Metacognition About Collaborative Learning: Students’ Beliefs Are Inconsistent with Their Learning Preferences
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Collaborative Cognition
3. Metacognition
4. Students’ Perceptions of Collaboration
4.1. Beliefs, Frequency of Collaboration, and Preferences
4.2. Group Recall and Group Studying
4.3. Strategies Used When Studying Individually and in Groups
4.4. Characteristics of Other Group Members
4.5. Differences Between Students Who Frequently and Rarely Use Study Groups
5. Method
5.1. Participants
5.2. Materials and Procedure
6. Results
6.1. Beliefs, Frequency of Collaboration, and Preferences
6.2. Group Recall and Group Studying
6.3. Strategies Used When Studying Individually and in Groups
6.4. Characteristics of Other Group Members
6.5. Differences Between Students Who Frequently and Rarely Use Study Groups
7. Discussion
7.1. Beliefs, Frequency of Collaboration, and Preferences
7.2. Group Recall and Group Studying
7.3. Strategies Used When Studying Individually and in Groups
7.4. Characteristics of Other Group Members
7.5. Theoretical and Practical Implications
8. Limitations and Future Directions
9. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
- How often do you use study groups?
- Frequently (once or more per week)
- Moderately (once or twice per month)
- Rarely (once or twice per semester)
- Never
- Which do you think is more effective—studying in groups or studying individually?
- Why do you think it is more effective?
- Which do you think is more effective-- recalling (remembering) information in groups or recalling information individually?
- Why do you think it is more effective?
- Do you prefer to study for exams in a group or individually?
- Why do you prefer to study this way?
- List the strategies you use when studying individually and rank the strategies in terms of how frequently you use them (the first strategy you list should be the one you use most frequently; the last strategy you list should be the one you use least frequently). (e.g., 1, XXX 2, XXX 3, XXX 4, XXX 5, XXX)
- List the strategies you use when studying in a group and rank the strategies in terms of how frequently you use them (the first strategy you list should be the one you use most frequently; the last strategy you list should be the one you use least frequently). (e.g., 1, XXX 2, XXX 3, XXX 4, XXX 5, XXX)
- What characteristics of other group members do you think are important in order for study groups to be productive? (e.g., XXX, XXX, XXX, XXX…)
- Why do you think these characteristics are important?
- Do you have any other thoughts regarding study groups that you would like to share?
- Your age (in years): __________
- Gender:
- Male
- Female
- Prefer not to say
- Other: ____________
- Current year in school:
- First year college
- Second year college
- Third year college
- Fourth year college
- Other: _____________
- Ethnicity:
- Hispanic or Latino
- Not Hispanic or Latino
- Race:
- American Indian/Alaska Native
- Asian
- Black/African American
- Native Hawaiian/Other Pacific Islander
- White
- Prefer not to say
- Other:___________
References
- Rajaram, S.; Pereira-Pasarin, L. Collaborative Memory: Cognitive Research and Theory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 5, 649–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pociask, S.; Rajaram, S. The effects of collaborative practice on statistical problem solving: Benefits and boundaries. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2014, 3, 252–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bjork, R.A.; Dunlosky, J.; Kornell, N. Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 417–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Persellin, D.; Daniels, M.; Reder, M. A Concise Guide to Improving Student Learning; Stylus Publishing, LLC.: Sterling, VA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Wei, Y.; Charbonneau, B.Z.; Meade, M.L.; Hutchison, K.A. Examining the time course of post collaborative benefits across word lists and prose passages. Mem. Cogn. 2024, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blumen, H.M.; Rajaram, S. Influence of re-exposure and retrieval disruption during group collaboration on later individual recall. Memory 2008, 16, 231–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rajaram, S. Collaborative Inhibition in Group Recall: Cognitive Principles and Implications. In Collaborative Remembering: Theories, Research, and Applications; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Weldon, M.S.; Bellinger, K.D. Collective memory: Collaborative and individual processes in remembering. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1997, 23, 1160–1175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meade, M.L.; Roediger, H.L. Explorations in the Social Contagion of Memory. Mem. Cogn. 2002, 30, 995–1009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gabbert, F.; Wheeler, R. Memory conformity following collaborative remembering. In Collaborative Remembering: Theories, Research, and Applications; Meade, M.L., Harris, C.B., Van Bergen, P., Sutton, J., Barnier, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Hirst, W.; Yamashiro, J. Social aspects of forgetting. In Collaborative Remembering: Theories, Research, and Applications; Meade, M.L., Harris, C.B., Van Bergen, P., Sutton, J., Barnier, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Biasutti, M.; Frate, S. Group metacognition in online collaborative learning: Validity and reliability of the group metacognition scale (GMS). Educ. Tech Res. Dev 2018, 66, 1321–1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flavell, J.H. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. Am. Psychol. 1979, 34, 906–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, A. Metacognition and Consciousness. In The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness; Zelazo, P., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 289–325. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson, T.O.; Narens, L. Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Bower, G.H., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1990; pp. 1–45. [Google Scholar]
- Dunlosky, J.; Metcalfe, J. Metacognition; Sage Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes, M.G. Judgements of Learning: Methods, data, and Theory. In The Oxford Handbook of Metamemory; Dunlosky, J., Tauber, S.K., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Soderstrom, N.C.; Yue, C.L.; Bjork, E.L. Metamemory and education. In The Oxford Handbook of Metamemory; Dunlosky, J., Tauber, S.K., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Blasiman, R.; Dunlosky, J.; Rawson, K. The what, how much, and when of study strategies: Comparing intended versus actual study behaviour. Memory 2017, 25, 784–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karpicke, J.D.; Butler, A.C.; Roediger, H.L., III. Metacognitive strategies in student learning: Do students practice retrieval when they study on their own? Memory 2009, 17, 471–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gurung RA, R. How Do Students Really Study (and Does It Matter)? Teach. Psychol. 2005, 32, 239–241. [Google Scholar]
- Hartwig, M.K.; Dunlosky, J. Study strategies of college students: Are self-testing and scheduling related to achievement? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2012, 19, 126–134. Available online: https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/study-strategies-college-students-are-self/docview/1021977684/se-2 (accessed on 5 October 2024). [CrossRef]
- Roediger, H.L., III; Karpicke, J.D. Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 249–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rawson, K.A.; Dunlosky, J. When is practice testing most effective for improving the durability and efficiency of student learning? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 24, 419–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wissman, K.T.; Rawson, K.A. How do students implement collaborative testing in real-world contexts? Memory 2015, 24, 223–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rybczynski, S.M.; Schussler, E.E. Student use of out-of-class study groups in an introductory undergraduate biology course. CBE Life Sci. Educ. 2011, 10, 74–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cortright, R.N.; Collins, H.L.; Rodenbaugh, D.W.; DiCarlo, S.E. Student retention of course content is improved by collaborative-group testing. Adv. Physiol. Educ. 2003, 27, 102–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whillock, S.R.; Meade, M.L.; Hutchison, K.A.; Tsosie, M.D. Collaborative inhibition in same-age and mixed-age dyads. Psychol. Aging 2020, 35, 963–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCabe, J.A.; Lummis, S.N. Why and how do undergraduates study in groups? Scholarsh. Teach. Learn. Psychol. 2018, 4, 27–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dimmock, J.; Krause, A.E.; Rebar, A.; Jackson, B. Relationships between social interactions, basic psychological needs, and wellbeing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Health 2021, 37, 457–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Besser, A.; Flett, G.L.; Zeigler-Hill, V. Adaptability to a sudden transition to online learning during the COVID-19 pandemic: Understanding the challenges for students. Scholarsh. Teach. Learn. Psychol. 2022, 8, 85–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenfeld, D.L.; Balcetis, E.; Bastian, B.; Berkman, E.T.; Bosson, J.K.; Brannon, T.N.; Burrow, A.L.; Cameron, C.D.; Chen, S.; Cook, J.E.; et al. Psychological Science in the Wake of COVID-19: Social, Methodological, and Metascientific Considerations. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 17, 311–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCabe, J.A. Metacognitive awareness of learning strategies in undergraduates. Mem. Cogn. 2011, 39, 462–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Syed, M.; Nelson, S.C. Guidelines for Establishing Reliability When Coding Narrative Data. Emerg. Adulthood 2015, 3, 375–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landis, J.R.; Koch, G.G. The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics 1977, 33, 159–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Hillsdale, N.J., Ed.; L. Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Gravetter, F.J.; Wallnau, L.B. Essentials of Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 8th ed.; Wadsworth/Cengage: Belmont, CA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Chi-Square Test Calculator. (2024, September 10). Available online: https://www.socscistatistics.com/tests/chisquare2/default2.aspx (accessed on 5 October 2024).
- Hart, K.M.; Meade, M.L. Social contagion of memory and the role of self-initiated relative judgments. Acta Psychol. 2021, 212, 103189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Numbers, K.T.; Barnier, A.J.; Harris, C.B.; Meade, M.L. Ageing stereotypes influence the transmission of false memories in the social contagion paradigm. Memory 2019, 27, 368–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winne, P.H. Modeling self-regulated learning as learners doing learning science: How trace data and learning analytics help develop skills for self-regulated learning. Metacognition Learn. 2022, 17, 773–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
(a) | |||||
Strategy | Percent who list strategy | Percent who rank as #1 strategy | Mean rank | ||
Re-reading notes and textbooks | 69.8 | (141) | 32.7 | (66) | 1.8 |
Flashcards | 35.6 | (72) | 10.4 | (21) | 2.3 |
Do practice questions | 34.1 | (69) | 11.9 | (21) | 2.1 |
Practice recall/Quizlet | 31.2 | (63) | 12.9 | (26) | 2.0 |
Watching videos/outside resources | 28.7 | (58) | 5.9 | (12) | 2.5 |
Review sheet | 27.7 | (56) | 8.9 | (18) | 2.3 |
Re-write notes | 20.8 | (42) | 9.9 | (20) | 1.8 |
Memorize/repetition | 20.3 | (41) | 8.9 | (18) | 2.0 |
Focus on wrong answers/unclear information | 9.9 | (20) | 0.5 | (1) | 2.9 |
Mnemonics/association | 6.9 | (14) | 1.5 | (3) | 2.4 |
Highlight (in notes/books) | 5.9 | (12) | 1.5 | (3) | 2.2 |
Create a good environment | 5.9 | (12) | 3.5 | (7) | 1.6 |
Spaced repetition/interleaving | 4.0 | (8) | 1.0 | (2) | 2 |
Think of real life examples | 2.5 | (5) | 0.5 | (1) | 2.8 |
(b) | |||||
Strategy | Percent who list strategy | Percent who rank as #1 strategy | Mean rank | ||
Teach each other/discussion | 44.1 | (89) | 21.2 | (43) | 1.7 |
Quiz each other | 40.1 | (82) | 13.9 | (28) | 2.0 |
Ask for help/answers | 22.3 | (45) | 10.4 | (21) | 1.8 |
Flashcards | 20.8 | (42) | 9.4 | (19) | 1.9 |
Compare answers/notes | 20.3 | (41) | 7.4 | (15) | 2.0 |
Re-reading notes/textbooks | 20.3 | (41) | 5.9 | (12) | 2.3 |
Do practice problems | 18.3 | (37) | 9.4 | (19) | 1.8 |
Make review sheets/study guide | 18.3 | (37) | 9.9 | (20) | 1.6 |
Practice recall/Quizlet | 8.9 | (18) | 2.5 | (5) | 2.3 |
Focus on wrong answers | 7.9 | (16) | 3.0 | (6) | 1.9 |
Re-write notes | 7.9 | (16) | 1.0 | (2) | 2.8 |
Watch videos/ask professors/outside resources | 6.4 | (13) | 1.5 | (3) | 2.8 |
Group focuses on one topic | 4.0 | (10) | 1.0 | (4) | 2 |
Mnemonics/association | 3.5 | (7) | 1.0 | (2) | 2.3 |
Characteristics | Percent Who List Strategy | |
---|---|---|
Focused/task oriented | 56.4 | (114) |
Responsible/hard-working/reliable | 39.1 | (79) |
Motivated/desire to participate | 35.1 | (71) |
Flexible/receptive/cooperative | 23.8 | (48) |
Helpful/patient | 19.8 | (40) |
Kind/considerate | 13.9 | (28) |
Easy-going/talkative/fun | 10.9 | (22) |
Communication skills/organized | 8.9 | (18) |
Have similar goals | 5.4 | (11) |
Humble/willing to make mistakes | 5.4 | (11) |
Similar knowledge or skills | 5.0 | (10) |
Similar/equal contribution | 4.5 | (9) |
Leadership | 4.0 | (8) |
Skeptical/critical | 3.0 | (6) |
Frequently/Moderately Group | Rarely/Never group | ||
---|---|---|---|
Beliefs about studying | Study in groups is more effective | 38 | 64 |
Study individually is more effective | 13 | 87 | |
Beliefs about recalling | Recalling in groups is more effective | 36 | 84 |
Recalling individually is more effective | 15 | 67 | |
Studying preference | Study in groups for exams | 32 | 28 |
Study individually for exams | 15 | 97 | |
Favored characteristics of other group members | Focused | 26 | 114 |
Motivated | 17 | 71 | |
Reliable | 22 | 79 | |
Flexible | 23 | 48 | |
Helpful | 22 | 40 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wei, Y.; Soderstrom, N.C.; Meade, M.L.; Scott, B.G. Metacognition About Collaborative Learning: Students’ Beliefs Are Inconsistent with Their Learning Preferences. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 1104. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111104
Wei Y, Soderstrom NC, Meade ML, Scott BG. Metacognition About Collaborative Learning: Students’ Beliefs Are Inconsistent with Their Learning Preferences. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(11):1104. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111104
Chicago/Turabian StyleWei, Yunfeng, Nicholas C. Soderstrom, Michelle L. Meade, and Brandon G. Scott. 2024. "Metacognition About Collaborative Learning: Students’ Beliefs Are Inconsistent with Their Learning Preferences" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 11: 1104. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111104
APA StyleWei, Y., Soderstrom, N. C., Meade, M. L., & Scott, B. G. (2024). Metacognition About Collaborative Learning: Students’ Beliefs Are Inconsistent with Their Learning Preferences. Behavioral Sciences, 14(11), 1104. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111104