Application of Virtual Environments for Biophilic Design: A Critical Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Objective and Methodology
- First, a literature review is performed to understand biophilia, biophilic design, related health theories, categories, and the patterns of biophilic design.
- Next, VEs, their features, classification, experiential design in VE, and their applications are studied.
- Then, a review of studies investigating the applications of virtual natural environment, including mediated and simulated environments, are presented. In this step, applied methods and key factors of existing research are highlighted and major parameters for natural environment and biophilic design studies in VE are identified.
- Finally, based on above reviews, contributing factors to design biophilic environment experience are identified, and the status, applications, capabilities, and limitations of VEs for biophilic design, as well as the future research directions are presented.
3. Biophilia and Biophilic Design
3.1. Biophilic Design for Outdoor/Exterior and Indoor Environments
3.2. Biophilia and Health Theories
- Attention Restoration Theory (ART): a theory refers to a cognitive framework concentrating on the recovery of directed attention fatigue [14,52]. To illustrate, directed attention is very demanding, and attentional fatigue can happen after completing difficult tasks and facing environmental stimulation (e.g., traffic, advertisement, etc.). Directed attention fatigue may result in failure to recognize interpersonal cues and inability to plan. Based on ART, dealing with environments with fascinating triggers (e.g., natural settings) captures involuntary attention and can restore directed attention and cognitive recourses. Fascinating stimuli refer to elements or events that grab an individual’s attention effortlessly [53], which may affect a person in positive “soft fascination” or negative way “hard fascination”. From this point forward, fascination is used instead of soft fascination, which triggers human attention effortlessly and provides a pleasant experience for humans. ART suggests that natural environments are restorative environments because they capture involuntary attention. As a result, it is expected that individual performs required tasks directed to attention better after exposure to restorative environments. Besides fascination, there are three other key characteristics for a restorative environment, as stated by ART. They include “being away” (psychological or physical escape form routine environment), “coherence” (perceiving elements as a coherent picture), and “compatibility” (compatibility of environment’s attributes and person’s expectations) [52].
- Stress Recovery Theory (SRT): a psycho-evolutionary theory proposed by Ulrich [13] suggests that non-threatening natural environments are restorative environments and provide a more positive emotional state and decrease physiological arousal. Based on SRT, humans prefer natural environment evolution. Engaging with pleasant environment results in reducing stress by improving physiological responses and positive emotions as well as controlling negative emotions and thoughts.
3.3. Biophilic Design Patterns and Categories
4. Virtual Environments
4.1. Immersion and Presence
- Technology factors, including sensory and realism factors [66], such as multimodal information consistency [69] and presentation, environmental richness, information consistency with objective world, sense realism [66], meaningfulness of media content [70] and ease of navigation and equipment comfort [71].
- Social factors, which is the interaction between users and technology [67].
4.2. Presence and Experiential Design in VE
- Sensory dimension: all sensory inputs generated through hardware and software.
- Cognitive dimension: different types of task engagement or mental engagement related to participant’s motivation, task meaningfulness, and continuity.
- Affective dimension: an effective mimic of user’s emotional state in the VEs, same as the real-world environment.
- Active dimension: the degree of personal relation and connection to VE, scenario, and other avatars
- Relational dimension: experience related to co-presence and social presence.
4.3. Classification of VR Systems
- Non-immersive systems: this category is the basic type of VR systems and can be employed without any special input or output devices. Non-immersive systems provide the least immersion presented by 3D graphics and are screen-based and pointer- driven [77]. With non-immersive systems, users are able to interact with VE through computer screens, but not immersed in it [78].
- Partially immersive systems: this group includes enhanced systems that improve user’s immersion. They display VE on a large screen via a single projector [77]. They support partial body tracking or 3D experience of scenes through special gloves or special goggles, respectively.
- Fully immersive systems: the ultimate version of VR systems supports stereoscopic views of a scene [77]. Binocular head-based, such as Head Mounted Displays (HMDs), and room-based VR technology, such as Cave Automatic Virtual Environments (CAVEs), are subcategories of fully immersive systems.
4.4. Sensation and VE Sensory Input
4.5. Applications of VE in AEC
5. A Review of Virtual Natural Environment Studies
5.1. Review and Classification of Studies in Virtual Natural Environment
- Biophilic categories: this includes three major categories; natural elements, natural analogues, and experience of place and space.
- Biophilic patterns: patterns are summarized in Table 1 based on Kellert and Browning’s studies [7,55,56,57,58], which includes: “visual and non-visual connection with nature” [7], light, thermal and airflow variability [7], natural material, “natural shapes and forms, natural patterns and processes, as well as evolved human–nature and place-based relationship” [56].
- Biological responses: biological responses related to natural environment are “stress reduction”, “cognitive performance”, and “emotion, mood, and preference” [7].
- Collected data and measurements: physiological data, psychological data, cognitive data, and VE validation data. Types of tests and tasks that are carried out in the studies for data collection are provided in Table A2.
- Subjects: based on nature-health relations, subjects in the studies may be healthy or patient.
- Stress or mental fatigue induction: stress-inducing tasks may deploy as stressor before, during, or after exposure to natural environment. However, in some studies, mental fatigue induction tasks are considered according to research interest. Stress induction or mental fatigue tasks can be part of the design to assess biological responses in relation to potential stress recovery or restoration effects of virtual natural settings. Therefore, the Subfactors include pre-exposure, peri-exposure, post exposure, and none.
- Natural environment settings: based on reviewed studies, the subfactors are biophilic indoor environment, biophilic outdoor environment, urban green space or streetscape, and wild natural environments (such as a forest).
- Delivery mode of virtual nature: this can be either simulated or mediated environment [113]. Therefore, the delivery mode is classified to virtual 3D model, 360° video, 2D video, 360° photo, and static image.
- VE systems: VE systems are categorized into fully immersive (HMDs or CAVEs), partially immersive (projectors), non-immersive (screens), and none.
- VE presence: the studies are either single-user or multi-user system.
- Sensory inputs: sensory inputs include visual, auditory, olfactory, tactile, and thermal stimuli and wind.
- Research design: the design of research includes total settings/contexts and total conditions.
- Natural environment presentation: the subcategories are reality vs. virtual environment and virtual vs. virtual environment.
- Sample size: sample size of studies is categorized to less than 20, 20–50, 51–100, and over 100.
- Duration of exposure to each condition: the subcategories are less than 10 min, 10–20 min, 21–60 min, more than one hour, and more than to one day.
- Total duration of each session: total duration of each session is less than two hours, more than two hours, or more than one day.
5.2. Analysis and Discussion of Reviewed Literature
- Biophilic categories and patterns: among three major categories of biophilic design, natural elements are utilized the most (100%). Natural elements followed by natural analogues are easier to understand and implement compared to the experience of place and space. Thus, it might be the reason for such a pattern. For biophilic indoor environments, natural elements and natural analogues are common categories. Natural analogous has been explored, especially in most recent studies (2019 and 2020). Regarding biophilic patterns, the three most reported patterns are visual connection with nature (100%), light (84%), and followed by non-visual connection with nature (53.6%). In some studies, the authors consider wild natural environments, such as forest or coast, as their natural setting. In these studies, visual connection with nature and light are common biophilic patterns. Depending on research interests, there might be some non-visual connection with nature, including nature sounds (e.g., birds tweeting sound, breeze, etc.) or natural odors (e.g., forest, flowers, etc.). In addition, some studies consider evolved human–nature relationship pattern (13%), specifically prospect, and refuge features [23,119,124,126,135]. Figure 1 shows the distribution of reviewed studies based on biophilic patterns.
- Biological responses: all three responses, “stress reduction”, “cognitive performance”, as well as “emotion, mood, and performance,” have been studies based on research domain and interest. Attention restoration and/or stress recovery theories are studied in most of the reviewed literature. According to health-related theories discussed in Section 3.2, psychophysiological responses to natural settings are emphasized by SRT, while cognitive responses are highlighted by ART. Based on the literature, emotion, mood, and performance (73.6%) as well as stress reduction (71%) are the most reported biological responses and there is less research on cognitive performance (34.2%).
- Collected data: psychological data is the most reported collected data (86.8%) among the reviewed literature. Some researchers also collect physiological data (60.5%) to validate responses and results. Cognitive data is the least reported collected data (34.2%). The distribution of reviewed literature based on biological responses and collected data is illustrated in Figure 2.
- Measurements: there is a wide range of methods, tests, and tasks for data collection. Recording heart rate (HR) is the most reported measurement method for physiological data. Moreover, heart rate variability (HRV), skin conductance level (SCL), and/or blood pressure (BP) have been utilized in many studies. For psychological data related to emotion, mood, and performance, several types of self-rated questionnaires are used. The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) and Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) are the most reported standard psychological measurements. PANAS assesses participant’s mood [142,144] and the PRS is a four-factor model that evaluates the restorative quality of environment through four elements of ART [145,146]. Regarding cognitive data, several quizzes and tests are reported. One of the cognitive tests is the Attention Network Test (ANT), which measures attentional performance of individuals in a single integrated task through alerting, orienting, and executive control [147,148]. In addition to ANT, reaction time tests, working memory tests, Stroop task, Droodle task, and Compatibility task are some examples of cognitive tests and tasks that have been employed in the reviewed literature. In some VE research, there are also VE-related measurements, including the Igroup Presence Questionnaire (IPQ), VE system ease of usability and navigation [137], and the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) [140].
- Subjects: although there are many studies on patients with mental disease in the psychology and therapy domain, this reviewed literature is focused on healthy subjects. Except in [121] (patients with substance use disorder), [122] (patients with cognitive or physical impairments), and [143] (patients with stress and/or burnout syndrome), other studies involved healthy users (92%). It is noteworthy that although Gerber et al. [134] studied healthy patients in an intensive care unit (ICU), he proposed that VR stimulation in ICU can also be possible and beneficial for critically ill patients.
- Stress or mental fatigue induction: since many studies worked on natural environments as restorative environments, different types of stress or mental fatigue inducting tasks are utilized pre- (44.7%), peri- (21%), or post (26.3%) exposure to the natural environment. The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) [124,130,136], Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) [129], and various arithmetic tests [34,113,133,140] are examples of stress inducing tasks to stimulate user’s stress level. TSST is a widely used protocol that triggers participant’s social stress in laboratory settings [149], and PASAT consists of a couple of questions pertaining to mental mathematic calculation skills [150]. In addition, some physical stress inducting tasks, including cold pressor task and undergoing dental treatment as a stressor [131], are reported in the literature. Besides research that studied cognitive performance, some studies used cognitive tests only as a stressor to create mental fatigue in subjects in the pre-restoration part (e.g., [34,123,133]).
- Natural environment setting/context: wild natural scenes, such as forest environment, which may include diverse foliage, birds singing, and various smells, are considered as natural environment settings in many of the reviewed literature (68.4%). Urban green spaces, e.g., public parks in urban context, is also considered in some studies (l8.4%, e.g., [21,23,123,125]). In the design domain, natural environment settings are mostly biophilic indoor environments (18.4%, e.g., [31,32,33,34]) or biophilic outdoor environments (less than 5%, e.g., [51])
- Delivery mode: various delivery modes have been employed, including virtual 3D model (34.2%), 2D videos (28.9%), 360° video (23.7%), static image (10.5%), and 360° photo (7.9%). The 2D and 360° videos are the most reported delivery modes because they are not only realistic but also are easier to get prepared for test compared to virtual 3D models. However, since 2010, simulated restorative environments also has attracted special attentions. Figure 3 illustrates the distribution of reviewed literature based on their delivery mode.
- VE system: depending on delivery mode, utilized VE systems are different. For virtual 3D models, 360° videos, and 360° photos, scholars mostly used fully immersive VE (58%) that provides the most immersive experience for participants. However, few studies employ non-immersive VE (8%) for 3D models, 360° videos, or 360° photos, which requires the least equipment, and can be presented through normal screens. In addition, many studies use 2D video (fixed-angle from one point of view) or static image delivery mode. Since these delivery modes do not provide an immersive experience for users, normal plasma screens are sufficient as a VE system. This VE system is classified as the “none” category, which is employed considerably (39.5%) due to the accessibility of this group.
- VE presence: VE studies are classified into single-user and multi-user based on the number of VE participants that share the same experience [81]. Reviewed literature shows that the single-user system is much more common (100%) in this domain. No research study with a multi-user system for biophilic design in VEs has been found.
- Sensory inputs: as expected, visual stimuli are the major sensory inputs in reviewed studies. In addition, auditory stimuli (55.2%), followed by olfactory cues (10.5%, e.g., [123,125,137,139]), tactile (less than 3%, [139]), thermal, and wind (less than 3%, [133]), are considered in researches. Generally, study on various sensory inputs especially draw attention in the psychology and informatics domain.
- Research design: based on research interest, scholars design different settings and conditions for their study. Most studies (about 79%) consider one or two major settings and design various conditions based on that. For example, Tabrizian et al. [23] study 18 different conditions to evaluate design elements. On the other hand, some researchers focus on only one setting and study factors, such as natural elements of the environment [51], immersion [113], VE interaction [135], sensory inputs [137].
- Natural elements presentations: since the literature review focuses on virtual natural environments, different research conditions and natural elements are presented and studied in virtual format (3D model, video, or photograph, 76.3%). There are also some research studies that explore natural elements presentation in virtual environment vs. actual natural environment (reality) (21%, e.g., [22,31,33]).
- Sample size: most research considers a small size sample (20–100) (71%). Few studies design the research for less than 20 (15.8%) or over 100 (13%) samples.
- Duration of exposure to each condition: although exposures of longer than 10 min are recommended for change in restorativeness [137], there are many studies with an exposure of fewer than 10 min (52.6%) to the natural environment. Overall, the literature shows that a 20-min exposure to natural elements is effective for restoration and human biological reactions. Therefore, most of the reviewed studies consider an exposure of up to 20 min (92.1%) to each condition for biophilic studies. However, based on a recent study [34], the impact of exposure to the biophilic environment on physiological responses is immediate, especially in the first four minutes of exposure. Figure 4 highlights the distribution of reviewed literature based on duration of exposure to each condition as well as employed sensory inputs and sample size.
- The total duration of each session: the total duration of each session is mostly less than two hours (94.7%). However, there are some studies (e.g., [20]), especially in the psychology domain, which investigate the individual’s performance and biological responses under real circumstance over several weeks or months.
6. Design Biophilic Environment Experience in VE: Applications, Capabilities, and Limitations
- Sensory dimension: although most of the biophilic studies in VE focus on visual stimuli as single sensory input (see Table A4), adding other sensory modalities (e.g., auditory, haptic, olfactory, thermal) improves the user’s VE experience [74] and affect biological responses to natural settings [123,139].
- Cognitive dimension: it comprises different types of task engagement or mental engagement in the VE experiment [74], which provides an opportunity to apply the ART theory. This dimension allows studying individual’s cognitive framework and task performance after exposure to biophilic environment. Participant’s cognitive performance can be measured by validated cognitive tests (e.g., PRS, ANT).
- Affective dimension: it supports the simulation of the user’s emotional state in virtual biophilic environments similar to real-world situations [74]. For example, based on the ART theory, elements of “soft fascination” such as wind breezes can create an affectively positive experience [53], which may be replicated in VE. The affective dimension in VE can be assessed by different physiological (e.g., HR, SCL, BP) and psychological (e.g., PANAS) measurements.
- Active dimension: it refers to personal connections with the surroundings (biophilic elements and patterns). An individual’s background and past experience, such as childhood experience of nature, can also have an impact on personal connection to biophilic elements and nature [9].
- Technical factors: immersion and realism.
- User factors: they include mood, age, and familiarity with technology. For example, Weech et al. [158] discussed that people with more gaming experience indicated partially a higher level of presence in VE. Other studies showed that older participants experienced greater difficulties when mismatch or incorrect visual sensory stimuli existed [159], which also had an impact on participants’ mobility and balance [160]. Moreover, there are some additional factors related to users that impact biophilia studies in VEs:
- ▪
- Gender: this factor has not been studied in-details in literature. However, some studies demonstrated that participants had different responses to biophilic elements based on their gender. For example, Yin et al. [32] showed that during their experiment, female participants spent more time on biophilic elements in VE.
- ▪
- Childhood experience of nature: the literature suggests that childhood experience of nature is related to an individual’s meaningful relationship with nature and natural environments [161]. This also may affect the perception of the restorative effect of the natural environment [23]. Therefore, it can result in the increase of calming effects of nature for those persons and more motivation to engage with natural elements [9]. In virtual biophilic environment exposure, Yin et al. [32] also found that individuals growing up in rural or suburban areas spent more time engaging with natural and biophilic elements.
- ▪
- Individual preferences, as well as differences in perception and cognition: based on information processing theory [162], a perception system creates meaningful representations of numerous sensory stimuli in the external environment and organizes psychological apprehension. On the other hand, the ART theory suggests that the cognitive resources of people can be restored by interacting with attractive stimuli of natural settings [52]. However, individual differences may impact his or her perception and cognition processes, i.e., the way individuals perceive an environment and biophilic stimuli; and, consequently, the potential of the VE’s restorativeness [135].
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Literature | Biophilic Categories and Patterns | Biological Responses | Collected Data | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Natural Elements | Natural Analogues | Experience of Place and Space | Stress Reduction | Emotion, Mood and Preference | Cognitive Performance | Physiological Data | Psychological Data | Cognitive Data | |||||||
Visual | Non-Visual | Light | Thermal and Airflow | Material | Shapes and Forms | Patterns and Processes | Human–Nature | Place-Based | |||||||
[34] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||
[33] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[119] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||
[120] | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[121] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||
[122] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[32] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||
[123] | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||
[124] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||
[125] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[126] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||
[127] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||
[128] | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[31] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[22] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||
[129] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||
[23] | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[130] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[131] | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
* | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[51] | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[132] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[19] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||
[21] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[133] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||
[134] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[135] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[136] | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||
[137] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
* | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[138] | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[139] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[140] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[143] | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||
[20] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[141] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[142] | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[113] | * | * | * | * | * | * |
Literature | Measurements | ||
---|---|---|---|
Physiological Data | Psychological or Cognitive Data | VE Validation Data | |
[34] | heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV), skin conductance level (SCL), blood pressure (BP) | State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) | |
[33] | HR | Working memory tests (Cog: color and shape); Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (mood), | Igroup Presence Questionnaire (IPQ) |
[119] | SCL | PANAS (mood), perceived restorativeness scale (PRS) (restorativeness), DSS, EBS (disgust and beauty of nature) | |
[120] | Summary of Positive and Negative Experiences (SPANE) (mood), Multidimensional State Boredom Scale (MSBS) (boredom), Inclusion of Nature in Self Scale (INS) | Presence and Judgment Questionnaire | |
[121] | HR | PANAS (mood), Overall mood assessment | |
[122] | STAI (anxiety), Music in Dementia Assessment Scales (MiDAS) (mood), Observation | VE Experience Questionnaire | |
[32] | BP; HR; HRV; SCL | Cog: Stroop test (color-word); Guilford’s Alternative Uses (AU test); Eye tracking; PSY: self-reported preference to biophilic patterns | |
[123] | NSR (pleasantness of window view); PRS; Nitsch’s Personal State Scale (mood, fatigue, and arousal) | ||
[124] | HR, BP, Salivary Amylase | Brief Profile of Mood States (BPOMS) | |
[125] | SCL | Perceived Pleasantness | |
[126] | HR, HRV, SCL | Digit Span Backwards (DSB), Necker Cube Pattern Control (NCPC) (attention and memory) | |
[127] | Electroencephalogram (EGG) | Profile of Mood States (POMS-SF) (stress), Cog: Stroop color task | |
[128] | PANAS, 8 Emotion | ITC- Sense of Presence Inventory (ITC-SOPI) | |
[31] | HR; SCL; BP | Cog: visual reaction time task; visual backward digit span task; Stroop task; PSY: self-reported emotion changes | |
[22] | walking speed; HR; | Perceived Environmental Restorativeness (PER); Physical Activity Affect Scale (PAAS); Ratings of Perceived Exertion (RPE); enjoyment scale; perceived motivational effect; open-ended questions | Presence scale |
[129] | Systolic Blood Pressure (SBP); HR; Salivary α amylase activity (activity of the sympathetic nervous system) | POMS (profile of mood state) | |
[23] | PRS; Perceived Safety (PS) | ||
[130] | Stress and emotion: HRV; salivary cortisol | STAI; PANAS | IPQ (presence); Game Experience Questionnaire (immersion) |
[131] | Numeric Rating Scale (NRS) for pain experience; McGill Pain Questionnaire (SR-MPQ); NRS recalled pain at 1week follow-up | ||
Modified Dental Anxiety Scale (MDAS); NRS pain experience; NRS for stress (From the Profile of Mood States); PRS; NRS recalled pain | SR based on IPQ, reality judgement and presence Q | ||
[51] | PRS | ||
[132] | HRV; HR; Near-Infrared Time-Resolved Spectroscopy (TRS) | Subjective feeling measured with SD method (semantic differential method) | |
[19] | Biophilic Attitudes Inventory (BAI); PANAS; PRS; Cog: Attention Network Test (ANT); Proof Reading Task (PRT); Digit-span test; Compatibility task; | ||
[21] | PRS; PANAS; Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21) | ||
[133] | HRV, Electrodermal activity (EDA) | PANAS | Modified Reality Judgement and Presence Questionnaire (MRJPQ) |
[134] | Vital monitoring system | Cog: use Eye-tracker | IPQ, Presence Questionnaire (PQ), System Usability Scale (SUS), Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) |
[135] | PRS; observation; semi-structured interview | ||
[136] | HR, HRV, T-wave amplitude; saliva cortisol; subjective | Short scale (sense of presence) | |
[137] | NSR (Anxiety and Relaxation) | VE System usability (hand-controller and navigation) | |
HR; SCL | NSR (odor and preference) | ||
[138] | Cog: Trail Making Task (TMT), Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWA) (attention) | ||
[139] | HR; SCL | Zuckerman Inventory of Personal Reactions (ZIPERS); PSY: mental-arithmetic quiz | IPQ |
[140] | HR; SCL | Cog: Sustain Attention to Response Task (SART); ZIPERS; Self Report stress | Simulator Sickness Questionnaire |
[143] | Pulse; SBP; DBP | Self-estimated level of current stress; Emotional state test; Stress and energy test (SE); Experienced Deviation from Normal state (EDN); mental fatigue: Syllogism I–II | |
[20] | Semi-structured interview; work satisfaction, office perception, and mood surveys; responses to email queries; journal entries; | ||
[141] | HR; IBI; | Camera; PRT; name-a-Droodle’’ task; ‘‘invent-a-Droodle’’ task; ‘‘tin can unusual uses’’ task | |
[142] | PANAS; Cog: backwards digit- span task; ANT | ||
[113] | HR (inter beat interval, IBI); SCL | Affect questionnaire based on PANAS and ZIPERS | ITC-SOPI |
Literature | Subjects | Stress or Mental Fatigue Induction | Natural Environment Setting/ Context | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Healthy | Patient | Pre-Exposure | Peri-Exposure | Post Exposure | None | Biophilic Indoor Environment | Biophilic Outdoor Environment | Urban Green Space or Streetscape | Wild Nature | |
[34] | * | * | * | |||||||
[33] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[119] | * | * | * | |||||||
[120] | * | * | * | |||||||
[121] | * | * | * | |||||||
[122] | * | * | * | |||||||
[32] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[123] | * | * | * | |||||||
[124] | * | * | * | |||||||
[125] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[126] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[127] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[128] | * | * | * | |||||||
[31] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[22] | * | * | * | |||||||
[129] | * | * | * | |||||||
[23] | * | * | * | |||||||
[130] | * | * | * | |||||||
[131] | * | * | * | |||||||
* | * | * | * | |||||||
[51] | * | * | * | |||||||
[132] | * | * | * | |||||||
[19] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[21] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[133] | * | * | * | |||||||
[134] | * | * | * | |||||||
[135] | * | * | * | |||||||
[136] | * | * | * | |||||||
[137] | * | * | * | |||||||
* | * | * | ||||||||
[138] | * | * | * | |||||||
[139] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[140] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[143] | * | * | * | |||||||
[20] | * | * | * | |||||||
[141] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||
[142] | * | * | * | * | ||||||
[113] | * | * | * |
Literature | Delivery Mode | VE System | VE Presence | Sensory Inputs | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Simulated | Mediated | Fully Immersive (HMDs or CAVEs) | Partially Immersive, Projectors | Non-Immersive, Screen | None | Single-User System | Multi-User System | Visual | Auditory | Olfactory | Tactile | Thermal and Wind | ||||
Virtual 3D Model | 360° Video | 2D Video | 360° Photo | Static Image | ||||||||||||
[34] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[33] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[119] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[120] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[121] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[122] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[32] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[123] | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||
[124] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[125] | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||
[126] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[127] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[128] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[31] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[22] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[129] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[23] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[130] | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||
[131] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
* | * | * | * | |||||||||||||
[51] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[132] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[19] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[21] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[133] | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||
[134] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[135] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[136] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[137] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
* | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[138] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[139] | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||
[140] | * | * | * | * | * | |||||||||||
[143] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[20] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[141] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[142] | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||||
[113] | * | * | * | * |
Literature | Research Design | |||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Design | Natural Elements Presentation | Sample Size | Duration of Exposure to Each Condition | Total Duration of Each Session | ||||||||||||
Total Settings/Contexts | Total Conditions | Reality vs. Virtual Environment | Virtual vs. Virtual Environment | Less than 20 | 20–50 | 51–100 | Over 100 | Less than 10 min | 10–20 min | 21–60 min | More than One Hour | More than One Day | Less than Two Hours | More than Two Hours | More than One Day | |
[34] | 1 | 4 | * | 100 | 6 | * | ||||||||||
[33] | 2 | 4 | * | * | 35 | 5 | * | |||||||||
[119] | 2 | 3 | * | * | * | * | ||||||||||
[120] | 1 | 3 | * | 96 | 5 | * | ||||||||||
[121] | 2 | 2 | - | - | 36 | 10 | * | |||||||||
[122] | 1 | 1 | - | - | 66 | 3–10 | 10–20 | * | ||||||||
[32] | 2 | 8 | * | 30 | 13 | * | ||||||||||
[123] | 2 | 5 | * | 122 | 15 | * | ||||||||||
[124] | 7 | 7 | * | 96 | 5 | * | ||||||||||
[125] | 3 | 3 | * | 154 | 3 | * | ||||||||||
[126] | 1 | 3 | * | 60 | 5 | * | ||||||||||
[127] | 6 | 6 | * | 120 | 5 | * | ||||||||||
[128] | 1 | 2 | * | 50 | 5 | * | ||||||||||
[31] | 2 | 4 | * | * | 28 | 10 | * | |||||||||
[22] | 2 | 3 | * | * | 26 | 10 | * | |||||||||
[129] | 2 | 2 | * | 30 | 9.5 | * | ||||||||||
[23] | 2 | 18 | * | 87 | * | * | ||||||||||
[130] | 2 | 3 | * | 62 | 7 | * | ||||||||||
[131] | 2 | 3 | * | 85 | 4 | * | ||||||||||
3 | 3 | * | 70 | * | * | |||||||||||
[51] | 1 | 2 | * | 68 | 20 | * | ||||||||||
[132] | 2 | 2 | * | 17 | 1.5 | * | ||||||||||
[19] | 2 | 5 | * | 184 | 10 | * | ||||||||||
[21] | 3 | 3 | * | 220 | 2–3 | * | ||||||||||
[133] | 3 | 3 | * | 18 | 15 | * | ||||||||||
[134] | 3 | 3 | - | - | 37 | 15 | * | |||||||||
[135] | 1 | 2 | * | 20 | 4–7 | * | ||||||||||
[136] | 2 | 3 | * | 30 | 40 | * | ||||||||||
[137] | 2 | 4 | * | 14 | 5 | * | ||||||||||
1 | 3 | * | 14 | 3 | * | |||||||||||
[138] | 1 | 2 | * | 40 | 20 | * | ||||||||||
[139] | 2 | 2 | * | 22 | 10 | * | ||||||||||
[140] | 3 | 3 | * | 69 | 10 | * | ||||||||||
[143] | 1 | 2 | * | 18 | 30 | * | ||||||||||
[20] | 2 | 2 | - | - | 7 | 6-weeks | * | |||||||||
[141] | 2 | 3 | * | 90 | 35 | * | ||||||||||
[142] | 2 | 2 | * | 38 | 10 | * | ||||||||||
[113] | 1 | 2 | * | 80 | 10 | * |
References
- United Nations. World Population Prospects 2019: Highlights. 2019. Available online: https://www.un.org/development/desa/publications/world-population-prospects-2019-highlights.html (accessed on 19 March 2021).
- Unsplash, T.J. Urban Health. World Health Organization. 2021. Available online: www.who.int/health-topics/urban-health (accessed on 19 March 2021).
- Dye, C. Health and urban living. Science 2008, 319, 766–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- United States Environmental Protection Agency, Indoor Air Quality. 2018. Available online: https://www.epa.gov/report-environment/indoor-air-quality (accessed on 23 August 2019).
- Klepeis, N.E.; Nelson, W.C.; Ott, W.R.; Robinson, J.P.; Tsang, A.M.; Switzer, P.; Behar, J.V.; Hern, S.C.; Engelmann, W.H. The National Human Activity Pattern Survey (NHAPS): A resource for assessing exposure to environmental pollutants. J. Expo. Anal. Environ. Epidemiol. 2001, 11, 231–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cox, D.T.C.; Shanahan, D.F.; Hudson, H.L.; Fuller, R.A.; Gaston, K.J. The impact of urbanisation on nature dose and the implications for human health. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2018, 179, 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Browning, W.; Ryan, C.; Clancy, J. 14 Patterns of Biophilic Design; Terrapin Bright Green, LLC: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Mehta, R.; Zhu, R.; Cheema, A. Is Noise Always Bad? Exploring the Effects of Ambient Noise on Creative Cognition. J. Consum. Res. 2012, 39, 784–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hinds, J.; Sparks, P. The affective quality of human-natural environment relationships. Evol. Psychol. 2011, 9, 451–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tyrväinen, L.; Ojala, A.; Korpela, K.; Lanki, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lohr, V.I.; Pearson-Mims, C.H.; Goodwin, G.K. Interior plants may improve worker productivity and reduce stress in a windowless environment. J. Environ. Hortic. 1996, 14, 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gray, T.; Birrell, C. Are biophilic-designed site office buildings linked to health benefits and high performing occupants? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 12204–12222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Losito, B.D.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.A.; Zelson, M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaplan, R.; Kaplan, S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- White, M.; Smith, A.; Humphryes, K.; Pahl, S.; Snelling, D.; Depledge, M. Blue space: The importance of water for preference, affect, and restorativeness ratings of natural and built scenes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 482–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bedimo-Rung, A.L. The Significance of Parks to Physical Activity and Public Health: A Conceptual Model. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2005, 28, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuo, F.E.; Bacaicoa, M.; Sullivan, W.C. Transforming Inner-City Landscapes: Trees, Sense of Safety, and Preference. Environ. Behav. 1998, 33, 343–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- James, P.; Hart, J.E.; Banay, R.F.; Laden, F. Exposure to greenness and mortality in a nationwide prospective cohort study of women. Environ. Health Perspect. 2016, 124, 1344–1352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Boggs, J.B. The Roles of Biophilic Attitudes and Auditory Stimuli within Attention Restoration Theory; University of Nevada: Las Vegas, NE, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman, B.; Freier, N.G.; Kahn, P.H.; Lin, P.; Sodeman, R. Office window of the future?-Field-based analyses of a new use of a large display. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2008, 66, 452–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McAllister, E.; Bhullar, N.; Schutte, N.S. Into the woods or a stroll in the park: How virtual contact with nature impacts positive and negative affect. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Calogiuri, G.; Litleskare, S.; Fagerheim, K.A.; Rydgren, T.L.; Brambilla, E.; Thurston, M. Experiencing nature through immersive virtual environments: Environmental perceptions, physical engagement, and affective responses during a simulated nature walk. Front. Psychol. 2018, 8, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tabrizian, P.; Baran, P.K.; Smith, W.R.; Meentemeyer, R.K. Exploring perceived restoration potential of urban green enclosure through immersive virtual environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 55, 99–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heydarian, A.; Pantazis, E.; Gerber, D.; Becerik-Gerber, B. Use of Immersive Virtual Environments to Understand Human-Building Interactions and Improve Building Design. In Proceedings of the 2015 ASCE International Workshop on Computing in Civil Engineering, Austin, TX, USA, 21–23 June 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Vigier, T.; Siret, D.; Moreau, G.; Lescop, L. Feeling the urban project: The use of virtual reality for a perceptual approach of the urban climatic environment. In Proceedings of the Envisioning Architecture: Design, Evaluation, Communication-Proceedings of the 11th conference of the European Architectural Envisioning Association (EAEA-11), Milan, Italy, 25–28 September 2013; pp. 297–304. [Google Scholar]
- Zou, Z.; Ergan, S. Where do we look? An eye-tracking study of architectural features in building design. In Proceedings of the 35th CIB W78 2018 Conference: IT in Design, Construction, and Management, Chicago, IL, USA, 1–3 October 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Abdelhameed, W.A. Virtual reality use in architectural design studios: A case of studying structure and construction. Procedia Comput. Sci. 2013, 25, 220–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Liu, Y.; Lather, J.; Messner, J. Virtual Reality to Support the Integrated Design Process: A Retrofit Case Study. Comput. Civ. Build. Eng. 2014, 2014, 801–808. [Google Scholar]
- Pedro, A.; Le, Q.T.; Park, C.S. Framework for Integrating Safety into Construction Methods Education through Interactive Virtual Reality. J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract. 2016, 142, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, D.; Lucas, J. Virtual reality simulation for construction safety promotion. Int. J. Inj. Control Saf. Promot. 2015, 22, 57–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yin, J.; Zhu, S.; Macnaughton, P.; Allen, J.G.; Spengler, J.D. Physiological and cognitive performance of exposure to biophilic indoor environment. Build. Environ. 2018, 132, 255–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yin, J.; Arfaei, N.; MacNaughton, P.; Catalano, P.J.; Allen, J.G.; Spengler, J.D. Effects of Biophilic Interventions in Office on Stress Reaction and Cognitive Function: A Randomized Crossover Study in Virtual Reality. Indoor Air 2019, 29, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Emamjomeh, A.; Zhu, Y.; Beck, M. The potential of applying immersive virtual environment to biophilic building design: A pilot study. J. Build. Eng. 2020, 32, 101481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yin, J.; Yuan, J.; Arfaei, N.; Catalano, P.J.; Allen, J.G.; Spengler, J.D. Effects of biophilic indoor environment on stress and anxiety recovery: A between-subjects experiment in virtual reality. Environ. Int. 2020, 136, 105427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martín-Martín, A.; Orduna-Malea, E.; Thelwall, M.; Delgado López-Cózar, E. Google Scholar, Web of Science, and Scopus: A systematic comparison of citations in 252 subject categories. J. Informetr. 2018, 12, 1160–1177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Martín-Martín, A.; Thelwall, M.; Orduna-Malea, E.; Delgado López-Cózar, E. Google Scholar, Microsoft Academic, Scopus, Dimensions, Web of Science, and OpenCitations’ COCI: A Multidisciplinary Comparison of Coverage via Citations. Scientometrics 2021, 126, 871–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merriam-Webster. Biophilia. 2020. Available online: https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/biophilia (accessed on 26 December 2020).
- Fromm, E. The Anatomy of Human Destructiveness; Holt, Rinehart and Winston: New York, NY, USA, 1974; Volume 3, ISBN 0030075963. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, E.O. Biophilia; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Beatley, T. Biophilic Cities: Integrating Nature into Design and Planning; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2011; ISBN 9781597267144. [Google Scholar]
- Totaforti, S. Emerging biophilic urbanism: The value of the human-nature relationship in the urban space. Sustainability 2020, 12, 5487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mehaffy, M.W.; Salingaros, N.A. Design for a Living Planet: Settlement, Science, and the Human Future; Sustasis Foundation: Portland, OR, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Park, S.H.; Mattson, R.H. Effects of flowering and foliage plants in hospital rooms on patients recovering from abdominal surgery. Horttechnology 2008, 18, 563–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Totaforti, S. Applying the benefits of biophilic theory to hospital design. City Territ. Arch. 2018, 5, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- The American Institute of Architects. Guidelines for Design and Construction of Hospital and Health Carev Facilities, 2001st ed.; The American Institute of Architects: Washington, DC, USA, 2001; Volume 22, ISBN 1571650024. [Google Scholar]
- Mygind, L.; Stevenson, M.P.; Liebst, L.S.; Konvalinka, I.; Bentsen, P. Stress response and cognitive performance modulation in classroom versus natural environments: A quasi-experimental pilot study with children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 1098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Peters, T.; D’Penna, K. Biophilic design for restorative university learning environments: A critical review of literature and design recommendations. Sustainability 2020, 12, 7064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, S.J.; Lee, H.C. Spatial design of childcare facilities based on biophilic design patterns. Sustainability 2019, 11, 2851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Orman, P. Understanding the Biophilia Hypothesis through a Comparative Analysis of Residential Typologies in Phoenix, São Paulo, and Tokyo. Master’s Thesis, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, E.J.; Park, S.J. A framework of smart-home service for elderly’s biophilic experience. Sustainability 2020, 12, 8572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenbaum, M.S.; Ramirez, G.C.; Camino, J.R. A dose of nature and shopping: The restorative potential of biophilic lifestyle center designs. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2018, 40, 66–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaplan, S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995, 15, 169–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joye, Y.; Pals, R.; Steg, L.; Evans, B.L. New Methods for Assessing the Fascinating Nature of Nature Experiences. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kellert, S.R.; Wilson, E.O. The Biophilia Hypothesis; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Kellert, S.R.; Calabrese, E.F. The Practice of Biophilic Design; Terrapin Bright LLC: London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Kellert, S.R.; Heerwagen, J.; Mador, M. Biophilic Design: The Theory, Science and Practice of Bringing Buildings to Life; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Kellert, S.R. Nature By Design: The Practice of Biophilic Design; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan, C.O.; Browning, W.D.; Clancy, J.O.; Andrews, S.L.; Kallianpurkar, N.B. Biophilic design patterns: Emerging nature-based parameters for health and well-being in the built environment. Archnet-IJAR 2014, 8, 62–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loomis, J.M.; Blascovich, J.J.; Beall, A.C. Immersive virtual environment technology as a basic research tool in psychology. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1999, 31, 557–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ellis, S.R. What Are Virtual Environments? IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl. 1994, 14, 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanney, K.M.; Mourant, R.R.; Kennedy, R.S. Human Factors Issues in Virtual Environments: A review of the literature. Presence Teleoper. Virtual Environ. 1998, 7, 327–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merriam-Webster. Virtual Reality. 2020. Available online: https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/virtualreality (accessed on 25 December 2020).
- Sanchez-Vives, M.V.; Slater, M. From presence to consciousness through virtual reality. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 6, 332–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heeter, C. Being There: The Subjective Experience of Presence. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 1992, 1, 262–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cummings, J.J.; Bailenson, J.N. How Immersive Is Enough? A Meta-Analysis of the Effect of Immersive Technology on User Presence. Media Psychol. 2016, 19, 272–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Witmer, B.G.; Singer, M.J. Measuring Presence in Virtual Environments: A Presence Questionnaire. Presence 1998, 7, 225–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biocca, F.; Harms, C.; Burgoon, J.K. Toward a More Robust Theory and Measure of Social Presence: Review and Suggested Criteria. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 2003, 12, 456–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, K.M. Why Presence Occurs: Evolutionary Psychology, Media Equation, and Presence. Presence 2004, 13, 494–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Held, R.M.; Durlach, N.I. Telepresence. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 1992, 1, 109–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, H.; Prothero, J.; Wells, M.J.; Groen, J. Virtual Chess: Meaning Enhances Users’ Sense of Presence in Virtual Environments. Int. J. Human Comput. Interact. 1998, 10, 251–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barfield, W.; Weghorst, S. The sense of presence within virtual environments: A conceptual framework. In Human-Computer Interaction: Software and Hardware Interface; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1993; pp. 699–704. [Google Scholar]
- Lombard, M.; Ditton, T. At the Heart of It All: The Concept of Presence. J. Comput. Commun. 1997, 3, JCMC321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lombard, M.; Reich, R.D.; Grabe, M.E.; Bracken, C.C.; Ditton, T.B. Presence and Television: The Role of Screen Size. Hum. Commun. Res. 2000, 26, 75–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chertoff, D.B.; Schatz, S.L.; McDaniel, R.; Bowers, C.A. Improving Presence Theory Through Experiential Design. Presence 2008, 17, 405–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pine, B.J.; Gilmore, J.H. The experience economy. Harv. Bus. Rev. 1998, 76, 18–23. [Google Scholar]
- Chertoff, D.B.; Goldiez, B.; LaViola, J.J. Virtual experience test: A virtual environment evaluation questionnaire. Proc. IEEE Virtual Real. 2010, 103–110. [Google Scholar]
- Muhanna, M.A. Virtual reality and the CAVE: Taxonomy, interaction challenges and research directions. J. King Saud Univ. -Comput. Inf. Sci. 2015, 27, 344–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mandal, S. Brief Introduction of Virtual Reality & its Challenges. Int. J. Sci. Eng. Res. 2013, 4, 304–309. [Google Scholar]
- Rohrer, M.W. Seeing is believing: The importance of visualization in manufacturing simulation. In Proceedings of the 2000 Winter Simulation Conference Proceedings (Cat. No.00CH37165), Orlando, FL, USA, 10–13 December 2000; Volume 2, pp. 1211–1216. [Google Scholar]
- LaValle, S. Virtual Reality; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017; ISBN 0436412128. [Google Scholar]
- Duarte, E.; Rebelo, F.; Wogalter, M.S. Virtual Reality and Its Potential for Evaluating Warning Compliance. Hum. Factors Ergon. Manuf. Serv. Ind. 2010, 20, 526–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dinh, H.Q.; Walker, N.; Hodges, L.F.; Song, C.; Kobayashi, A. Evaluating the importance of multi-sensory input on memory and the sense of presence in virtual environments. In Proceedings of the Proceedings-Virtual Reality Annual International Symposium, Houston, TX, USA, 13–17 March 1999; pp. 222–228. [Google Scholar]
- Hülsmann, F.; Fröhlich, J.; Mattar, N.; Wachsmuth, I. Wind and warmth in virtual reality: Implementation and evaluation. In Proceedings of the Virtual Reality International Conference - Laval Virtual, Laval, France, 9–13 April 2014; 2014; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Gaudina, M.; Brogni, A.; Caldwell, D. Irradiating heat in virtual environments: Algorithm and implementation. In Virtual and Mixed Reality-New Trends. VMR 2011. Lecture Notes in Computer Science; Shumaker, R., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2011; Volume 6773, pp. 194–203. [Google Scholar]
- Ho, H.-N.; Van Doorn, G.H.; Kawabe, T.; Watanabe, J.; Spence, C. Colour-temperature correspondences: When reactions to thermal stimuli are influenced by colour. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e91854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Balcer, C.A. Visual Cues Effects on Temperature Perception. Master’s Thesis, Northern Michigan University, Marquette, MI, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Congress of the United States. Virtual Reality and Technologies for Combat Simulation; Government Printing Office: Washington, DC, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Tzanavari, A.; Matsentidou, S.; Christou, C.G.; Poullis, C. User experience observations on factors that affect performance in a road-crossing training application for children using the CAVE. Lect. Notes Comput. Sci. 2014, 8524 LNCS, 91–101. [Google Scholar]
- Dörr, K.-U.; Schiefele, J.; Kubbat, W. Virtual Cockpit Simulation for Pilot Training. 2000. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/235033136_Virtual_Cockpit_Simulation_for_Pilot_Training (accessed on 1 February 2021).
- Roettl, J.; Terlutter, R. The same video game in 2D, 3D or virtual reality–How does technology impact game evaluation and brand placements? PLoS ONE 2018, 13, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zyda, M. From Visual to Virtual Reality to Games. IEEE Comput. Soc. 2005, 38, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuliga, S.F.; Thrash, T.; Dalton, R.C.; Hölscher, C. Virtual reality as an empirical research tool-Exploring user experience in a real building and a corresponding virtual model. Comput. Environ. Urban Syst. 2015, 54, 363–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhu, Y.; Saeidi, S.; Rizzuto, T.; Roetzel, A.; Kooima, R. Potential and challenges of immersive virtual environments for occupant energy behavior modeling and validation: A literature review. J. Build. Eng. 2018, 19, 302–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roupé, M.; Yao, J.; Khosrowshahi, F.; Fernando, T.; Skjærbæk, J. The impact of immersive virtual reality on visualisation for a design review in construction. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Information Visualisation, London, UK, 26–29 July 2010; pp. 585–589. [Google Scholar]
- Castronovo, F.; Nikolic, D.; Liu, Y.; Messner, J. An Evaluation Of Immersive Virtual Reality Systems For Design Reviews. In Proceedings of the 13th International Conference on Construction Applications of Virtual Reality, London, UK, 30–31 October 2013; pp. 22–29. [Google Scholar]
- Dunston, P.S.; Arns, L.L.; Mcglothlin, J.D.; Lasker, G.C.; Kushner, A.G. An immersive virtual reality mock-up for design review of hospital patient rooms. In Collaborative Design in Virtual Environments; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2011; pp. 167–176. ISBN 9400706049. [Google Scholar]
- Mackie, C.; Cowden, J.; Bowman, D.; Thabet, W. Desktop and immersive tools for residential home design. In Proceedings of the CONVR Conference on Construction Applications of Virtual Reality, Lisbon, Portugal, 14–15 September 2004; pp. 63–70. [Google Scholar]
- Paes, D.; Irizarry, J. Virtual Reality Technology Applied in the Building Design Process: Considerations on Human Factors and Cognitive Processes. Adv. Ergon. Des. 2016, 3–15. [Google Scholar]
- Paes, D.; Arantes, E.; Irizarry, J. Immersive environment for improving the understanding of architectural 3D models: Comparing user spatial perception between immersive and traditional virtual reality systems. Autom. Constr. 2017, 84, 292–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Portman, M.E.; Natapov, A.; Fisher-Gewirtzman, D. To go where no man has gone before: Virtual reality in architecture, landscape architecture and environmental planning. Comput. Environ. Urban Syst. 2015, 54, 376–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roupé, M. Development and Implementations of Virtual Reality for Decision-making in Urban Planning and Building Design. Ph.D. Thesis, Chalmers University of Technology, Göteborg, Sweden, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Tutt, D.; Harty, C.; Smith, S.D.; Ahiaga-Dagbui, D.D. Journeys through the CAVE: The use of 3D immersive environments for client engagement practices in hospital design. In Proceedings of the 29th Annual ARCOM Conference, Association of Researchers in Construction Management, Reading, UK, 2–4 September 2013; pp. 2–4. [Google Scholar]
- Schnabel, M.A.; Kvan, T.; Kruijff, E.; Donath, D. The First Virtual Environment Design Studio. In Proceedings of the 19th eCAADe Conference Proceedings, Helsinki, Finland, 29–31 August 2001; pp. 394–400. [Google Scholar]
- Natephra, W.; Motamedi, A.; Fukuda, T.; Yabuki, N. Integrating building information modeling and virtual reality development engines for building indoor lighting design. Vis. Eng. 2017, 5, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Heydarian, A.; Pantazis, E.; Carneiro, J.P.; Gerber, D.; Becerik-Gerber, B. Lights, building, action: Impact of default lighting settings on occupant behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016, 48, 212–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Messner, J.I.; Yerrapathuruni, S.C.M.; Baratta, A.J.; Whisker, V.E. Using virtual reality to improve construction engineering education. In Proceedings of the American Society for Engineering Education Annual Conference and Exposition, Nashville, TN, USA, 22–25 June 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Dzeng, R.-J.; Lin, C.-T.; Fang, Y.-C. Using eye-tracker to compare search patterns between experienced and notice workers for site hazard identification. Saf. Sci. 2016, 82, 56–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xie, H.; Tudoreanu, M.E.; Shi, W. Development of a Virtual Reality Safety-Training System for Construction Workers. In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Construction Applications of Virtual Reality (CONVR 2006), Orlando, FL, USA, 3–4 August 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Saeidi, S.; Zhu, Y.; Zadghorban Lifkoohee, M.; Mollazadeh, M. Co-Presence in a Shared Virtual Environment (SVE): A Case Study of Highway Work Zone Construction. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Construction and Real Estate Management, Banff, AB, Canada, 21–24 May 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Setareh, M.; Bowman, D.A.; Kalita, A. Development of a virtual reality structural analysis system. J. Archit. Eng. 2005, 11, 156–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, B.; Ellobody, E.; Hu, T.W.C. 3D Visualization of Structures Using Finite-Element Analysis in Teaching. J. Prof. Issues Eng. Educ. Pract. 2012, 138, 131–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chou, C.; Hsu, H.L.; Yao, Y.U.S. Construction of a Virtual Reality Learning Environment for Teaching Structural Analysis. Comput. Appl. Eng. Educ. 1997, 5, 223–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Kort, Y.A.W.; Meijnders, A.L.; Sponselee, A.A.G.; IJsselsteijn, W.A. What’s wrong with virtual trees? Restoring from stress in a mediated environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 309–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, M.P.; Yeo, N.L.; Vassiljev, P.; Lundstedt, R.; Wallergård, M.; Albin, M.; Lõhmus, M. A prescription for “nature”–The potential of using virtual nature in therapeutics. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2018, 14, 3001–3013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Berto, R. The role of nature in coping with psycho-physiological stress: A literature review on restorativeness. Behav. Sci. 2014, 4, 394–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brookes, J.; Warburton, M.; Alghadier, M.; Mon-Williams, M.; Mushtaq, F. Studying human behavior with virtual reality: The Unity Experiment Framework. Behav. Res. Methods 2020, 52, 455–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smith, J.W. Immersive virtual environment technology to supplement environmental perception, preference and behavior research: A review with applications. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 11486–11505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paljic, A. Ecological Validity of Virtual Reality: Three Use Cases. In International Conference on Image Analysis and Processing (ICIAP 2017), Lecture Notes in Computer Science; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; Volume 10590, pp. 301–310. [Google Scholar]
- Browning, M.H.E.M.; Mimnaugh, K.J.; van Riper, C.J.; Laurent, H.K.; LaValle, S.M. Can Simulated Nature Support Mental Health? Comparing Short, Single-Doses of 360-Degree Nature Videos in Virtual Reality With the Outdoors. Front. Psychol. 2020, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yeo, N.L.; White, M.P.; Alcock, I.; Garside, R.; Dean, S.G.; Smalley, A.J.; Gatersleben, B. What is the best way of delivering virtual nature for improving mood? An experimental comparison of high definition TV, 360◦ video, and computer generated virtual reality. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 72, 101500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reynolds, L.; Rogers, O.; Benford, A.; Ingwaldson, A.; Vu, B.; Holstege, T.; Alvarado, K. Virtual Nature as an Intervention for Reducing Stress and Improving Mood in People with Substance Use Disorder. J. Addict. 2020, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Appel, L.; Appel, E.; Bogler, O.; Wiseman, M.; Cohen, L.; Ein, N.; Abrams, H.B.; Campos, J.L. Older Adults With Cognitive and/or Physical Impairments Can Benefit From Immersive Virtual Reality Experiences: A Feasibility Study. Front. Med. 2020, 6, 329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sona, B.; Dietl, E.; Steidle, A. Recovery in sensory-enriched break environments: Integrating vision, sound and scent into simulated indoor and outdoor environments. Ergonomics 2019, 62, 521–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wang, X.; Shi, Y.; Zhang, B.; Chiang, Y. The influence of forest resting environments on stress using virtual reality. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hedblom, M.; Gunnarsson, B.; Iravani, B.; Knez, I.; Schaefer, M.; Thorsson, P.; Lundström, J.N. Reduction of physiological stress by urban green space in a multisensory virtual experiment. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9. [Google Scholar]
- Snell, T.L.; McLean, L.A.; McAsey, F.; Zhang, M.; Maggs, D. Nature Streaming: Contrasting the Effectiveness of Perceived Live and Recorded Videos of Nature for Restoration. Environ. Behav. 2019, 51, 1082–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gao, T.; Zhang, T.; Zhu, L.; Gao, Y.; Qiu, L. Exploring psychophysiological restoration and individual preference in the different environments based on virtual reality. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chirico, A.; Gaggioli, A. When Virtual Feels Real: Comparing Emotional Responses and Presence in Virtual and Natural Environments. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2019, 22, 220–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yu, C.P.; Lee, H.Y.; Luo, X.Y. The effect of virtual reality forest and urban environments on physiological and psychological responses. Urban For. Urban Green. 2018, 35, 106–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liszio, S.; Graf, L.; Masuch, M. The relaxing effect of virtual nature: Immersive technology provides relief in acute stress situations. Annu. Rev. Cyberther. Telemed. 2018, 16, 87–93. [Google Scholar]
- Tanja-Dijkstra, K.; Pahl, S.; White, M.P.; Auvray, M.; Stone, R.J.; Andrade, J.; May, J.; Mills, I.; Moles, D.R. The Soothing Sea: A Virtual Coastal Walk Can Reduce Experienced and Recollected Pain. Environ. Behav. 2018, 50, 599–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of visual stimulation with forest imagery. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Anderson, A.P.; Mayer, M.D.; Fellows, A.M.; Cowan, D.R.; Hegel, M.T.; Buckey, J.C. Relaxation with immersive natural scenes presented using virtual reality. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 2017, 88, 520–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerber, S.M.; Jeitziner, M.M.; Wyss, P.; Chesham, A.; Urwyler, P.; Müri, R.M.; Jakob, S.M.; Nef, T. Visuo-acoustic stimulation that helps you to relax: A virtual reality setup for patients in the intensive care unit. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Helmisaari, M. Interaction in virtual restorative environments: How do different possibilities to interact affect the perceived restorativeness of a virtual environment. Master’s Thesis, University of Skövde, Skövde, Sweden, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Annerstedt, M.; Jönsson, P.; Wallergård, M.; Johansson, G.; Karlson, B.; Grahn, P.; Hansen, Å.M.; Währborg, P. Inducing physiological stress recovery with sounds of nature in a virtual reality forest-Results from a pilot study. Physiol. Behav. 2013, 118, 240–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knight, J.F.; Stone, R.J.; Qian, C. Virtual restorative environments: Preliminary studies in scene, sound and smell. Int. J. Gaming Comput. Simul. 2012, 4, 73–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lassonde, K.A.; Gloth, C.A.; Borchert, K. Windowless Classrooms or a Virtual Window World: Does a Creative Classroom Environment Help or Hinder Attention? Teach. Psychol. 2012, 39, 262–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valtchanov, D.; Barton, K.R.; Ellard, C. Restorative effects of virtual nature settings. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2010, 13, 503–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valtchanov, D.; Ellard, C. Physiological and affective responses to immersion in virtual reality: Effects of nature and urban settings. J. Cyber Ther. Rehabil. 2010, 3, 359–373. [Google Scholar]
- Kahn, P.H.; Friedman, B.; Gill, B.; Hagman, J.; Severson, R.L.; Freier, N.G.; Feldman, E.N.; Carrère, S.; Stolyar, A. A plasma display window?-The shifting baseline problem in a technologically mediated natural world. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008, 28, 192–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berman, M.G.; Jonides, J.; Kaplan, S. The Cognitive Benefits of Interacting with Nature. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 1207–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kjellgren, A.; Buhrkall, H. A comparison of the restorative effect of a natural environment with that of a simulated natural environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartig, T.; Kaiser, F.G.; Bowler, P.A. Further Development of a Measure of Perceived Environmental Restorativeness; Working Paper; Institute for Housing Research: Gävle, Sweden, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Pasini, M.; Berto, R.; Brondino, M.; Hall, R.; Ortner, C. How to Measure the Restorative Quality of Environments: The PRS-11. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 159, 293–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Xiao, M.; Ge, H.; Khundrakpam, B.S.; Xu, J.; Bezgin, G.; Leng, Y.; Zhao, L.; Tang, Y.; Ge, X.; Jeon, S.; et al. Attention performance measured by attention network test is correlated with global and regional efficiency of structural brain networks. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fan, J.; McCandliss, B.D.; Sommer, T.; Raz, A.; Posner, M.I. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002, 14, 340–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmer, P.; Buttlar, B.; Halbeisen, G.; Walther, E.; Domes, G. Virtually stressed? A refined virtual reality adaptation of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) induces robust endocrine responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2019, 101, 186–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brooks, J.B.B.; Giraud, V.O.; Saleh, Y.J.; Rodrigues, S.J.; Daia, L.A.; Fragoso, Y.D. Paced auditory serial addition test (PASAT): A very difficult test even for individuals with high intellectual capability. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 2011, 69, 482–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brett, E. Architects in the Design of Virtual Reality Spaces; UC Berkeley: Berkeley, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Ângulo, A.; de Velasco, G.V. Immersive Simulation of Architectural Spatial Experiences. In Proceedings of the 17th Conference of the Iberoamerican Society of Digital Graphics, Valparaiso, Chile, 20–22 November 2013; pp. 495–499. [Google Scholar]
- Nikolic, D. Evaluating Relative Impact of Virtual Reality Components Detail and Realism on Spatial Comprehension and Presence. Ph.D. Thesis, Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Regenbrecht, H.; Schubert, T. Real and illusory interactions enhance presence in virtual environments. Presence Teleoper. Virtual Environ. 2002, 11, 425–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrett, J. Side Effects of Virtual Environments: A Review of the Literature; DSTO Information Sciences Laboratory: Edinburgh, Australia, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Y.Y.; Kim, H.J.; Kim, E.N.; Ko, H.D.; Kim, H.T. Characteristic changes in the physiological componentsof cybersickness. Psychophysiology 2005, 42, 616–625. [Google Scholar]
- Stanney, K.M.; Kingdon, K.S.; Graeber, D.; Kennedy, R.S. Human performance in immersive virtual environments: Effects of exposure duration, user control, and scene complexity. Hum. Perform. 2002, 15, 339–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weech, S.; Kenny, S.; Barnett-Cowan, M. Presence and cybersickness in virtual reality are negatively related: A review. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- de Dieuleveult, A.L.; Siemonsma, P.C.; van Erp, J.B.F.; Brouwer, A.-M. Effects of aging in multisensory integration: A systematic review. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2017, 9, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berard, J.; Fung, J.; Lamontagne, A. Impact of aging on visual reweighting during locomotion. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012, 123, 1422–1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kahn, P.H. The Human Relationship with Nature: Development and Culture; The IMT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Chan, C.-S. Design Cognition: Cognitive Science in Design; China Architecture and Building Press: Beijing, China, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Hill, D. How Virtual Reality Impacts the Landscape Architecture Design Process at Various Scales. Master’s Thesis, Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Saeidi, S.; Zhu, Y.; Choi, J.-H.; Zhong, C.; Wang, Q. Immersive Virtual Environment as an Apparatus for Occupant Behavior Studies. In Proceedings of the ConVR 2016, Hong Kong, China, 11–13 December 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Ford, C.G.; Manegold, E.M.; Randall, C.L.; Aballay, A.M.; Duncan, C.L. Assessing the feasibility of implementing low-cost virtual reality therapy during routine burn care. Burns 2018, 44, 886–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, K.; Rosenthal, M.Z.; Zielinski, D.J.; Brady, R. Effects of virtual environment platforms on emotional responses. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 2014, 113, 882–893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruiz, A.S.; Peralta-Ramirez, M.I.; Garcia-Rios, M.C.; Muñoz, M.A.; Navarrete-Navarrete, N.; Blazquez-Ortiz, A. Adaptation of the trier social stress test to virtual reality: Psycho-phsyiological and neuroendocrine modulation. J. Cyber Ther. Rehabil. 2010, 3, 405–415. [Google Scholar]
- Schebella, M.F.; Weber, D.; Schultz, L.; Weinstein, P. The nature of reality: Human stress recovery during exposure to biodiverse, multisensory virtual environments. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- van Vliet, E.; Dane, G.; Weijs-Perrée, M.; van Leeuwen, E.; van Dinter, M.; van den Berg, P.; Borgers, A.; Chamilothori, K. The influence of urban park attributes on user preferences: Evaluation of virtual parks in an online stated-choice experiment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowman, D.A.; McMahan, R.P. Virtual reality: How much immersion is enough? Computer 2007, 40, 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saeidi, S.; Rizzuto, T.; Zhu, Y.; Kooima, R. Measuring the Effectiveness of Immersive Virtual Environment for the Modeling and Prediction of Occupant Behavior. Sustain. Hum. Build. Ecosyst. 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Higuera-Trujillo, J.L.; López-Tarruella Maldonado, J.; Llinares Millán, C. Psychological and physiological human responses to simulated and real environments: A comparison between Photographs, 360° Panoramas, and Virtual Reality. Appl. Ergon. 2017, 65, 398–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, T.C.; Sit, C.H.P.; Tang, T.W.; Tsai, C.L. Psychological and physiological responses in patients with generalized anxiety disorder: The use of acute exercise and virtual reality environment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Category | Patterns | Description | Elements or Features |
---|---|---|---|
Natural Elements | Visual Connection with Nature [7] | View to natural environment and living systems (real or simulated) | Plants Animals Water Fire Habitats and ecosystems Image of nature Views and vistas |
Non-Visual Connection with Nature [7] | Sensory stimuli, except visual, that positively refer to living systems or nature | Smell of Plants Animal sounds Waterfall sound Fireplace sound | |
Light | Various intensities of real or simulated light and shadow similar to conditions that occur in nature | Natural light and shadow Filtered light Warm light Reflected light | |
Thermal and Airflow Variability [7] | Gentle changes in thermal and airflow variables that mimic nature (real or simulated) | Surface temperature Airflow (e.g., wind, breeze) Humidity | |
Natural Analogues | Natural material | Materials and elements from nature | Natural materials |
Natural Shapes and Forms [56] | A symbolic reference to forms and shapes that exist in the natural environment | Natural geometries Natural colors Biomimicry Biomorph | |
Natural Patterns and Processes [56] | A symbolic reference to patterns and processes that exist in the natural environment | Growth Change and age Patterned wholes Integration of parts to wholes Fractals Central focal point Transitional spaces Bounded spaces | |
Experience of Place and Space | Evolved Human–Nature Relationship [56] | Spatial configurations in the natural environment. It includes human’s desire to feel and experience beyond surroundings | Refuge and prospect Discovery and exploration Mastery Curiosity Protection and security Order and complexity |
Place-based Relationship [56] | Place attachment and individual’s natural for familiar places | Historical, cultural, geographic, or ecological connection to place Culture and ecology integration Landscape orientation Landscape features and ecology Indigenous materials Spirit of place |
Parameters of Biophilic Design Studies | General VE Capabilities and Limitations | VE Application for Biophilic Design Studies |
---|---|---|
Biophilic design categories (Natural elements Natural analogues Experience of space and place) | VE has wide applications in Architecture, Engineering, and Construction (AEC) research for visualization, design review, pre-occupancy evaluation, and decision making (e.g., [25,98,100,102]) | Implementing all three categories are reported in the literature (e.g., [32,34,119,124]). Natural analogues and experience of space and place are less reported in the literature, but it is technically feasible to implement. |
Biophilic design patterns | VE has wide application for visualization and design review in AEC. Simulating and representing various design elements, forms, and patterns are possible through features of game engines and VE input devices (e.g., [25,82,99,163]). | Generally, some biophilic patterns are less familiar to participants and less explicit to understand and implement compared to others. For example, participants felt more connected with nature in VE setting with natural light, indoor plants, or a view to nature compared to natural material and forms [32]. “Natural patterns and processes” is more psychological, required precise design, and less clear to participants than green natural elements or material. Therefore, this biophilic pattern is not reported in the literature; however, technically, there is no challenge for simulating this pattern in VE. Regarding thermal and airflow patterns, simulation in VE is also possible through thermal panels and climate chambers. Such simulations are reported in energy and occupant behavior studies [164]. However, comprehensive studies regarding thermal and airflow simulation are not reported in biophilic design studies. Therefore, there is potential for further research in this area. |
Biological responses (Physiological responses Psychological responses Cognitive responses) | Collecting human physiological, psychological, and cognitive data is possible in VE, especially in human-related studies. For quantifying human’s physiological responses to biophilic environments, there are a variety of wearable bio-monitoring sensors, such as heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV), blood pressure (BP), and skin conductance level (SCL). In addition, due to technology advancements, researchers can apply eye-tracking technology (e.g., Tobii Pro), which monitors participant’s attention toward specific parts of design during the exposure period (e.g., [26,107]). Regarding psychological and cognitive measurements in VE, the flexibility of VEs for supporting programming and on-screen surveys allows participants to be immersed in VE while providing answers to various psychological and/or cognitive tests [23]. On the other hand, a potential issue is the clumsiness of wired devices that can impact VE experiments. Therefore, streamlined apparatus is preferable [165]. Another challenge is that participant’s biological responses and task performance can be affected due to VE platform or display methods [166]. Furthermore, possible cybersickness may also negatively impact the user’s task performance and biological responses. | Eye-tracking technology in combination with VE and bio-monitoring sensors is an effective research tool for biophilic design [32]. |
Stress induction Mental fatigue | As mentioned above, on-screen tests and programming in VEs enable scholars to create various tests, including mental fatigue tests in VEs. Moreover, increasing stress-inducing tasks is also reported in the literature. For example, VR-Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) is a confirmed tool in stress research (e.g., [149,167]). | According to the two fundamental biophilic theories, Attention Restoration Theory (ART) and Stress Recovery Theory (SRT), in many biophilic studies, researchers consider stress induction or mental fatigue through different tests and tasks. VR-TSST is stress inducing task in VE that also has been reported in biophilic studies (e.g., [130,168]). Therefore, VE can create stress induction required for some biophilic studies. |
Natural environment setting (Indoor and outdoor built environment Urban space Wild nature) | The modeling and programming capability of VE software such as Unity and Unreal Engine as well as 360-degree videos or panoramic images of a real environment support researchers to create or represent various natural environmental settings in VE (e.g., [33,34,120,124,135,137]). | Various natural environment settings in VE are reported in the literature, including empirical studies on the indoor environment (e.g., [31,33]), outdoor environment such as shopping mall plaza (e.g., [51]), large scale urban park (e.g., [23,169]), etc. There is a limited number of studies that quantify the physiological and cognitive benefits of indoor biophilic elements and patterns. It is recommended to also apply virtual biophilic environment stimulation to other indoor settings, such as education, housing, recreation, and retail |
VE Sensory inputs (Visual Auditory Olfactory Tactile Thermal and wind) | VE supports visual, auditory, olfactory, tactile inputs. As mentioned, realistic experience in VE and, consequently, successful application of VE depends on sensory fidelity, auditory, olfactory, and other sensory cues similar to reality [170]. Realistic experience in VE, high level of immersion, and presence are related concepts that have been explored over the years. For example, in [82], a multimodal VE via visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory stimuli is provided. However, technically, there are still limitations for generating auditory output in real-time with dynamic changes [63], for example. In addition, haptic stimulation is possible but in a restricted domain of applications. | Diverse combinations of sensory stimuli have been studied for biophilic design in VEs. As mentioned above, non-visual connection with nature is a biophilic design pattern. It is recommended to design for simultaneous visual and non-visual connections to nature [7], which increase positive health impacts and facilitate stress reduction. Therefore, sensory inputs improve immersion and the individual’s presence in VE, which contributes to biophilia and biophilic design. Although there are some research studies with multimodal cues (e.g., [123,125,137,168]), most studies incorporate only auditory and visual cues. To illustrate, an actual biophilic environment like a forest incorporates five senses as well as air, temperature, humidity, etc., while most VE studies only focus on visual and auditory stimulation. Therefore, there are opportunities to stimulate and engage the more senses in VE and further study individuals’ biological responses in relation to biophilic theories, immersion, and presence. |
VE validation VE presence (Single user vs. multi-user) | Similar results and effects in exposure to virtual vs. actual natural environment are reported in the literature (e.g., [31,92,171]). Under controlled laboratory circumstances, VEs can provide more immersive experiences and more realistic physiological responses, comparing to mediated environments [172]. Regarding VE presence, the majority of the studies are designed for a single user, although multi-user experiments are technically possible. | Empirical studies show consistent biological responses to biophilic design in the real-world and 360-degree video in VE (VE validation) [31]. However, in a study [33], cognitive performance in biophilic design in VE is not consistent with that in-situ. More studies are required. |
Subjects and sample size | The sample size in most VE studies are small, e.g., less than 200 [93]. Regarding subjects in different domains, scholars have studied participants with a diverse range of ages and backgrounds in VEs. | To be able to generalize research results to the general population, it is necessary to include a variety of participants with representative samples. For example, individual differences, such as gender, childhood experiences, have an impact on participant’s responses to biophilic environments and biophilic design (e.g., [9,23,32]). Moreover, currently, most research studies include healthy users; there are very few studies on users with physical or cognitive impairment (e.g., [121,122,143,173]). More in-depth research is recommended in order to explore virtual biophilic environment’s impact on individuals with different demographic characteristics and backgrounds. |
Exposure duration | VE research studies typically have short experiment sessions to avoid possible issues, such as cybersickness. | Based on empirical research, effective exposure to biophilic environments for stress reduction, positive emotions, and restoration can be 5 to 20 min [7]. Therefore, VE, specifically VR technology, is capable of supporting the required exposure time for effective biophilic studies. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Mollazadeh, M.; Zhu, Y. Application of Virtual Environments for Biophilic Design: A Critical Review. Buildings 2021, 11, 148. https://doi.org/10.3390/buildings11040148
Mollazadeh M, Zhu Y. Application of Virtual Environments for Biophilic Design: A Critical Review. Buildings. 2021; 11(4):148. https://doi.org/10.3390/buildings11040148
Chicago/Turabian StyleMollazadeh, Maryam, and Yimin Zhu. 2021. "Application of Virtual Environments for Biophilic Design: A Critical Review" Buildings 11, no. 4: 148. https://doi.org/10.3390/buildings11040148
APA StyleMollazadeh, M., & Zhu, Y. (2021). Application of Virtual Environments for Biophilic Design: A Critical Review. Buildings, 11(4), 148. https://doi.org/10.3390/buildings11040148