Using Eye Tracking to Reveal Responses to the Built Environment and Its Constituents
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Software
2.2. Image Presentation and Data Acquisition
2.3. Content of the 7 Studies
- Building Study-1 compared images of traditional and modern civic architectures. The images were provided by the National Civic Art Society (NCAS.org) and originally used in a 2020-NCAS-sponsored Harris Poll, in exactly the same pairings as presented in our eye-tracking study. Seventy-six people completed the study, with 60 responses meeting the quality standards for data processing. 182 people abandoned the study without completing it.
- Building Study-2 used photographs provided by Nikola Olic, a Texas-based photographer (structurephotography.org) whose pictures of buildings, he calls ‘Architectural Portraiture’, have appeared in the New York Times. Sixty-eight people completed the study, with 62 responses meeting the quality standards for data processing; 115 people abandoned the study without completing it.
- Building Study-3 looked at the impact of texture in architectural images, comparing images of buildings with processed versions of the same structure with and without detail. Fifty-six people completed the study, with 56 responses meeting the quality standards for data processing; 85 people abandoned the study without completing it.
- Building Study-4 further explored how people view buildings versus natural settings using photographs from the US, Canada and Europe. 50 people completed the study, with 49 responses meeting the quality standards for data processing; 124 people abandoned the study without completing it.
- Building Study-5 focused on the importance of fenestration and architectural detail, presenting processed versions of buildings from the United States, Greece and Italy, with windows or architectural features removed. Fifty-four people completed the study, with 52 responses meeting the quality standards for data processing; 92 people abandoned the study without completing it.
- Building Study-6 further explored the importance of detail, fenestration and face-like features in architecture and other images. Fifty-one people completed the study, with 48 responses meeting the quality standards for data processing; 98 people abandoned the study without completing it.
- Building Study-7 used architectural photographs from Brazil, Finland and Greece and artifacts with and without face-like features. Seventy-one people completed the study, with 54 responses meeting the quality standards for data processing; 210 people abandoned the study without completing it.
3. Results
3.1. Study 1
3.2. Study 2
3.3. Study 3
3.4. Study 4
3.5. Study 5
3.6. Study 6
3.7. Study 7
3.8. Results Summary
4. Discussion
Potential Shortcomings
5. Conclusions and Future Prospects
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Lisińska-Kuśnierz, M.; Krupa, M. Suitability of Eye Tracking in Assessing the Visual Perception of Architecture-A Case Study Concerning Selected Projects Located in Cologne. Buildings 2020, 10, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sussman, A.; Ward, J. Eye-tracking Boston City Hall to better understand human perception and the architectural experience. New Design Ideas 2019, 3, 53–59. [Google Scholar]
- Zou, Z.; Yu, X.; Ergan, S. Integrating Biometric Sensors, VR, and Machine Learning to Classify EEG Signals in Alternative Architecture Designs. Comput. Civ. Eng. 2019 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Lavdas, A.A.; Salingaros, N.A. Architectural Beauty: Developing a Measurable and Objective Scale. Challenges 2022, 13, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lavdas, A.A.; Salingaros, N.A.; Sussman, A. Visual Attention Software: A New Tool for Understanding the “Subliminal” Experience of the Built Environment. Appl. Sci. 2021, 11, 6197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sussman, A.; Ward, J. Game-Changing Eye-Tracking Studies Reveal How We Actually See Architecture. Available online: https://commonedge.org/game-changing-eye-tracking-studies-reveal-how-we-actually-see-architecture/ (accessed on 1 August 2023).
- Itti, L.; Koch, C. Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 2, 194–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tollner, T.; Zehetleitner, M.; Gramann, K.; Muller, H.J. Stimulus saliency modulates pre-attentive processing speed in human visual cortex. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e16276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellard, C. Places of the Heart: The Psychogeography of Everyday Life; Perseus Books LLC, Ed.; Perseus: New York City, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Merrifield, C.; Danckert, J. Characterizing the psychophysiological signature of boredom. Exp. Brain Res. 2014, 232, 481–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alexander, C.; Ishikawa, S.; Silverstein, M.; Jacobson, M.; Fiksdahl King, I.; Angel, S. A Pattern Language; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, R.P. Reduction of Physiological Stress Using Fractal Art and Architecture. Leonardo 2006, 39, 245–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, R.P.; Spehar, B.; Van Donkelaar, P.; Hagerhall, C.M. Perceptual and Physiological Responses to Jackson Pollock’s Fractals. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mandelbrot, B.B. How long is the coast of Britain? Statistical self-similarity and fractional dimension. Science 1967, 156, 636–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischmeister, F.P.; Martins, M.J.D.; Beisteiner, R.; Fitch, W.T. Self-similarity and recursion as default modes in human cognition. Cortex 2017, 97, 183–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martins, M.J.; Fischmeister, F.P.; Puig-Waldmuller, E.; Oh, J.; Geissler, A.; Robinson, S.; Fitch, W.T.; Beisteiner, R. Fractal image perception provides novel insights into hierarchical cognition. Neuroimage 2014, 96, 300–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ulrich, R.S. View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 1984, 224, 417–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frumkin, H. Beyond toxicity: Human health and the natural environment. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2001, 20, 234–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Joye, Y. Architectural lessons from environmental psychology: The case of biophilic architecture. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2007, 11, 305–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salingaros, N.A. The laws of architecture from a physicist’s perspective. Phys. Essays 1995, 8, 638–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salingaros, N.A. A Scientific Basis for Creating Architectural Forms. J. Arch. Plan. Res. 1998, 15, 283–293. [Google Scholar]
- Salingaros, N.A.; Mehaffy, M.W. A Theory of Architecture; Umbau-Verlag: Solingen, Germany, 2006; 278p. [Google Scholar]
- Salingaros, N.A. Unified Architectural Theory: Form, Language, Complexity: A Companion to Christopher Alexander’s “The Phenomenon of Life: The Nature of Order, Book 1”; Sustasis Foundation: Portland, OR, USA, 2013; 300p. [Google Scholar]
- Zeki, S. Beauty in Architecture: Not a Luxury—Only a Necessity. Arch. Des. 2019, 89, 14–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- NCAS. Americans’ Preferred Architecture for Federal Buildings. Available online: https://www.civicart.org/americans-preferred-architecture-for-federal-buildings (accessed on 1 August 2023).
- iMotions. iMotions. Available online: https://imotions.com/ (accessed on 21 October 2023).
- Lavdas, A.A.; Schirpke, U. Aesthetic preference is related to organized complexity. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0235257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albright, T.D. Neuroscience for Architecture. In Mind in Architecture: Neuroscience, Embodiment, and the Future of Design; Robinson, S., Pallasmaa, J., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2015; pp. 197–217. [Google Scholar]
- Chatterjee, A.; Coburn, A.; Weinberger, A. The neuroaesthetics of architectural spaces. Cogn. Process. 2021, 22, 115–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coburn, A.; Vartanian, O.; Chatterjee, A. Buildings, Beauty, and the Brain: A Neuroscience of Architectural Experience. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 29, 1521–1531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grabowski, B. “P < 0.05” Might Not Mean What You Think: American Statistical Association Clarifies P Values. J. Natl. Cancer Inst. 2016, 108, djw194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Slater, A.; Schulenburg, C.V.D.; Brown, E.; Badenoch, M. Newborn infants prefer attractive faces. Infant Behav. Dev. 1998, 21, 345–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Langlois, J.H.; Ritter, J.M.; Roggman, L.A.; Vaughn, L.S. Facial diversity and infant preferences for attractive faces. Dev. Psychol. 1991, 21, 79–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Le, Q.; Isbell, L.A.; Matsumoto, J.; Nguyen, M.; Hori, E.; Maior, R.S.; Tomaz, C.; Tran, A.H.; Ono, T.; Nishijo, H. Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 19000–19005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bulf, H.; de Hevia, M.D.; Gariboldi, V.; Cassia, V.M. Infants learn better from left to right: A directional bias in infants’ sequence learning. Sci. Rep.-Uk 2017, 7, 2437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rugani, R.; Kelly, D.M.; Szelest, I.; Regolin, L.; Vallortigara, G. Is it only humans that count from left to right? Biol. Lett. 2010, 6, 290–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shemesh, A. Affective Response to Architecture—Investigating Human Reaction to Spaces with Different Geometry. Arch. Sci. Rev. 2016, 60, 116–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hu, M.; Simon, M.; Fix, S.; Vivino, A.A.; Bernat, E. Exploring a sustainable building’s impact on occupant mental health and cognitive function in a virtual environment. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 5644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valentine, C. Health Implications of Virtual Architecture: An Interdisciplinary Exploration of the Transferability of Findings from Neuroarchitecture. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 2735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Rosas, H.J.; Sussman, A.; Sekely, A.C.; Lavdas, A.A. Using Eye Tracking to Reveal Responses to the Built Environment and Its Constituents. Appl. Sci. 2023, 13, 12071. https://doi.org/10.3390/app132112071
Rosas HJ, Sussman A, Sekely AC, Lavdas AA. Using Eye Tracking to Reveal Responses to the Built Environment and Its Constituents. Applied Sciences. 2023; 13(21):12071. https://doi.org/10.3390/app132112071
Chicago/Turabian StyleRosas, Hernan J., Ann Sussman, Abigail C. Sekely, and Alexandros A. Lavdas. 2023. "Using Eye Tracking to Reveal Responses to the Built Environment and Its Constituents" Applied Sciences 13, no. 21: 12071. https://doi.org/10.3390/app132112071
APA StyleRosas, H. J., Sussman, A., Sekely, A. C., & Lavdas, A. A. (2023). Using Eye Tracking to Reveal Responses to the Built Environment and Its Constituents. Applied Sciences, 13(21), 12071. https://doi.org/10.3390/app132112071