1. Introduction
Currently, there are rising levels of forced displacement of people globally and not enough funds or assistance to sufficiently aid in the humanitarian crisis, according to the global humanitarian assistance report in 2021. While scholars have researched the ways in which message framing can impact monetary donations [
1,
2,
3,
4,
5,
6,
7], less research has investigated the ways to increase an alternate form of assistance: donating time.
Volunteer tourism, which involves “aiding or alleviating the material poverty of some groups in society, the restoration of certain environments or research into aspects of society or environment” (p. 1) [
8], is a meaningful and rewarding form of tourism since a tourist can gain self-development and altruistic experiences through participating in environmental conservation work or interactions with local communities [
9,
10]. As more and more organizations are advertising for this type of tourism, volunteer tourism seems to be gaining in popularity (e.g., the result of a google search “volunteer tourism” on 12 June 2022 returned 2,140,000,000 hits). However, previous empirical studies in the volunteer tourism field focused mainly on personal characteristics of volunteer tourists or perspective of community members at a particular destination on volunteer tourism [
9,
10,
11]. In other words, scholars in the volunteer tourism context have neglected how and why individuals participate in volunteer tourism via marketing communication channels, such as social media posts and messages. Therefore, this research attempts to identify motivational factors of participating in volunteer tourism (or time donation) from a general traveler’s perspectives on an advertisement. This study’s approach helps practitioners and scholars in this field to market a volunteer tourism product and make it more attractive to general travelers, by focusing on the identified motivational factors for the advertising strategy.
Although volunteer tourism has been conceptually divided into community-based and nature-based [
11], prior empirical research has been conducted within more of an environmental conservation than a community interaction context [
11,
12]. Some scholars suggest that there is a critical need for research on providing a way of investigating each type of volunteer tourism [
13]. This is due in part to people’s differing motivations for participation, such as that community-based volunteer tourism participants may elicit different benefits and expected experiences than those of the nature-based programs.
Since there has been comprehensive literature on monetary donation for people and natural environment [
3,
4,
5,
6,
14], this study examines the influence of type of message on time donation based on the compassion fade framework. Specifically, when confronted with one child who suffers from starvation, individuals often move their hearts and help the child with their donation [
3]. However, if the number of children increases to two or three, both emotional and behavioral compassion will begin to wane, which is referred to as compassion fade (i.e., reduce supportive or helping behaviors) [
3,
4]. It is necessary to investigate whether or not compassion fade can be applicable to time spending of voluntary activities rather than money spending, such as volunteer tourism activities. Since time spending requires higher levels of efforts and involvements than monetary donations, the compassion fade framework should be applied to the volunteer tourism context with consideration of its core components, including emotional involvement, credibility, and positive affect, along with attitudes towards the message. To be specific, individuals’ assessment of money is less likely to be ambiguous and their perception of monetary constraints tends to be less elastic than those of time donation, such as volunteer tourism [
12,
14]. In addition, volunteer tourism provides participants with direct experiences that require high levels of involvement, emotions, and feelings with community members at a particular destination in need [
13]. However, monetary donations require individuals to send money or click a donation button at home or work. These characteristics of volunteer tourism (or time donation) lead scholars to consider participants’ emotions and extra efforts for others in need. Furthermore, since monetary donations are relatively simpler than time donations (e.g., indirect support via a click button at home/work vs. direct support via interactions with people in need at a particular destination), individuals’ intention to participate in monetary donations is likely to be easily influenced by contextual or personal factors (i.e., income level or economic conditions) [
8,
9]. More interestingly, from a behavioral perspective, the intention to donate time tends to be perceived as a more responsible act than a monetary donation among individuals because time donations enable individuals to create positive attributions via the particular behavior [
6,
7]. For example, the perceived goal of monetary donation is associated with individuals’ economic utility, whereas the perceived goal of time donation is related to their emotional health and responsibility [
13].
Prior research in the charitable donation field has focused primarily on monetary donation based on the perspective on compassion fade, neglecting time donation. Time donations can lead participants to build higher levels of social capital and loyalty towards a particular place than monetary donations. The characteristics of time donations make this study important (e.g., social sustainability). Since there are behavioral and psychological differences between monetary donation and time donation, it is necessary to apply the impact of compassion fade to the time donation context, such as volunteer tourism. The approach of this study enables scholars and practitioners to develop a psychological framework of time donation behaviors within the context of volunteer tourism. Therefore, this study investigates the relationship between the type of volunteer tourism (human vs. flora vs. fauna) and the type of message (individual with no statistic vs. individual with small statistic vs. individual with large statistic) and potential tourists’ attitudes towards volunteer tourism and their intention to donate their time. To answer this research question, we conducted a between-subject 3×3 factorial design online experiment, where the influences of compassion fade on attitudes and behavioral intention to donate time for volunteer tourism, along with the impacts of positive affect, emotional involvement, and credibility, were examined.
5. Results
Compassion fade. A one-way ANOVA with type of message as the independent variable and compassion fade as the dependent variable was not significant for manipulation checks: source type, F (2,161) = 2.323, p > 0.05. There were not significant differences between participants’ level of compassion among the three message conditions.
Emotional involvement. A one-way ANOVA with type of message as the independent variable and emotional involvement as the dependent variable was not significant for manipulation checks: source type, F (2,161) = 0.664, p > 0.05. There were not significant differences between participants’ emotional involvement among the three message conditions.
The study’s main research question asked about the relationship between type of volunteer tourism (human vs. flora vs. fauna) and type of message (individual with no statistic vs. individual with small statistic vs. individual with large statistic) and potential tourists’ attitudes towards volunteer tourism and attitude towards the ad and intention to donate time.
The results of ANOVA with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as fixed factors and attitude towards volunteer tourism as the dependent variable revealed that the overall model was significant, F (8,152) = 2.667, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.122. The analysis showed a significant main effect for the two-way interaction between dimensions, F (4,152) = 3.230, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.078, but no significant effect was found for the type of volunteer tourism, F (2,152) = 1.945, and type of message, F (2,152) = 2.262. The results of ANOVA with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as fixed factors and attitude towards the ad as the dependent variable revealed that the overall model was not significant, F (8,152) = 0.383. Therefore, no significant main affect and no significant two-way interaction between dimensions were found. Therefore, type of message/type of volunteer tourism did not affect attitude towards volunteer tourism and attitude towards the ad. Therefore, the results indicated that type of volunteer tourism and type of message do not affect attitude towards volunteer tourism and attitude towards the ad.
The results of an analysis of logistic regression with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as covariates and actual charitable behavior as the dependent variable indicated that type of volunteer tourism (EXP (B) = 0.778, Wald (1) = 1.115, p = 0.291) and type of message (EXP (B) = 0.863, Wald (1) = 0.374, p = 0.541) were not significant predictors of participants’ actual charitable behavior. Furthermore, the results of an analysis of logistic regression with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as covariates and charitable behavioral intention as the dependent variable indicated that type of volunteer tourism (EXP (B) = 0.959, Wald (1) = 0.042, p = 0.838) and type of message (EXP (B) = 0.882, Wald (1) = 0.375, p = 0.541) were not significant predictors of participants’ charitable behavioral intention. Therefore, the results indicated that there is no relationship between type of volunteer tourism/type of message and actual charitable behavior and charitable behavioral intention.
H1a predicted that as the number of victims increased, participants would feel less compassion, which would lead to a decrease in attitude towards volunteer tourism, and H3a predicted that for the human condition, participants would show higher compassion, which would lead to an increase in attitude towards volunteer tourism. In order to examine these hypotheses, an ANOVA was conducted with compassion fade as a covariate, along with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as independent variables and attitude towards volunteer tourism as the dependent variable. The overall model was significant, F (9,152) = 17.053, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.502. The analysis showed a significant main effect for a two-way interaction between dimensions, F (4,152) = 2.633, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.065. Compassion fade was also shown to be a significant predictor of attitude towards volunteer tourism, F (1,152) = 139.676, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.479. However, no significant main effects were found for (H1a) type of message, F (2,152) = 0.319, or (H3a) type of volunteer tourism, F (2,153) = 1.318. Therefore, H1a and H3a were not supported.
H1b predicted that as the number of victims increased, participants would show less emotional involvement, which would lead to a decrease in attitude towards volunteer tourism, and H3b predicted that for the human condition, participants would show higher emotional involvement, which would lead to an increase in attitude towards volunteer tourism. Again, an ANOVA was conducted with emotional involvement as a covariate, along with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as independent variables, and attitude towards volunteer tourism as the dependent variable. The overall model was significant, F (9,152) = 19.289, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.533. Emotional involvement was shown to be a significant predictor of attitude towards volunteer tourism, F (1,152) = 158.972, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.511. However, no significant main effects were found for (H1b) type of message, F (2,152) = 2.125, or (H3b) type of volunteer tourism, F (2,152) = 0.850 and there were no significant interactions, F (4,152) = 2.170. Therefore, H1b and H3b were not supported.
H1c predicted that as the number of victims increased, participants would feel less positive affect, which would lead to a decrease in attitude towards volunteer tourism, and H3c predicted that for the human condition, participants would feel more positive affect, which would lead to an increase in attitude towards volunteer tourism. Again, an ANOVA was conducted, with positive affect as a covariate, along with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as independent variables, and attitude towards volunteer tourism as the dependent variable. The overall model was significant, F (9,152) = 9.446, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.359. The analysis showed a significant main effect for (H3c) type of volunteer tourism, F (2, 152) = 10.303, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.119, such that participants in the human condition had more positive attitude towards volunteer tourism (M = 5.507, SD = 0.154) than those in the flora volunteer tourism condition (M = 4.574, SD = 0.151) and fauna conditions (M = 4.804, SD = 1.50). Positive affect was also shown to be a significant predictor of attitude towards volunteer tourism, F (1,152) = 49.808, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.247. Furthermore, a significant main effect for two-way interaction between dimensions, F (4,152) = 2.761, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.068, was found. No significant main effects were found for (H1c) type of message, F (2,152) = 1.779. Therefore, H3c was supported and H1c was not supported.
H2a predicted that as the number of victims increased, participants would feel less compassion, which would lead to a decrease in attitude towards the ad, and H4a predicted that for the human condition, participants would show higher compassion, which would lead to an increase in attitude towards the ad. In order to examine these hypotheses, an ANOVA was conducted with compassion fade as a covariate, along with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as independent variables and attitude towards the ad as the dependent variable. The overall model was significant, F (9,152) = 7.814, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.316. The analysis showed a significant main effect for (H4a) type of volunteer tourism, F(2, 152) = 4.001, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.050, such that participants in human volunteer tourism condition had less positive attitude towards the ad (M = 4.640, SD = 0.154) than those in the flora volunteer tourism condition (M = 5.291, SD = 0.157), and there was no difference between participants’ attitude towards the ad in human and fauna conditions (M = 4.973, SD = 1.53). Compassion fade was also shown to be a significant predictor of attitude towards the ad, F (1,152) = 58.059, p < 0.05, R2 =0.276. No significant main effects were found for (H2a) type of message, F (2,152) = 1.395, and there were no significant interactions, F (4,152) = 0.369. Therefore, H2a and H4a were not supported.
H2b predicted that as the number of victims increased, participants would show less emotional involvement, which would lead to a decrease in attitude towards the ad, and H4b predicted that for the human condition, participants would show higher emotional involvement, which would lead to an increase in attitude towards the ad. Again, an ANOVA was conducted, with emotional involvement as a covariate along with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as independent variables and attitude towards the ad as the dependent variable. The overall model was significant, F (9,152) = 9.377, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.357. Emotional involvement was also shown to be a significant predictor of attitude towards the ad, F (1,152) = 78.313, p < 0.05, R2 =.340. The analysis showed no significant main effect for (H4b) type of volunteer tourism, F (2,152) = 2.237, or (H2b) type of message, F (2,152) = 0.425. Furthermore, no significant main effect for the two-way interaction between dimensions was found. Therefore, H2b and H4b were not supported.
H2c predicted that as the number of victims increased, participants would feel less positive affect, which would lead to a decrease in attitude towards the ad, and H4c predicted that for the human condition, participants would feel more positive affect, which would lead to an increase in attitude towards the ad. Again, an ANOVA was conducted, with positive affect as a covariate, along with type of volunteer tourism and type of message as independent variables and attitude towards the ad as the dependent variable. The overall model was significant, F (9,152) = 9.419, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.358. Positive affect was shown to be a significant predictor of attitude towards ad, F (1,152) = 77.779, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.338. The analysis showed no significant main effect for (H4c) type of volunteer tourism, F (2,152) = 1.562, or (H2c) type of message F (2,152) = 0.945. Furthermore, no significant main effect for the two-way interaction between dimensions was found. Therefore, H2c and H4c were not supported.
H5 predicted that there would be a positive relationship between perceived ad credibility and attitude towards the ad. The result of single linear regression with perceived ad credibility as the independent variable and attitude towards the ad as the dependent variable indicated that there is a statistically significant positive relationship between perceived ad credibility (b = 0.640, t(160)= 11.144, p < 0.05) and attitude towards the ad. As participants perceived more ad credibility, they showed higher positive attitude towards the ad. Therefore, H5 was significant.
Furthermore, H6 predicted that there is a positive relationship between perceived ad credibility and attitude towards volunteer tourism. The result of linear regression with perceived ad credibility as the independent variable and attitude towards volunteer tourism as the dependent variable indicated that there is a statistically significant positive relationship between perceived ad credibility (b = 0.690, t(160) = 12.509, p < 0.05) and attitude towards volunteer tourism. As participants perceived more ad credibility, they showed higher positive attitude towards volunteer tourism. In sum, H6 was significant.
In sum, the results of the current study revealed that type of volunteer tourism and type of message do not affect attitude towards volunteer tourism and attitude towards the ad. Furthermore, there is no relationship between type of volunteer tourism and type of message and attitude towards the ad and attitude towards volunteer tourism. Further analysis indicated that, among all three mediating variables (e.g., compassion fade, emotional involvement, positive affect) only positive affect mediated the relationship between type of volunteer tourism and attitude towards volunteer tourism (H3c), and all other hypotheses were not statistically significant. Moreover, results indicated that there is a positive relationship between perceived ad credibility and attitude towards the ad and also between perceived ad credibility and attitude towards volunteer tourism. The implications of these results are discussed below.
6. Discussion
While the global humanitarian crisis continues to receive attention [
34], research has asked how message framing can impact people’s monetary donations [
1,
2,
3,
5,
6,
7], yet less research has asked how appeals can be adapted to garner volunteers to donate their time instead of their money. Volunteer tourism or “voluntourism” is the act of people traveling to other countries to donate their time to help the community. Informed by compassion fade as an explanatory framework, the present study examined the effects of message type on participant attitudes and intention to donate time. Additionally, this project attempts to establish three distinct types of voluntourism: fauna, flora, and humans. The implications of the study’s findings are discussed below.
First, although voluntourism has been conceptually divided into two realms and studied in terms of the community-based and nature-based [
11], this paper offers a conceptual frame to include flora (plants), fauna (animals), and humans as distinct forms of voluntourism, since people’s motivation, benefits, and expectations may vary greatly between the three distinct categories. As a step in the right direction, in the current study, when participants were randomly assigned to one of the three types of voluntourism, they indicated the category that corresponded to their stimuli most of the time (93%). In future studies, this relationship could be elucidated further by examining people’s expectations and motivation for each of the distinct categories, but this study offers an initial step to determining the different categorizations of this type of tourism.
Although compassion fade is usually studied with monetary donations, this study attempted to extend the literature to donations of time and attitudes towards such types of volunteering activities. Consistent with the compassion fade framework, research shows that unlike a group of people, a single individual is viewed differently psychologically. People respond to single individuals as a well-defined unit, which leads the viewer to process the information more and create a stronger impression about the individual as opposed to a group [
7,
35,
36]. Furthermore, research shows that stronger affect feelings are evoked when people consider single victims as opposed to a group of victims; specifically, participants reported stronger feelings of distress and compassion, which lead to higher donations when examining a single identified victim than compared to a group of victims [
2]. Therefore, in the current study, we examined if this effect could be studied when participants were asked to donate their time as opposed to their money to help victims. As similarly predicted in Västfjäll et al. [
7] for charitable donations, we expected positive affect to be strongest for a single, identified individual. This hypothesis was not supported. Furthermore, in line with the compassion fade construct, we predicted that as the number of victims increased, the reported compassion would decrease, which would lead to a decrease in intention to donate time, just as it has been shown with monetary donations. This hypothesis was also not supported. We also could not find consistent results with participants’ attitude towards the ad or attitude towards volunteer tourism. However, we did find support for the credibility hypothesis, which bolsters the argument that the more credible the participant perceived the stimuli, the more favorable they reported their (a) feelings towards volunteer tourism and (b) feelings towards the ad. We suspect that we encountered such non-significant findings because the manipulation checks on the stimuli failed. Although these findings appear challenging, the possible limitations that could account for these results are discussed below, as well as future directions for this body of work.
While the results of the present study hold some theoretical and practical implications, several limitations should contextualize the findings. Overall, this study could not replicate many of the findings that have been found in the compassion fade literature. One reason this could have occurred is because our sample size was relatively small (n = 162). Several of the main effects were approaching significance and it would take further study power to determine if more of the study’s hypotheses are actually significant. Another possible reason we did not find many significant results could be that the manipulation was too subtle. In future studies, to make the numeracy manipulation more salient, we could emphasize the statistic either by presenting it separately from the advertisement or by building in a manipulation check to see if they could even recall the statistic that was shown to them. Moreover, this study also suffered measurement errors. Most notably, we measured participants’ intention to donate on a binary (yes/no) scale. In reality, if we had measured it on a Likert-type scale (1 = not at all; 7 = very likely), we could have run more dynamic statistical tests to examine the relationship between variables better, so this should be taken into account for future studies. Additionally, the stimuli were modeled after an actual ad, and showed neutrally framed wording to accompany a pleasant photo (i.e., a smiling boy; an elephant with perky ears). Future studies could consider manipulating the frame of the wording to see if a gain vs. loss framing could stimulate people with a call-to-action (i.e., if people do not volunteer their time, something adverse would happen to the people/plant/animal featured in the stimuli). Taking all of these concerns into consideration, this study has potential to be improved.
In a related vein, another plausible reason for finding inconsistencies across conditions could be that individual differences could account more for people’s willingness to travel and not necessarily measurement errors. According to a recent study by Hostelworld Global Traveler Report, nearly one-third of Americans (29%) have never been abroad, and they are about half as likely to travel to more than one country as compared to Europeans [
37]. This would suggest that perhaps participants’ past international travel experience could impact the way they view and respond to the stimuli. Future studies should consider measuring for this construct to rule out this possible alternative explanation. Another way to gain perspective on this particular variable would be to replicate the current study, but instead of using poor, foreign nations, research could study the effects of psychological distance on participants’ willingness to donate their time. Therefore, would something geographically closer to the participant make a difference in their attitudes and intentions?