 546HiRes 90 K HiFocus Helix electrode2 November 2011-
Table 2. Ripple density resolution data.
Table 2. Ripple density resolution data.
Experimental Group
CodeBefore Training
Ripples/oct
After Training
Ripples/oct
After/Before Ratio
EG11.79.55.6
EG21.24.84.0
EG31.23.42.8
EG41.34.93.8
EG51.02.62.6
EG65.97.01.2
EG73.15.31.7
EG80.82.12.6
EG95.49.21.7
EG104.412.12.8
Control Group
CodeTestRetestRetest/Test Ratio
CG11.51.40.9
CG23.43.31.0
CG31.52.61.7
CG46.15.50.9
CG51.71.91.1
Table 3. Word recognition data.
Table 3. Word recognition data.
IDLong Before, %Before Training, %After Training, %
EG1-6090
EG28090100
EG3555570
EG4808080
EG5505570
EG6808095
EG7-90100
EG8-2065
EG9-6095
EG10-8595
TestRetest
CG1-8080
CG2-4545
CG3-2025
CG4-10095
CG5-9095
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nechaev, D.; Goykhburg, M.; Supin, A.; Bakhshinyan, V.; Tavartkiladze, G. Application of Signals with Rippled Spectra as a Training Approach for Speech Intelligibility Improvements in Cochlear Implant Users. J. Pers. Med. 2022, 12, 1426. https://doi.org/10.3390/jpm12091426

AMA Style

Nechaev D, Goykhburg M, Supin A, Bakhshinyan V, Tavartkiladze G. Application of Signals with Rippled Spectra as a Training Approach for Speech Intelligibility Improvements in Cochlear Implant Users. Journal of Personalized Medicine. 2022; 12(9):1426. https://doi.org/10.3390/jpm12091426

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nechaev, Dmitry, Marina Goykhburg, Alexander Supin, Vigen Bakhshinyan, and George Tavartkiladze. 2022. "Application of Signals with Rippled Spectra as a Training Approach for Speech Intelligibility Improvements in Cochlear Implant Users" Journal of Personalized Medicine 12, no. 9: 1426. https://doi.org/10.3390/jpm12091426

APA Style

Nechaev, D., Goykhburg, M., Supin, A., Bakhshinyan, V., & Tavartkiladze, G. (2022). Application of Signals with Rippled Spectra as a Training Approach for Speech Intelligibility Improvements in Cochlear Implant Users. Journal of Personalized Medicine, 12(9), 1426. https://doi.org/10.3390/jpm12091426

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop