Beyond the Surface: A New Perspective on Dual-System Theories in Decision-Making
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Two Systems of Reasoning
3. Compensatory vs. Noncompensatory Models
4. Reconsidering the Dual-System Approach
4.1. Simultaneous Contradictory Belief
4.2. Between-System Processes vs. Within-System Processes
5. Dissociation Between Behavior and Autonomic Responses
5.1. Case Study 1: Between-System Processes vs. Within-System Processes
5.2. Case Study 2: Peripheral Arterial Tone vs. Behavioral Responses
5.2.1. Method
5.2.2. Results and Brief Discussion
5.3. Between-System Processes vs. Within-System Processes
6. Processes Within the Two Systems
6.1. Examining the Processes Underlying Risky Choices
6.2. Further Modeling Analysis
Summary of the Results and Brief Discussion
7. Summary
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Beach, D.; Pedersen, R.B. Process-Tracing Methods: Foundations and Guidelines; University of Michigan Press: Ann Arbor, MI, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Ford, J.K.; Schmitt, N.; Schechtman, S.L.; Hults, B.M.; Doherty, M.L. Process tracing methods: Contributions, problems, and neglected research problems. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1989, 43, 75–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isen, A.M.; Means, B. The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategy. Soc. Cogn. 1983, 2, 18–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, E.J. Expertise and decision under uncertainty: Performance and process. In The Nature of Expertise; Chi, M.T.H., Glaser, R., Farr, M.J., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1988; pp. 209–228. [Google Scholar]
- Onken, J.; Hastie, R.; Revelle, W. Individual differences in the use of simplification strategies in a complex decision-making task. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1985, 11, 14–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Witteman, C.; van Geenen, E. Cognitive process analysis. In Foundations for Tracing Intuition; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2009; pp. 53–68. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, E.J.; Payne, J.W.; Bettman, J.R. Information displays and preference reversals. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1988, 42, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klayman, J. Analysis of predecisional information search patterns. In Analyzing and Aiding Decision Processes; Humphreys, P., Svenson, O., Vari, A., Eds.; North-Holland: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Payne, J.W.; Bettman, J.R.; Johnson, E.J. Adaptive strategy selection in decision making. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1988, 14, 534–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, E.J.; Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M.; Willemsen, M.C. Process models deserve process data: Comment on Brandstätter, Gigerenzer, and Hertwig (2006). Psychol. Rev. 2008, 115, 263–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norman, E.; Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M. Take a quick click at that! Mouselab and eye-tracking as tools to measure intuition. In Foundations for Tracing Intuition; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2009; pp. 32–52. [Google Scholar]
- Ayal, S.; Hochman, G. Ignorance or integration: The cognitive processes underlying choice behavior. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2009, 22, 455–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brandstätter, E.; Gigerenzer, G.; Hertwig, R. The priority heuristic: Making choices without trade-offs. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 113, 409–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hochman, G.; Ayal, S.; Ariely, D. Fairness requires deliberation: The primacy of economic over social considerations. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glöckner, A.; Betsch, T. Do people make decisions under risk based on ignorance? An empirical test of the priority heuristic against cumulative prospect theory. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2008, 107, 75–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hochman, G.; Glöckner, A.; Yechiam, E. Physiological measures in identifying decision strategies. In Foundations for Tracing Intuition; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2009; pp. 147–167. [Google Scholar]
- De Neys, W. Conflict detection, dual processes, and logical intuitions: Some clarifications. Think. Reason. 2014, 20, 169–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epstein, S. Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious. Am. Psychol. 1994, 49, 709–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pennycook, G. A framework for understanding reasoning errors: From fake news to climate change and beyond. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2023, 67, 131–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sloman, S.A. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 119, 3–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? In Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment; Gilovich, T., Griffin, D.W., Kahneman, D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002; pp. 421–440. [Google Scholar]
- Müller-Lyer, F.C. Optische Urteilstauschungen. Arch. Anat. Physiol. 1889, 2, 263–270. [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman, D.; Slovic, E.; Tversky, A. Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Yechiam, E.; Hochman, G. Losses as modulators of attention: Review and analysis of the unique effects of losses over gains. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 497–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirkpatrick, L.A.; Epstein, S. Cognitive–experiential self-theory and subjective probability: Further evidence for two conceptual systems. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 63, 534–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, J.S.B. In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 454–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D.; Frederick, S. Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment; Gilovich, T., Griffin, D.W., Kahneman, D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002; pp. 49–81. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, J.S.B.T. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008, 59, 255–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Acker, F. New findings on unconscious versus conscious thought in decision making: Additional empirical data and meta-analysis. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2008, 3, 292–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bago, B.; De Neys, W. Fast logic? Examining the time course assumption of dual process theory. Cognition 2017, 158, 90–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bruine de Bruin, W.; Parker, A.M.; Fischhoff, B. Individual differences in adult decision-making competence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 938–956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Davis, D.G.S.; Staddon, J.E.R.; Machado, A.; Palmer, R.G. The process of recurrent choice. Psychol. Rev. 1993, 100, 320–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Neys, W.; Pennycook, G. Logic fast and slow: Advances in dual-process theorizing. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 28, 503–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glöckner, A. Does intuition beat fast and frugal heuristics? A systematic empirical analysis. In Intuition in Judgment and Decision Making; Plessner, H., Betsch, C., Betsch, T., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007; pp. 309–325. [Google Scholar]
- Glöckner, A.; Herbold, A.K. An eye-tracking study on information processing in risky decisions: Evidence for compensatory strategies based on automatic processes. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2011, 24, 71–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Usher, M.; Russo, Z.; Weyers, M.; Brauner, R.; Zakay, D. The impact of the mode of thought in complex decisions: Intuitive decisions are better. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Neys, W. On dual-and single-process models of thinking. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2021, 16, 1412–1427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keren, G.; Schul, Y. Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 4, 533–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melnikoff, D.E.; Bargh, J.A. The mythical number two. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018, 22, 280–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dörner, D.; Funke, J. Complex problem solving: What it is and what it is not. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cronin, M.A.; Gonzalez, C.; Sterman, J.D. Why don’t well-educated adults understand accumulation? A challenge to researchers, educators, and citizens. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2009, 108, 116–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Funke, J. Complex problem solving: A case for complex cognition? Cogn. Process. 2010, 11, 133–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Herde, C.N.; Wüstenberg, S.; Greiff, S. Assessment of complex problem solving: What we know and what we don’t know. Appl. Meas. Educ. 2016, 29, 265–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wüstenberg, S.; Greiff, S.; Funke, J. Complex problem solving—More than reasoning? Intelligence 2012, 40, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sloman, S.A. Two systems of reasoning. In Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment; Gilovich, T., Griffin, D.W., Kahneman, D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002; pp. 379–398. [Google Scholar]
- Chaiken, S. Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 39, 752–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epstein, S.; Pacini, R. Some basic issues regarding dual-process theories from the perspective of cognitive-experiential theory. In Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology; Chaiken, S., Trope, Y., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 462–482. [Google Scholar]
- Hammond, K.R. Human Judgment and Social Policy; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman, M.D. Reflective and reflexive judgment processes: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. In Social Judgments: Implicit and Explicit Processes; Forgas, J.P., Williams, K.R., von Hippel, W., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2003; pp. 44–67. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, E.R.; DeCoster, J. Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2000, 4, 108–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epstein, S. Intuition from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In Intuition in Judgment and Decision Making; Plessner, H., Betsch, C., Betsch, T., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007; pp. 23–37. [Google Scholar]
- Finucane, M.L.; Alhakami, A.; Slovic, P.; Johnson, S.M. The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2000, 13, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Agnoli, F. Development of judgmental heuristics and logical reasoning: Training counteracts the representativeness heuristic. Cogn. Dev. 1991, 6, 195–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isen, A.M.; Nygren, T.E.; Ashby, F.G. Influence of positive affect on the subjective utility of gains and losses: It is just not worth the risk. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 55, 710–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krava, L.A.; Ayal, S.; Hochman, G. Time is money: The effect of mode-of-thought on financial decision-making. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 735823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bago, B.; Rand, D.G.; Pennycook, G. Fake news, fast and slow: Deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news headlines. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2020, 149, 1608–1613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goel, V.; Dolan, R.J. Explaining modulation of reasoning by belief. Cognition 2003, 87, B11–B22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goel, V.; Buchel, C.; Frith, C.; Dolan, R.J. Dissociation of mechanisms underlying syllogistic reasoning. Neuroimage 2000, 12, 504–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lieberman, M.D. The X- and C-systems. In Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior; Harmon-Jones, E., Winkielman, P., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 290–315. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman, L.; Rock, I. The moon illusion I. Science 1962, 136, 1023–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denes-Raj, V.; Epstein, S.; Cole, J. The generality of the ratio-bias phenomenon. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 21, 1083–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D. A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. Am. Psychol. 2003, 58, 697–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Einhorn, H.J. Use of nonlinear, noncompensatory models as a function of task and amount of information. Organ. Behav. Hum. Perform. 1971, 6, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elrod, T.; Johnson, R.D.; White, J. A new integrated model of noncompensatory and compensatory decision strategies. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2004, 95, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Payne, J.W.; Bettman, J.R.; Johnson, E.J. The Adaptive Decision Maker; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Von Gunten, C.D.; Scherer, L.D. Self–other differences in multiattribute decision making: Compensatory versus noncompensatory decision strategies. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2019, 32, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Neumann, J.; Morgenstern, O. Theory of Games and Economic Behavior; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1944. [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 1979, 47, 263–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. J. Risk Uncertain. 1992, 5, 297–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, A. Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychol. Rev. 1972, 79, 281–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fishburn, P.C. Lexicographic orders, utilities, and decision rules: A survey. Manag. Sci. 1974, 20, 1442–1472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bröder, A. Take the best, Dawes’ rule, and compensatory decision strategies: A regression-based classification method. Qual. Quant. 2002, 36, 219–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bröder, A.; Schiffer, S. “Take the best” versus simultaneous feature matching: Probabilistic inferences from memory and effects of representation format. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2003, 132, 277–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rieskamp, J.; Otto, P.E. SSL: A theory of how people learn to select strategies. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2006, 135, 207–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bröder, A. Assessing the empirical validity of the “Take-The-Best” heuristic as a model of human probabilistic inference. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2000, 26, 1332–1346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gigerenzer, G.; Goldstein, D.G. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 650–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Erev, I.; Ert, E.; Roth, A.E.; Haruvy, E.; Herzog, S.M.; Hau, R.; Hertwig, R.; Stewart, T.; West, R.; Lebiere, C. A choice prediction competition: Choices from experience and from description. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2010, 23, 15–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoerl, C.; McCormack, T. Thinking in and about time: A dual systems perspective on temporal cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 2019, 42, e244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychol. Rev. 1983, 90, 293–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Risen, J.L. Believing what we do not believe: Acquiescence to superstitious beliefs and other powerful intuitions. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 123, 182–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schurr, A.; Erev, I. The effect of base rate careful analysis and the distinction between decisions from experience and from description. Behav. Brain Sci. 2007, 30, 281–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erev, I.; Ert, E.; Yechiam, E. Loss aversion, diminishing sensitivity, and the effect of experience on repeated decisions. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2008, 21, 575–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ert, E.; Erev, I. On the descriptive value of loss aversion in decisions under risk: Six clarifications. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2013, 8, 214–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hochman, G.; Yechiam, E. Loss aversion in the eye and in the heart: The autonomic nervous system’s responses to losses. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2011, 24, 140–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yechiam, E.; Ashby, N.J.; Hochman, G. Are we attracted by losses? Boundary conditions for the approach and avoidance effects of losses. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2019, 45, 591–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iani, C.; Gopher, D.; Lavie, P. Effects of task difficulty and invested mental effort on peripheral vasoconstriction. Psychophysiology 2004, 41, 789–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ganster, D.C.; Crain, T.L.; Brossoit, R.M. Physiological measurement in the organizational sciences: A review and recommendations for future use. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 2018, 5, 267–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D. Attention and Effort; Prentice-Hall: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwenhuis, S.; Aston-Jones, G.; Cohen, J.D. Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychol. Bull. 2005, 131, 510–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bechara, A.; Damasio, H.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, A.R. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 1997, 275, 1293–1295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hochman, G.; Glöckner, A.; Fiedler, S.; Ayal, S. “I can see it in your eyes”: Biased processing and increased arousal in dishonest responses. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2016, 29, 322–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yechiam, E.; Telpaz, A.; Hochman, G. The complaint bias in subjective evaluations of incentives. Decision 2014, 1, 147–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, D.G.; Gigerenzer, G. Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychol. Rev. 2002, 109, 75–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Volz, K.G.; von Cramon, D.Y. Can neuroscience tell a story about intuition? In Intuition in Judgment and Decision Making; Plessner, H., Betsch, C., Betsch, T., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007; pp. 71–87. [Google Scholar]
- McCraty, R.; Atkinson, M.; Bradley, R.T. Electrophysiological evidence of intuition: Part I. The surprising role of the heart. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 2004, 10, 133–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Camerer, C.F.; Babcock, L.; Loewenstein, G.; Thaler, R.H. Labor supply of New York City cabdrivers: One day at a time. Q. J. Econ. 1997, 112, 407–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, A.L.; Imai, T.; Vieider, F.M.; Camerer, C.F. Meta-analysis of empirical estimates of loss aversion. J. Econ. Lit. 2024, 62, 485–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thaler, R.H.; Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D.; Schwartz, A. The effect of myopia and loss aversion on risk-taking: An experimental test. Q. J. Econ. 1997, 112, 647–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tom, S.M.; Fox, C.R.; Trepel, C.; Poldrack, R.A. The neural basis of loss aversion in decision-making under risk. Science 2007, 315, 515–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benartzi, S.; Thaler, R. Myopic loss aversion and the equity premium puzzle. Q. J. Econ. 1995, 110, 73–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samuelson, W.; Zeckhauser, R. Status quo bias in decision making. J. Risk Uncertain. 1988, 1, 7–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kermer, D.A.; Driver-Linn, E.; Wilson, T.D.; Gilbert, D.T. Loss aversion is an affective forecasting error. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 649–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levin, P.I.; Hart, S.S. Risk preferences in young children: Early evidence of individual differences in reaction to potential gains and losses. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2003, 16, 397–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ahern, S.K.; Beatty, J. Physiological signs of information processing vary with intelligence. Science 1979, 205, 1289–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ahern, S.K.; Beatty, J. Physiological evidence that demand for processing capacity varies with intelligence. In Intelligence and Learning; Friedman, M., Dos, J.P., O’Connor, N., Eds.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1981; pp. 121–128. [Google Scholar]
- Beatty, J. Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychol. Bull. 1982, 91, 276–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradshaw, J.L. Pupil size and problem solving. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1968, 20, 116–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldwater, B.C. Psychological significance of pupillary movements. Psychol. Bull. 1972, 77, 340–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landers, R.N.; Sanchez, D.R. Game-based, gamified, and gamefully designed assessments for employee selection: Definitions, distinctions, design, and validation. Int. J. Sel. Assess. 2022, 30, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Béchard, B.; Hodgetts, H.; Morneau-Guérin, F.; Ouimet, M.; Tremblay, S. Political complexity and the pervading role of ideology in policy-making. J. Dyn. Decis. Mak. 2023, 9, 121–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamberts, K.; Brockdorff, N.; Heit, E. Feature sampling and random walk models of individual stimulus recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2003, 132, 351–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Read, D.; Loewenstein, G. The diversification bias: Explaining the difference between prospective and real-time taste for variety. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 1995, 1, 34–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schnall, R.P.; Shlitner, A.; Sheffy, J.; Kedar, R.; Lavie, P. Periodic profound peripheral vasoconstriction: A new marker of obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep 1999, 22, 939–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pillar, G.; Bar, A.; Schnall, R.; Shefy, J.; Lavie, P. Autonomic arousal index: An automated detection based on peripheral arterial tonometry. Sleep 2002, 25, 541–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ayal, S.; Zakay, D.; Hochman, G. Deliberative adjustments of intuitive anchors: The case of diversification behavior. Synthese 2012, 189, 131–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brusovansky, M.; Glickman, M.; Usher, M. Fast and effective: Intuitive processes in complex decisions. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 1542–1548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krava, L.A.; Ayal, S.; Hochman, G. Time is money: The advantages of quick and intuitive financial decision-making. In Behavioral Finance: The Coming of Age; Plessner, H., Betsch, C., Betsch, T., Eds.; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2019; pp. 37–56. [Google Scholar]
- Hochman, G.; Erev, I. The partial-reinforcement extinction effect and the contingent-sampling hypothesis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2013, 20, 1336–1342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ayal, S.; Rusou, Z.; Zakay, D.; Hochman, G. Determinants of judgment and decision-making quality: The interplay between information processing style and situational factors. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grosskopf, B.; Erev, I.; Yechiam, E. Foregone with the wind: Indirect payoff information and its implications for choice. Int. J. Game Theory 2006, 34, 285–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ayal, S.; Zakay, D. The perceived diversity heuristic: The case of pseudodiversity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 96, 559–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gigerenzer, G.; Todd, P.M.; The ABC Research Group. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Busemeyer, J.R.; Townsend, J.T. Decision field theory: A dynamic cognition approach to decision making. Psychol. Rev. 1993, 100, 432–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gigerenzer, G.; Gaissmaier, W. Heuristic decision making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011, 62, 451–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; Farrar, Straus and Giroux: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Todd, P.M.; Gigerenzer, G. Environments that make us smart: Ecological rationality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 16, 167–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klein, G. Naturalistic decision making. Hum. Factors 2008, 50, 456–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simon, H.A. Invariants of human behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1990, 41, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lo, A.W.; Repin, D.V. The psychophysiology of real-time financial risk processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002, 14, 323–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Camerer, C.; Loewenstein, G.; Prelec, D. Neuroeconomics: How neuroscience can inform economics. J. Econ. Lit. 2005, 43, 9–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Hochman, G. Beyond the Surface: A New Perspective on Dual-System Theories in Decision-Making. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 1028. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111028
Hochman G. Beyond the Surface: A New Perspective on Dual-System Theories in Decision-Making. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(11):1028. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111028
Chicago/Turabian StyleHochman, Guy. 2024. "Beyond the Surface: A New Perspective on Dual-System Theories in Decision-Making" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 11: 1028. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111028
APA StyleHochman, G. (2024). Beyond the Surface: A New Perspective on Dual-System Theories in Decision-Making. Behavioral Sciences, 14(11), 1028. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111028