Use of the Milan Pet Quality of Life Instrument (MPQL) to Measure Pets’ Quality of Life during COVID-19
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.1.1. Inclusion Criteria
2.1.2. Exclusion Criteria
2.2. Survey
2.2.1. Demographic Data about the Participant
2.2.2. Questions Relative to the COVID-19 Pandemic
2.2.3. Human Personality (RST-Human)
2.2.4. Pet Demographics
2.2.5. Personality of the Pet (RST-Pet)
2.2.6. Quality of Life of the Pet (Milan Pet Quality of Life, MPQL)
2.2.7. Pet–Owner Relationship (LAPS)
2.3. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Subjects
3.2. Descriptive Results
3.3. Pets’ Characteristics
3.4. Factor Analysis and Questionnaire Validation
Pets’ Quality of Life during COVID-19
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Demographic Questions about the Participant
- -
- Age
- -
- Gender
- -
- Nationality
- -
- State of residence at the time of participation
- -
- Household composition
- -
- Relationship status
- ○
- Single
- ○
- Married/Living as married
- ○
- Separated/Divorced
- ○
- Widowed
- ○
- In relationship but not living together
- -
- Level of education
- -
- Geographical characteristics of their living area:
- ○
- Urban and suburban area, defined as an agglomeration of buildings with a high density of population and infrastructures, such as cities, towns, metropolises, and metropolitan areas
- ○
- Rural area, defined as an area characterised by low density of population and buildings or infrastructures, such as agricultural areas, forests, etc.
- -
- Type of residence: apartment, house, cottage, etc., with or without garden, farm with private fields
- -
- How the responder perceived the size of their home: small, medium, large
- -
- Whether they had unrestricted access to outdoor space (yes/no).
Group | |||
---|---|---|---|
Non-Pet Owners % (N) | Pet Owners % (N) | Total % (N) | |
Age (yrs) | |||
18–24 | 11 (6) | 8 (18) | 8 (24) |
25–34 | 20 (11) | 27 (63) | 25 (74) |
35–44 | 39 (22) | 27 (64) | 30 (86) |
45–54 | 9 (5) | 22 (52) | 20 (57) |
55–64 | 16 (9) | 13 (31) | 14 (40) |
65–74 | 5 (3) | 2 (6) | 3 (9) |
75–84 | - | - | - |
>84 | - | 1 (1) | 0 (1) |
Gender | |||
Female | 68 (38) | 89 (210) | 85 (248) |
Male | 32 (18) | 10 (23) | 14 (41) |
Other/Prefer not to say | - | 1 (2) | 1 (2) |
Relationship status | |||
In a relationship and co-living | 43 (24) | 56 (132) | 54 (156) |
In a relationship and not co-living | 16 (9) | 13 (30) | 13 (39) |
Not in a relationship | 41(23) | 31 (73) | 33 (96) |
Level of education | |||
Up to secondary education | 16 (9) | 22 (51) | 21 (60) |
Tertiary education or above | 84 (47) | 78 (184) | 79 (231) |
Nationality | |||
Australia | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Brazil | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Bulgaria | - | - | - |
Canada | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Finland | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
France | 3 (2) | 0.5 (1) | 1 (3) |
Germany | - | 1 (3) | 1 (3) |
India | 2 (1) | - | 0.5 (1) |
Italy | 89 (48) | 77 (179) | 79 (227) |
Norway | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Poland | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Singapore | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
South Africa | - | 1 (2) | 1 (2) |
Spain | - | - | - |
Sweden | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Switzerland | 2 (1) | 1 (3) | 1 (4) |
Ukraine | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
United Kingdom | 2 (1) | 5 (12) | 4 (13) |
United States | 2 (1) | 10 (23) | 8 (24) |
Residency at time of participation | |||
Australia | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Brazil | - | - | |
Bulgaria | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Canada | - | 1 (2) | 1 (2) |
Finland | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
France | 2 (1) | 0.5 (1) | 1 (2) |
Germany | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
India | 4 (2) | - | 1 (2) |
Italy | 83 (45) | 75 (177) | 77 (222) |
Norway | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Poland | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Singapore | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
South Africa | - | 0.5 (1) | 1 (1) |
Spain | - | 0.5 (1) | 0.5 (1) |
Sweden | - | 0.5 (1) | 2 (7) |
Switzerland | 4 (2) | 2 (5) | 0.5 (1) |
United Kingdom | 6 (3) | 6 (15) | 6 (18) |
United States | 2 (1) | 10 (24) | 9 (25) |
Ukraine | - | - | - |
Language | |||
Italian | 89 (50) | 80 (187) | 81 (237) |
British English | 11 (6) | 20 (48) | 19 (54) |
Living situation | |||
Geographical area | |||
Urban and suburban | 93 (52) | 72 (169) | 76 (221) |
Rural | 7 (4) | 28 (66) | 24 (70) |
Size of home | |||
Small | 30 (17) | 15 (34) | 18 (51) |
Medium/Large | 70 (39) | 85 (201) | 82 (240) |
Unrestricted access to outdoors | |||
Yes | 45 (25) | 67 (158) | 62 (183) |
No | 55 (31) | 33 (77) | 37 (108) |
Private outdoor access | |||
None | 14 (8) | 11 (27) | 12 (35) |
Balcony | 71 (40) | 41 (97) | 47 (137) |
Garden or field | 14 (8) | 47 (111) | 41 (119) |
COVID-19 | |||
Perceived risk of contracting COVID-19 | |||
Low | 45 (25) | 43 (102) | 44 (127) |
Medium–high | 55 (31) | 56 (133) | 56 (164) |
Illness | |||
Yes | 16 (9) | 17 (39) | 16 (48) |
No | 84 (47) | 83 (196) | 84 (243) |
Current lockdown level | |||
No measures | - | 3 (6) | 2 (6) |
Social distancing | 71 (40) | 76 (179) | 75 (219) |
National lockdown | 29 (16) | 21 (50) | 23 (66) |
Limitations to freedom of movement | |||
None/Minimal limitations | 13 (7) | 28 (65) | 25 (72) |
Extensive limitations | 87 (49) | 72 (170) | 75 (219) |
Financial distress | |||
None/Slight financial distress | 86 (48) | 80 (188) | 81 (236) |
Significant financial distress | 14 (8) | 20 (47) | 19 (55) |
Comparison of wellbeing | |||
Worse than before COVID-19 | 46 (26) | 38 (90) | 40 (116) |
As before COVID-19 | 41 (23) | 46 (108) | 45 (131) |
Better than before COVID-19 | 12 (7) | 16 (37) | 15 (44) |
- (1)
- Completely isolated (i.e., they cannot leave their home due to quarantine or because of lockdown measures)
- (2)
- Very limited (i.e., their country had issued some restrictions to movements, but they could still leave their home and/or had some opportunities to meet other people, for example, when they go to the supermarket or the pharmacy, when they exercise or walk their dog/pet, or when they care for a relative or similar)
- (3)
- Several limitations (i.e., regardless of the nationally issued restrictions, they need to leave their home regularly and therefore interact regularly with other people because their job is listed as “essential”)
- (4)
- Some small limitations (i.e., their country had not issued formal restrictions to the movement of people, however, there are social distancing rules, e.g., a 2 m distance between people or a limit to the size of gatherings)
- (5)
- No limitations (i.e., there is not any limitation in their country)
- (1)
- Yes, completely (e.g., they had lost their job, or their salary, and they had no other income)
- (2)
- Yes, very (e.g., they had a job and/or they had government support, however, their income was reduced)
- (3)
- Yes, partly (e.g., they could still work remotely, however, their income was reduced)
- (4)
- Not at all (e.g., they are essential workers and/or they could work remotely, therefore their income had not changed)
Appendix B. Demographic Questions about the Pet
- (1)
- Registered breeder
- (2)
- Unregistered breeder
- (3)
- Pet shop/found on the internet
- (4)
- Friends/relatives/born at home
- (5)
- Found in the streets
- (6)
- Shelter/rescuer
- (1)
- 0–60 days
- (2)
- 60–70 days (8–10 weeks)
- (3)
- 71–90 days (10–13 weeks)
- (4)
- 91–150 days (3–5 months)
- (5)
- 6 months–1 year
- (6)
- More than 1 year.
- (1)
- Company
- (2)
- Is a family member
- (3)
- He/she was in need of a home
- (4)
- Guard/keeping away pests and wild animals
- (5)
- Protection/police dog (dogs only)
- (6)
- Hunting/herding (dogs only)
- (7)
- Sport (dogs only)
- (8)
- Assistance to myself (emotional support, person with disabilities, pet therapy/animal-assisted interventions)
- (9)
- Assistance to someone I work or volunteer with, outside my home and household (emotional support, person with disabilities, pet therapy/animal-assisted interventions).
Loadings | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Physical | Psychological | Environmental | Social | |
How would you describe your pet’s general behaviour? (score from multiple options regarding pet’s mobility) | 0.59 | −0.15 | −0.12 | −0.02 |
To the best of your knowledge, based on your pet behaviour, which of the following describes best your pet’s eyesight? | 0.56 | −0.04 | 0.01 | −0.02 |
To the best of your knowledge, based on your pet behaviour, which of the following describes best your pet’s hearing? | 0.60 | 0.05 | 0.05 | −0.08 |
How often during the last 15 days did you see your pet using or playing alone with toys (e.g., balls, ropes, sticks, kongs, stuffed animals, squeaky toys) or playing with other people (with or without toys). (Playing alone) | 0.60 | 0.17 | 0.7 | 0.10 |
How often during the last 15 days did you see your pet using or playing alone with toys (e.g., balls, ropes, sticks, kongs, stuffed animals, squeaky toys) or playing with other people (with or without toys). (Playing with people) | 0.45 | 0.16 | 0.03 | 0.06 |
Is your pet receiving treatment for their conditions? (score from multiple options) | −0.32 | 0.20 | 0.21 | −0.13 |
Thinking about the last 15 days, did your pet get along with other animals? If your pet was never in contact with other pets in the last 15 days or in general, please choose N/A. (Other animals of the same species as my pet) | 0.25 | −0.06 | 0.05 | 0.09 |
In the past 15 days, did your pet show any of the following behaviours? (score from multiple options of negative affect) | 0.03 | −0.57 | 0.19 | 0.01 |
When your pet is left alone, does he/she do any of the following? (score from multiple options) | 0.11 | 0.35 | −0.01 | 0.04 |
In the last 15 days, how often would you say that your pet slept poorly (i.e., was restless or whining, etc.) | −0.02 | 0.33 | 0.15 | −0.03 |
How often during the last 15 days did your pet have a procedure done that he/she finds uncomfortable done to him/her? For example, trimming nails, bathing, having an injection, administering pills, etc. | −0.18 | 0.33 | 0.00 | 0.02 |
In the last 15 days, how often was your pet in contact with anyone else who causes him/her distress? (e.g., the mailman, a neighbour) | 0.06 | 0.31 | 0.01 | 0.02 |
Would you say that in the last 15 days your pet was interested in his/her food? | 0.04 | −0.27 | −0.13 | 0.26 |
In the last 15 days, did you find that taking care of your pet was economically challenging? Think in terms of food quality, toys and tools, medical bills. | 0.08 | −0.29 | −0.01 | −0.08 |
In the last 15 days, was your pet bothered by medical (physical) issues, such as: skin conditions (their skin is itchy, irritated, infected, he/she has ear irritation, irritation or lumps in the mouth, etc.), bladder and/or bowel control issues (i.e., struggling with urinating/defecating or unvoluntary urination/defecation, vomiting and/or diarrhoea, seizures). Note, this does not refer to problems with house training or when dogs urinate when excited/submissive | 0.25 | −0.29 | −0.10 | 0.01 |
Please think if there is anything else or any other event that causes your pet distress. For example, things like thunderstorms, loud noises, going to the vet’s clinic, etc. How often during the last 15 days would you say that your pet has been distressed by any of these things? | −0.03 | 0.29 | −0.01 | −0.08 |
It is not unusual for pets to misbehave every once in a while. For example, they may chew up a pair of shoes or jump up to take food off of the table. When pets do things they are not supposed to, it is not uncommon for people to correct the pet in some way. For example, to scold the pet, correct the pet physically with a swat or confine the pet to a kennel or a room. In the last 15 days, how often have you scolded your pet or physically corrected their behaviour? | 0.17 | 0.28 | 0.11 | −0.021 |
During the last 15 days, how often was your pet able to freely move between indoors and outdoors? (e.g., you leave doors/windows or a pet door open) | −0.01 | 0.05 | −0.63 | 0.02 |
Which of the following statements best describes your pet’s situation when he/she was OUTDOORS UNDER SOMEONE’S SUPERVISION? This does not include taking a dog for daily walks. For example, it could be when you and your pet were in the yard for whatever reason, or when you and your pet were visiting the local park. (If more than one option applies, choose the most common scenario.) (score from multiple options) | −0.02 | −0.06 | 0.47 | 0.07 |
Which of the following statements best describes your pet’s situation when he/she was OUTDOORS WITHOUT SUPERVISION in the last 15 days? (If more than one option applies, choose the most common scenario.) | 0.04 | −0.02 | 0.44 | 0.19 |
When alone, where or how was your pet confined? | −0.03 | 0.01 | −0.39 | 0.17 |
In the last 15 days, when your pet was OUTSIDE ON HIS/HER OWN, did he/she have access to shelter (e.g., an enclosed porch or dog house)? | −0.03 | 0.05 | −0.22 | −0.07 |
Thinking about the last 15 days, how often did your pet engage in obedience training, competitive sport, or other training activities (tricks training, dog dance, etc.)? (dog) Thinking about the last 15 days, how often did your pet engage in obedience training, or other training activities (trick training, cat agility, etc.)? (cat) | −0.02 | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.57 |
In the past 15 days, did your pet show any of the following behaviours? (score from multiple options of positive affect) | −0.19 | −0.23 | −0.00 | −0.32 |
During the last 15 days, was your pet fed their meals at the same time? | −0.02 | −0.03 | 0.04 | 0.28 |
During the last 15 days, on average per day, how many hours did you and/or other household members spend with the pet? For example, this would include things like playing with them, petting them, talking to them, or having them in the same room while you are asleep or watching TV. | −0.04 | 0.10 | 0.19 | 0.27 |
- -
- In general, does your pet suffered from reproductive problems? For example, false pregnancies or loss of appetite/nervous when in heat (if female) or when other animals nearby are in heat (if male).
- -
- In the last 15 days, when your pet has been INDOORS (e.g., in your home) while SOMEONE WAS WITH HIM/HER, how much access did he/she have?
- -
- Over the last 15 days, in a typical day, that is, a 24 h period, what was the longest period that your pet was left alone?
- -
- Thinking about the last 15 days, has your pet spent time with any HOUSEHOLD MEMBER he/she does NOT get along with. That is, when your pet physically interacted with them or was simply in their presence, he/she was restless and/or anxious and/or aggressive.
- -
- In the last 15 days, how was your pet with STRANGERS and/or REGULAR VISITORS? That is, while physically interacting with them or simply being in their presence.
- -
- Thinking about the last 15 days, did your pet get along with other animals? If your pet was never in contact with other pets in the last 15 days or in general, please choose N/A. (Other animals of other species)
- -
- For different reasons, it’s not always possible to take pets for walks or allow them outside every day. During the last 15 days, how often would you say that your pet has been taken for a walk and/or was allowed outside?
Variable | Mean | SD | Skewness | Kurtosis | Shapiro–Wilk Normality Test |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
RST-PQ | |||||
BAS-Interest | 19.06 | 3.94 | −0.15 | −0.36 | 0.99 ** |
BAS-Persistence | 20.47 | 3.99 | −0.19 | −0.52 | 0.98 *** |
BAS-Reactivity | 26.71 | 4.94 | −0.12 | −0.29 | 0.99, n.s. |
BAS-Impulsivity | 17.81 | 4.09 | 0.38 | −0.17 | 0.98 *** |
BIS | 55.21 | 13.83 | 0.11 | −0.74 | 0.98 ** |
FFFS | 24.96 | 5.79 | −0.22 | −0.46 | 0.98 ** |
RST-Cat | |||||
BAS | 3.24 | 0.70 | −0.06 | −1.04 | 0.96 * |
BIS | 3.24 | 0.87 | −0.61 | −0.04 | 0.96 * |
FFFS | 3.19 | 0.75 | −0.16 | 0.3 | 0.99, n.s. |
RST-Dog | |||||
BAS | 3.80 | 0.76 | −0.54 | −0.41 | 0.96 *** |
BIS | 2.90 | 0.99 | −0.13 | −0.94 | 0.97 * |
FFFS | 2.49 | 0.85 | 0.26 | −0.07 | 0.98 * |
MPQL-Cat | |||||
Physical | 85 | 9.15 | −0.28 | −0.48 | 0.96 * |
Emotional | 94 | 12.90 | −0.15 | −0.76 | 0.97 *** |
Social | 60 | 13.84 | −0.74 | 0.09 | 0.93 *** |
Environmental | 66 | 7.83 | 0.58 | −0.23 | 0.94 ** |
MPQL-Dog | |||||
Physical | 87 | 10.54 | −0.80 | 0.15 | 0.93 *** |
Emotional | 94 | 12.82 | −0.19 | −0.68 | 0.96 ** |
Social | 63 | 10.26 | −0.37 | −0.25 | 0.95 *** |
Environmental | 73 | 9.62 | −0.43 | −0.19 | 0.96 *** |
LAPS | |||||
General | 36.49 | 3.99 | −0.7 | −0.31 | 0.93 *** |
Animal Rights | 16.39 | 1.16 | −0.74 | 0.68 | 0.88 *** |
People Substitute | 21.15 | 4.35 | −0.2 | −0.84 | 0.97 *** |
References
- Maugeri, G.; Castrogiovanni, P.; Battaglia, G.; Pippi, R.; D’Agata, V.; Palma, A.; Di Rosa, M.; Musumeci, G. The impact of physical activity on psychological health during covid-19 pandemic in Italy. Heliyon 2020, 6, e04315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Dobbenburgh, R.; De Briyne, N. Impact of Covid-19 on animal welfare. Vet. Rec. 2020, 187, e31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowen, J.; García, E.; Darder, P.; Argüelles, J.; Fatjó, J. The Effects of the Spanish COVID-19 lockdown on people, their pets, and the human-animal bond. J. Vet. Behav. 2020, 40, 75–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García Pinillos, R. One welfare impacts of COVID19—A summary of key highlights within the one welfare framework. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 236, 105262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNicholas, J.; Collis, G.M. Dogs as catalysts for social interactions: Robustness of the effect. Br. J. Psychol. 2000, 91, 61–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNicholas, J.; Gilbey, A.; Rennie, A.; Ahmedzai, S.; Dono, J.-A.; Ormerod, E. Pet ownership and human health: A brief review of evidence and issues. BMJ 2005, 331, 1252–1254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Saunders, J.; Parast, L.; Babey, S.H.; Miles, J.V. Exploring the differences between pet and non-pet owners: Implications for human-animal interaction research and policy. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0179494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratschen, E.; Shoesmith, E.; Shahab, L.; Silva, K.; Kale, D.; Toner, P.; Reeve, C.; Mills, D.S. Human-animal relationships and interactions during the Covid-19 lockdown phase in the UK: Investigating links with mental health and loneliness. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0239397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yeates, J.; Main, D. Assessment of companion animal quality of life in veterinary practice and research. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2009, 50, 274–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Belshaw, Z.; Asher, L.; Harvey, N.D.; Dean, R. Quality of life assessment in domestic dogs: An evidence-based rapid review. Vet. J. 2015, 206, 203–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- WHOQOL Group. Study protocol for the World Health Organisation project to develop a quality of life assessment instrument (WHOQOL). Qual. Life Res. 1993, 2, 153–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verdugo, M.A.; Schalock, R.L.; Keith, K.D.; Stancliffe, R.J. Quality of life and its measurement: Important principles and guidelines. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2005, 49, 707–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Corr, P.J. The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. In The Cambridge Handbook of Personality Psychology; Corr, P.J., Matthews, G., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2008; pp. 347–376. [Google Scholar]
- Satchell, L.P.; Bacon, A.M.; Firth, J.L.; Corr, P.J. Risk as reward: Reinforcement sensitivity theory and psychopathic personality perspectives on everyday risk-taking. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 128, 162–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harper, C.A.; Satchell, L.P.; Fido, D.; Latzman, R.D. Functional fear predicts public health compliance in the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2020, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jagoe, A.; Serpell, J. Owner characteristics and interactions and the prevalence of canine behaviour problems. Hum. Anim. Interact. 1996, 47, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arhant, C.; Bubna-Littitz, H.; Bartels, A.; Futschik, A.; Troxler, J. Behaviour of smaller and larger dogs: Effects of training methods, inconsistency of owner behaviour and level of engagement in activities with the dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 123, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finka, L.R.; Ward, J.; Farnworth, M.J.; Mills, D.S. Owner personality and the wellbeing of their cats share parallels with the parent-child relationship. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0211862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, S.S.; Finka, L.; Mills, D.S. A systematic scoping review: What is the risk from child-dog interactions to dog’s quality of life? J. Vet. Behav. 2019, 33, 16–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foreman-Worsley, R.; Farnworth, M.J. A systematic review of social and environmental factors and their implications for indoor cat welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 220, 104841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellis, S.L.H.; Rodan, I.; Carney, H.C.; Heath, S.; Rochlitz, I.; Shearburn, L.D.; Sundahl, E.; Westropp, J.L. AAFP and ISFM feline environmental needs guidelines. J. Feline Med. Surg. 2013, 15, 219–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adamelli, S.; Marinelli, L.; Normando, S.; Bono, G. Owner and cat features influence the quality of life of the cat. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005, 94, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, T.R.; Lyons, J.B.; Tetrick, M.A.; Accortt, E.E. Multidimensional quality of life and human–animal bond measures for companion dogs. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2010, 5, 287–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marinelli, L.; Adamelli, S.; Normando, S.; Bono, G. Quality of life of the pet dog: Influence of owner and dog’s characteristics. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 108, 143–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mullan, S. Assessment of quality of life in veterinary practice: Developing tools for companion animal carers and veterinarians. Vet. Med. Res. Rep. 2015, 203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wojciechowska, J.I.; Hewson, C.J.; Stryhn, H.; Guy, N.C.; Patronek, G.J.; Timmons, V. Development of a discriminative questionnaire to assess nonphysical aspects of quality of life of dogs. Am. J. Vet. Res. 2005, 66, 1453–1460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piotti, P.; Satchell, L.P.; Lockhart, T.S. Impulsivity and behaviour problems in dogs: A reinforcement sensitivity theory perspective. Behav. Process. 2018, 151, 104–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dodman, N.H.; Brown, D.C.; Serpell, J.A. Associations between owner personality and psychological status and the prevalence of canine behavior problems. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0192846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leue, A.; Beauducel, A. A meta-analysis of reinforcement sensitivity theory: On performance parameters in reinforcement tasks. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 12, 353–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corr, P.J.; Cooper, A.J. The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality questionnaire (rst-pq): Development and validation. Psychol. Assess. 2016, 28, 1427–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanchard, D.; Hynd, A.; Minke, K.; Minemoto, T.; Blanchard, R. Human defensive behaviors to threat scenarios show parallels to fear- and anxiety-related defense patterns of non-human mammals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2001, 25, 761–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perkins, A.M.; Corr, P.J. Reactions to threat and personality: Psychometric differentiation of intensity and direction dimensions of human defensive behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 2006, 169, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pirrone, F.; Pierantoni, L.; Mazzola, S.M.; Vigo, D.; Albertini, M. Owner and animal factors predict the incidence of, and owner reaction toward, problematic behaviors in companion dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 10, 295–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pirrone, F.; Pierantoni, L.; Pastorino, G.Q.; Albertini, M. Owner-reported aggressive behavior towards familiar people may be a more prominent occurrence in pet shop-traded dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 11, 13–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, T.P.; Garrity, T.F.; Stallones, L. Psychometric evaluation of the lexington attachment to pets scale (Laps). Anthrozoos 1992, 5, 160–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Testoni, I.; De Cataldo, L.; Ronconi, L.; Zamperini, A. Pet loss and representations of death, attachment, depression, and euthanasia. Anthrozoös 2017, 30, 135–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; RDC Team: Vienna, Austria, 2020; ISBN 3-900051-07-0. [Google Scholar]
- Revelle, W. Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research; Northwestern University: Evanston, IL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Bernaards, C.A.; Jennrich, R.I. Gradient Projection Algorithms and Software for Arbitrary Rotation Criteria in Factor Analysis. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2005, 65, 676–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosseel, Y. An R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bates, D.; Machler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using Lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuznetsova, A.; Brockhoff, P.B.; Christensen, R.H.B. ImerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 82, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Gregorio, J.; Lee, J.-W. Education and income inequality: New evidence from cross-country data. Rev. Income Wealth 2002, 48, 395–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pierantoni, L.; Albertini, M.; Pirrone, F. Prevalence of owner-reported behaviours in dogs separated from the litter at two different ages. Vet. Rec. 2011, 169, 468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lowell, K.J.; Delgado, M.M.; Mederos, S.L.; Bain, M.J. The effect of premature maternal separation on distress vocalizations and activity in kittens (felis catus) during a brief nest separation. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020, 232, 105130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corr, P.J.; McNaughton, N. Neural Mechanisms, low trait anxiety, risk, externalizing behaviour. In Oxford Library of Psychology. The Oxford Handbook of Externalizing Spectrum Disorders; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2016; pp. 220–238. [Google Scholar]
- Serrano-Ibáñez, E.R.; Ramírez-Maestre, C.; López-Martínez, A.E.; Esteve, R.; Ruiz-Párraga, G.T.; Jensen, M.P. Behavioral inhibition and activation systems, and emotional regulation in individuals with chronic musculoskeletal pain. Front. Psychiatry 2018, 9, 394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Corsetti, S.; Borruso, S.; Traglia, M.D.; Lai, O.; Alfieri, L.; Villavecchia, A.; Cariola, G.; Spaziani, A.; Natoli, E. Bold personality makes domestic dogs entering a shelter less vulnerable to diseases. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0193794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Burman, O. Do Dogs Show an Optimistic or Pessimistic Attitude to Life?: A Review of Studies Using the “Cognitive Bias” Paradigm to Assess Dog Welfare; Elsevier Inc.: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; ISBN 978-0-12-407818-5. [Google Scholar]
- Savalli, C.; Albuquerque, N.; Vasconcellos, A.S.; Ramos, D.; de Mello, F.T.; Mills, D.S. Assessment of emotional predisposition in dogs using PANAS (Positive and Negative Activation Scale) and associated relationships in a sample of dogs from Brazil. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 18386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Corr, P.J.A. Grayïs Reinforcement sensitivity theory: Test of the joint subsystems hypotesis of anxiety and impulsivity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2002, 33, 511–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, J.M.; Lee, J.; Fitzgerald, H.N.; Oosterhoff, B.; Sevi, B.; Shook, N.J. Job insecurity and financial concern during the COVID-19 pandemic are associated with worse mental health. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2020, 62, 686–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Albuquerque, N.; Guo, K.; Wilkinson, A.; Savalli, C.; Otta, E.; Mills, D. Dogs recognize dog and human emotions. Biol. Lett. 2016, 12, 20150883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ford, G.; Guo, K.; Mills, D. Human facial expression affects a dog’s response to conflicting directional gestural cues. Behav. Process. 2019, 159, 80–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Urfer, S.R.; Greer, K.; Wolf, N.S. Age-related cataract in dogs: A biomarker for life span and its relation to body size. Age 2011, 33, 451–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Urfer, S.R.; Kaeberlein, T.L.; Mailheau, S.; Bergman, P.J.; Creevy, K.E.; Promislow, D.E.L.; Kaeberlein, M. Asymptomatic heart valve dysfunction in healthy middle-aged companion dogs and its implications for cardiac aging. Geroscience 2017, 39, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Szabó, D.; Gee, N.R.; Miklósi, A. Natural or Pathologic? Discrepancies in the study of behavioral and cognitive signs in aging family dogs. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2016, 11, 86–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piotti, P.; Szabó, D.; Wallis, L.; Bognár, Z.; Stiegmann, B.S.; Egerer, A.; Marty, P.; Kubinyi, E. The effect of age on visuo-spatial short-term memory in family dogs. Pet Behav. Sci. 2017, 4, 17–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smit, I.; Szabo, D.; Kubinyi, E. Age-related positivity effect on behavioural responses of dogs to human vocalisations. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 20201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hirai, T.; Kojima, S.; Shimada, A.; Umemura, T.; Sakai, M.; Itakura, C. Age-related changes in the olfactory system of dogs. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 1996, 22, 531–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bellows, J.; Colitz, C.M.H.; Daristotle, L.; Ingram, D.K.; Lepine, A.; Marks, S.L.; Sanderson, S.L.; Tomlinson, J.; Zhang, J. Common physical and functional changes associated with aging in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2015, 246, 67–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bellows, J.; Center, S.; Daristotle, L.; Estrada, A.H.; Flickinger, E.A.; Horwitz, D.F.; Lascelles, B.D.X.; Lepine, A.; Perea, S.; Scherk, M.; et al. Aging in cats: Common physical and functional changes. J. Feline Med. Surg. 2016, 18, 533–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piotti, P. Positive emotions and quality of life in dogs. Anim. Sentience 2017, 90, 6. [Google Scholar]
- Wallis, L.J.; Szabó, D.; Erdélyi-Belle, B.; Kubinyi, E. Demographic change across the lifespan of pet dogs and their impact on health status. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evangelista, M.C.; Watanabe, R.; Leung, V.S.Y.; Monteiro, B.P.; O’Toole, E.; Pang, D.S.J.; Steagall, P.V. Facial expressions of pain in cats: The development and validation of a feline grimace scale. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 19128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Holden, E.; Calvo, G.; Collins, M.; Bell, A.; Reid, J.; Scott, E.M.; Nolan, A.M. Evaluation of facial expression in acute pain in cats. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2014, 55, 615–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Reid, J.; Scott, E.M.; Calvo, G.; Nolan, A.M. Definitive glasgow acute pain scale for cats: Validation and intervention Level. Vet. Rec. 2017, 108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Downes, M.J.; Devitt, C.; Downes, M.T.; More, S.J. Understanding the context for pet cat and dog feeding and exercising behaviour among pet owners in Ireland: A qualitative study. Ir. Vet. J. 2017, 70, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fatjó, J.; Bowen, J. Making the case for multi-axis assessment of behavioural problems. Animals 2020, 10, 383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pirrone, F. Animal Assisted Intervention (AAI) for children in either research, practice or policy from a one health perspective. Ann. DellIstituto Super. Sanità 2017, 53, 273–274. [Google Scholar]
Group | |||
---|---|---|---|
Cats % (N) | Dogs % (N) | Total % (N) | |
Demographic characteristics of the pet | |||
Age of the pet | |||
6–12 months | 3 (2) | 3 (5) | 3 (7) |
1–5 years | 35 (26) | 38 (62) | 37 (88) |
5–10 years | 35 (26) | 40 (64) | 38 (90) |
More than 10 years | 27 (20) | 19 (30) | 21 (50) |
Sex of the pet | |||
Male intact | - | 21 (34) | 14 (34) |
Female intact | - | 14 (23) | 10 (23) |
Male neutered | 54 (40) | 21 (34) | 31 (74) |
Female neutered | 46 (34) | 44 (70) | 44 (104) |
Breed | |||
Pure breed (with pedigree) | 16 (12) | 49 (79) | 39 (91) |
Mix or unregistered breed | 84 (62) | 51 (82) | 61 (144) |
Size (dogs only) | |||
Below 10 kg | - | 22 (36) | - |
10–25 kg | - | 45 (73) | - |
Above 25 kg | - | 32 (52) | - |
Age when adopted | |||
Early (before 8 weeks) | 24 (18) | 40 (65) | 43 (101) |
Adequate (8–13 weeks) | 27 (20) | 42 (68) | 37 (88) |
Late (above 3 months) | 49 (36) | 17 (28) | 20 (46) |
Time since adoption | |||
6–12 months | 4 (3) | 8 (13) | 7 (16) |
1–5 years | 43 (32) | 43 (70) | 43 (102) |
5–10 years | 31 (23) | 34 (55) | 33 (78) |
More than 10 years | 22 (16) | 14 (23) | 17 (39) |
Origin of the animal | |||
Pet shop/unregistered breeder | 15 (11) | 24 (38) | 21 (49) |
Private owner or registered breeder | 31 (23) | 42 (67) | 38 (90) |
Shelter/streets | 54 (40) | 35 (56) | 41 (96) |
Attitude of the owner towards the adoption of the pet | |||
Emotional | |||
Yes | 100 (74) | 98 (157) | 98 (231) |
No | - | 2 (4) | 2 (4) |
Working | |||
Yes | 14 (10) | 16 (26) | 15 (36) |
No | 86 (64) | 84 (135) | 85 (199) |
Support to myself | |||
Yes | 8 (6) | 10 (16 | 9 (22) |
No | 92 (68) | 90 (145) | 91 (213) |
Support to others | |||
Yes | 3 (2) | 4 (6) | 3 (8) |
No | 97 (72) | 96 (155) | 97 (227) |
Predictors | Physical QoL | Psychological QoL | Social QoL | Environmental QoL |
---|---|---|---|---|
Pet’s demographics | Rp2 = 0.33 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.07 p = 0.042 | Rp2 = 0.24 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.03 p = 0.542 |
Species | ||||
Cat (1) vs. Dog (2) | 0.63 (0.16) < 0.001 | 0.41 (0.17) 0.019 | −0.14 (0.14) 0.310 | 0.04 (0.18) 0.823 |
Pet’s age | ||||
Old (1) vs. Middle age (2) | −0.97 (0.12) < 0.001 | −0.11 (0.13) 0.653 | −0.01 (0.10) 0.992 | 0.16 (0.13) 0.414 |
Old (1) vs. Young (2) | −1.14 (0.30) 0.001 | −0.24 (0.32) 0.741 | −0.29 (0.27) 0.531 | 0.09 (0.33) 0.960 |
Young (1) Middle age (2) | 0.17 (0.29) 0.829 | 0.12 (0.31) 0.911 | 0.27 (0.26) 0.525 | 0.08 (0.3) 0.968 |
Pet’s sex | ||||
Female (1) vs. Male (2) | −0.01 (0.09) 0.966 | 0.05 (0.10) 0.617 | 0.02 (0.09) 0.854 | −0.10 (0.11) 0.337 |
Breed | ||||
Mix (1) vs. Purebred (2) | −0.31 (0.11) 0.003 | −0.07 (0.11) 0.555 | −0.33 (0.09) < 0.001 | −0.08 (0.11) 0.503 |
Size | ||||
Small (1) vs. Medium (2) | −0.23 (0.15) 0.274 | 0.07 (0.16) 0.903 | −0.31 (0.13) 0.059 | −0.16 (0.16) 0.590 |
Small (1) vs. Large (2) | −0.34 (0.16) 0.098 | −0.21 (0.17) 0.452 | −0.28 (0.14) 0.139 | 0.1 (0.18) 0.799 |
Medium (1) vs. Large (2) | −0.10 (0.13) 0.729 | −0.28 (0.14) 0.126 | 0.04 (0.12) 0.951 | 0.27 (0.14) 0.142 |
Body condition score | −0.12 (0.10) 0.251 | 0.03 (0.11) 0.784 | −0.17 (0.09) 0.069 | 0.01 (0.11) 0.910 |
Pet’s life experience | Rp2 = 0.22 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.04 p = 0.091 | Rp2 = 0.08 p = 0.003 | Rp2 = 0.03 p = 0.268 |
Origin of the pet | ||||
Pet shop/Unregistered breeder (1) vs. Private owner/Registered breeder (2) | −0.08 (0.14) 0.811 | 0.23 (0.14) 0.244 | −0.24 (0.13) 0.134 | −0.16 (0.14) 0.514 |
Pet shop/Unregistered breeder (1) vs. Streets/Shelter (2) | 0.13 (0.14) 0.630 | 0.01 (0.14) 0.997 | −0.04 (0.13) 0.951 | −0.12 (0.14) 0.680 |
Private owner/Registered breeder (1) vs. Streets/Shelter (2) | 0.21 (0.11) 0.158 | −0.22 (0.12) 0.157 | 0.20 (0.10) 0.130 | 0.04 (0.12) 0.947 |
Age at adoption | ||||
Adequate (1) vs. Early (2) | 0.02 (0.14) 0.984 | 0.27 (0.14) 0.149 | 0.21 (0.13) 0.216 | −0.18 (0.14) 0.431 |
Adequate (1) vs. Late (2) | 0.30 (0.11) 0.031 | −0.01 (0.12) 0.999 | 0.13 (0.11) 0.401 | −0.25 (0.12) 0.103 |
Early (1) vs. Late (2) | 0.27 (0.14) 0.127 | −0.27 (0.144) 0.138 | −0.07 (0.13) 0.816 | −0.07 (0.14) 0.887 |
Length pet ownership | −0.44 (0.06) < 0.001 | 0.055 (0.063) 0.378 | −0.17 (0.06) 0.002 | 0.04 (0.06) 0.490 |
Owner’s demographics | Rp2 = 0.07 p = 0.049 | Rp2 = 0.12 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.03 p = 0.609 | Rp2 = 0.08 p = 0.019 |
Age | 0.01 (0.047) 0.820 | 0.07 (0.04) 0.088 | 0.01 (0.04) 0.722 | −0.05 (0.04) 0.240 |
Gender | ||||
Female (1) vs. Male (2) | 0.13 (0.19) 0.749 | −0.04 (0.17) 0.966 | 0.10 (0.16) 0.827 | 0.47 (0.17) 0.021 |
Female (1) vs. Other (2) | 0.53 (0.61) 0.659 | 0.04 (0.55) 0.996 | 0.48 (0.52) 0.629 | −0.59 (0.56) 0.543 |
Male (1) vs. Other (2) | 0.39 (0.63) 0.808 | 0.08 (0.57) 0.987 | 0.39 (0.54) 0.757 | −1.06 (0.58) 0.163 |
Level of education | ||||
Higher (1) vs. Primary/Secondary (2) | 0.15 (0.13) 0.250 | 0.29 (0.12) 0.017 | 0.08 (0.12) 0.503 | 0.12 (0.13) 0.325 |
Relationship status | ||||
Co-living relationship (1) vs. No relationship (2) | 0.01 (0.12) 0.999 | 0.09 (0.11) 0.699 | −0.20 (0.11) 0.144 | −0.06 (0.11) 0.832 |
Co-living relationship (1) vs. Non-co-living relationship (2) | 0.33 (0.18) 0.171 | 0.14 (0.17) 0.681 | 0.08 (0.15) 0.858 | 0.24 (0.17) 0.347 |
No relationship (1) vs. Non-co-living relationship (2) | 0.32 (0.19) 0.211 | 0.05 (0.18) 0.959 | 0.289 (0.17) 0.204 | 0.30 (0.18) 0.211 |
Medical conditions | ||||
Yes (1) vs. No (2) | −0.38 (0.15) 0.012 | −0.3 (0.14) 0.006 | 0.01 (0.13) 0.963 | 0.02 (0.14) 0.900 |
Wellbeing | ||||
Improved (1) vs. Same (2) | 0.23 (0.16) 0.335 | 0.03 (0.15) 0.970 | 0.06 (0.14) 0.877 | 0.20 (0.15) 0.382 |
Improved (1) vs. Declined (2) | −0.06 (0.16) 0.910 | 0.29 (0.15) 0.128 | 0.02 (0.14) 0.990 | 0.29 (0.15) 0.154 |
Same (1) vs. Declined (2) | −0.30 (0.12) 0.042 | 0.26 (0.11) 0.055 | −0.05 (0.11) 0.892 | 0.09 (0.11) 0.728 |
Environment | Rp2 = 0.01 p = 0.853 | Rp2 = 0.05 p = 0.117 | Rp2 = 0.20 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.20 p < 0.001 |
Geographical area | ||||
Rural (1) vs. Urban/Suburban (2) | 0.15 (0.14) 0.276 | −0.02 (0.13) 0.875 | −0.05 (0.11) 0.644 | −0.08 (0.12) 0.511 |
Outdoor space | ||||
Windows (1) vs. Balcony (2) | 0.02 (0.20) 0.991 | 0.20 (0.19) 0.519 | −0.049 (0.16) 0.948 | 0.37 (0.17) 0.088 |
Windows (1) vs. Garden (2) | 0.01 (0.20) 0.999 | 0.17 (0.18) 0.637 | −0.38 (0.16) 0.044 | 0.69 (0.17) < 0.001 |
Balcony (1) vs. Garden (2) | −0.01 (0.134) 0.988 | −0.04 (0.12) 0.949 | −0.33 (0.10) 0.004 | 0.32 (0.11) 0.012 |
Perceived home size | ||||
Medium/Large (1) vs. Small (2) | −0.06 (0.17) 0.711 | 0.41 (0.16) 0.009 | 0.01 (0.13) 0.953 | −0.27 (0.14) 0.064 |
Access to outdoors | ||||
No outdoor access (1) vs. Unrestricted outdoor access (2) | 0.15 (0.14) 0.286 | 0.04 (0.13) 0.749 | −0.08 (0.11) 0.445 | 0.06 (0.12) 0.623 |
Country of residence | ||||
Italy (1) vs. Other (2) | −0.10 (0.14) 0.486 | −0.23 (0.13) 0.081 | −0.53 (0.11) < 0.001 | −0.39 (0.12) 0.002 |
COVID-19 | Rp2 = 0.01 p = 0.646 | Rp2 = 0.10 p = 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.10 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.03 p = 0.268 |
Perceived risk | ||||
Low risk (1) vs. Medium/High risk (2) | −0.01 (0.11) 0.932 | 0.14 (0.10) 0.167 | 0.24 (0.09) 0.011 | 0.09 (0.11) 0.378 |
Lockdown level | ||||
National/Strict lockdown (1) vs. No measures (2) | 0.07 (0.38) 0.979 | −0.11 (0.34) 0.942 | 0.67 (0.31) 0.082 | 0.21 (0.35) 0.811 |
National/Strict lockdown (1) vs. Social distancing/Relaxed measures (2) | 0.19 (0.14) 0.337 | 0.12 (0.12) 0.626 | −0.11 (0.11) 0.585 | −0.15 (0.13) 0.461 |
No measures (1) vs. Social distancing/Relaxed measures (2) | 0.12 (0.36) 0.938 | 0.23 (0.32) 0.761 | −0.78 (0.30) 0.024 | −0.37 (0.33) 0.510 |
Lockdown limitations | ||||
Large limitations (1) vs. No/minimum limitations (2) | −0.12 (0.13) 0.371 | 0.07 (0.12) 0.551 | −0.36 (0.11) 0.001 | 0.20 (0.12) 0.098 |
Financial loss | ||||
Large loss (1) vs. No/Small loss (2) | −0.09 (0.14) 0.512 | −0.58 (0.13) < 0.001 | 0.09 (0.11) 0.461 | −0.04 (0.13) 0.763 |
Human personality | Rp2 = 0.04 p = 0.172 | Rp2 = 0.10 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.02 p = 0.242 | Rp2 = 0.03 p = 0.268 |
FFFS | 0.01 (0.01) 0.840 | −0.01 (0.01) 0.516 | 0.01 (0.01) 0.918 | −0.01 (0.01) 0.911 |
BIS | −0.01 (0.01) 0.076 | −0.02 (0.01) < 0.001 | 0.01 (0.01) 0.798 | 0.01 (0.01) 0.471 |
BAS-Impulsivity | 0.02 (0.01) 0.274 | 0.02 (0.01) 0.277 | −0.01 (0.01) 0.983 | −0.01 (0.01) 0.328 |
BAS-Reward Reactivity | 0.01 (0.01) 0.632 | −0.01 (0.01) 0.548 | −0.03 (0.01) 0.043 | 0.02 (0.01) 0.256 |
BAS-Goal Drive Persistence | −0.04 (0.02) 0.035 | 0.01 (0.02) 0.678 | −0.01 (0.02) 0.362 | 0.02 (0.02) 0.309 |
BAS-Reward Interest | 0.01 (0.02) 0.602 | −0.04 (0.02) 0.021 | 0.02 (0.01) 0.200 | −0.03 (0.02) 0.039 |
Pet personality | Rp2 = 0.19 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.02 p = 0.145 | Rp2= 0.30 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.01 p = 0.481 |
FFFS | −0.14 (0.06) 0.022 | −0.05 (0.06) 0.396 | −0.10 (0.05) 0.036 | −0.05 (0.06) 0.452 |
BIS | −0.17 (0.06) 0.003 | −0.03 (0.06) 0.571 | −0.06 (0.04) 0.194 | −0.02 (0.06) 0.721 |
BAS | 0.29 (0.06) < 0.001 | −0.15 (0.067) 0.030 | 0.42 (0.05) <0.001 | −0.09 (0.07) 0.163 |
Pet–human relationship | Rp2 = 0.05 p = 0.013 | Rp2 = 0.01 p = 0.632 | Rp2= 0.10 p < 0.001 | Rp2 = 0.01 p = 0.513 |
General attachment | 0.06 (0.02) 0.003 | −0.01 (0.02) 0.500 | 0.05 (0.02) 0.001 | −0.015 (0.02) 0.403 |
People substitute | −0.01 (0.02) 0.442 | −0.01 (0.02) 0.569 | 0.02 (0.01) 0.399 | 0.02 (0.02) 0.161 |
Animal rights | −0.03 (0.04) 0.469 | 0.02 (0.04) 0.670 | −0.03 (0.03) 0.305 | 0.01 (0.04) 0.858 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Piotti, P.; Karagiannis, C.; Satchell, L.; Michelazzi, M.; Albertini, M.; Alleva, E.; Pirrone, F. Use of the Milan Pet Quality of Life Instrument (MPQL) to Measure Pets’ Quality of Life during COVID-19. Animals 2021, 11, 1336. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11051336
Piotti P, Karagiannis C, Satchell L, Michelazzi M, Albertini M, Alleva E, Pirrone F. Use of the Milan Pet Quality of Life Instrument (MPQL) to Measure Pets’ Quality of Life during COVID-19. Animals. 2021; 11(5):1336. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11051336
Chicago/Turabian StylePiotti, Patrizia, Christos Karagiannis, Liam Satchell, Manuela Michelazzi, Mariangela Albertini, Enrico Alleva, and Federica Pirrone. 2021. "Use of the Milan Pet Quality of Life Instrument (MPQL) to Measure Pets’ Quality of Life during COVID-19" Animals 11, no. 5: 1336. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11051336
APA StylePiotti, P., Karagiannis, C., Satchell, L., Michelazzi, M., Albertini, M., Alleva, E., & Pirrone, F. (2021). Use of the Milan Pet Quality of Life Instrument (MPQL) to Measure Pets’ Quality of Life during COVID-19. Animals, 11(5), 1336. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11051336