Is Visual Creativity Embodied? Thinking Aloud While Performing the Creative Mental Synthesis Task
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Method
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials and Procedure
2.3. Scoring Procedures
- (1) Motor thoughts had to include explicit motion-related verbs associated with stimuli manipulation and object use (e.g., move, shift, rotate, put, push, use, do) as well as all statements related to functionality of objects (e.g., it could be function as a screwdriver), based on the idea that motion-related information was implicit in such statements;
- (2) Spatial thoughts had to include information containing spatial locatives (e.g., up/down, left/right, below/above, in/out, near/far, front/back) and spatial transformation (e.g., changes in size and rotation, embedding);
- (3) Visual thoughts had to include information referred to basic visual features (e.g., rectangular block, sphere, cross), objects and parts of them imagined or created (e.g., water, glass, lamp, hat, car), colors (e.g., blue, red, white), materials (e.g., plastic, iron), and any other pictorial aspect (e.g., bright, dark);
- (4) Emotional thoughts had to include any statement related to emotions (sadness, happiness, disgust, anger, surprise, fear) but also moods, such a s anxiety, discomfort, irritation, as well as appreciation, comfort, and pleasure;
- (5) Reasoning thoughts had to include statements related to factual (e.g., if I do this then the object will be more sharp), counterfactual (e.g., if I had done like that, the object would have been better), or analytical (e.g., apply logic to find patterns or make inferences).
2.4. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Csikszentmihalyi, M. Implications of a systems perspective for the study of creativity. In Handbook of Creativity; Sternberg, R.J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 313–335. [Google Scholar]
- Forgeard, M.J.C.; Mecklenburg, A.C. The two dimensions of motivation and a reciprocal model of the creative process. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2013, 17, 255–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stein, M.I. Creativity and culture. J. Psychol. 1953, 36, 31–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simonton, D.K. Taking the U.S. patent office criteria seriously: A quantitative three-criterion creativity definition and its implications. Creat. Res. J. 2012, 24, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Runco, M.A. Creativity research: Originality, utility, and integration. Creat. Res. J. 1988, 1, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mace, M.A.; Ward, T. Modeling the Creative Process: A Grounded Theory Analysis of Creativity in the Domain of Art Making. Creat. Res. J. 2002, 14, 179–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weiss, M.; Hoegl, M.; Gibbert, M. Perceptions of Material Resources in Innovation Projects: What Shapes Them and How Do They Matter? Perceptions of Material Resources. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 2014, 31, 278–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varela, F.; Thompson, E.; Rosch, E. The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Malinin, L.H. How Radical Is Embodied Creativity? Implications of 4E approaches for creativity research and teaching. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frith, E.; Miller, S.; Loprinzi, P.D. A review of experimental research on embodied creativity: Revisiting the mind–body connection. J. Creat. Behav. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallagher, S. How embodied cognition is being disembodied. Philos. Magaz. 2015, 68, 96–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gibson, J.J. The theory of affordances. In Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology; Shaw, R., Bransford, J., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1977; pp. 67–82. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, J.J. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception; Mifflin and Company: Boston, MA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Suchman, L.A. Human-Machine Reconfigurations: Plans and Situated Actions, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, N.J.T. Are theories of imagery theories of imagination? An active perception approach to conscious mental content. Cogn. Sci. 1999, 23, 207–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, N.J.T. Visual imagery and consciousness. In Encyclopedia of Consciousness; Banks, W.P., Ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Academic Press: Oxford, UK, 2009; Volume 2, pp. 445–457. [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, N.J.T. The multidimensional spectrum of imagination: Images, dreams, hallucinations, and active, imaginative perception. Humanities 2014, 3, 132–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clark, A.; Chalmers, D. The extended mind. Analysis 1998, 58, 7–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foglia, L.; O’Regan, K.J. A new imagery debate: Enactive and sensorimotor accounts. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 2016, 7, 181–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palmiero, M.; Piccardi, L.; Giancola, M.; Nori, R.; D’Amico, S.; Olivetti Belardinelli, M. The format of mental imagery: From a critical review to an integrated embodied representation approach. Cogn. Process. 2019, 20, 277–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yaneva, A. Made by the Office for Metropolitan Architecture: An Ethnography of Design; 010 Publishers: Rotterdam, The Netherlands, 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Noë, A. Action in Perception; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Noë, A. Vision without representation. In Perception, Action, and Consciousness: Sensorimotor Dynamics and Two Visual Systems; Gangopadhya, N., Madary, M., Spicer, F., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 245–256. [Google Scholar]
- O’Regan, J.K.; Noë, A. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav. Brain. Sci. 2001, 24, 883–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barsalou, L.W. Perceptual symbol systems. Behav. Brain. Sci. 1999, 22, 577–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barsalou, L.W. Grounded cognition. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2008, 59, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hesslow, G. The current status of the simulation theory of cognition. Brain Res. 2012, 1428, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Palmiero, M. Imagery. In Encyclopedia of Creativity, 3rd ed.; Runco, M., Pritzker, S., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2020; pp. 612–616. [Google Scholar]
- Matheson, H.E.; Kenett, Y.N. The role of the motor system in generating creative thoughts. NeuroImage 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palmiero, M.; Giulianella, L.; Guariglia, P.; Boccia, M.; D’Amico, S.; Piccardi, L. The dancers’ visuo-spatial body map explains their enhanced divergence in the production of motor forms: Evidence in the early development. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kuo, C.Y.; Yeh, Y.Y. Sensorimotor-Conceptual integration in free walking enhances divergent thinking for young and older adults. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Oppezzo, M.; Schwartz, D.L. Give your ideas some legs: The positive effect of walking on creative thinking. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2014, 40, 1142–1152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leung, A.K.-Y.; Kim, S.; Polman, E.; Ong, L.S.; Qiu, L.; Goncalo, J.A.; Sanchez-Burks, J. Embodied metaphors and creative “acts”. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 502–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Benedek, M.; Jauk, E.; Sommer, M.; Arendasy, M.; Neubauer, A.C. Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence 2014, 46, 73–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Matheson, H.E.; Buxbaum, L.J.; Thompson-Schill, S.L. Differential tuning of ventral and dorsal streams during the generation of common and uncommon tool uses. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 29, 1791–1802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wu, X.; Yang, W.; Tong, D.; Sun, J.; Chen, Q.; Wei, D.; Zhang, Q.; Zhang, M.; Qiu, J. A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on divergent thinking using activation likelihood estimation. Hum. Brain Map. 2015, 36, 2703–2718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benedek, M.; Jauk, E.; Fink, A.; Koschutnig, K.; Reishofer, G.; Ebner, F.; Neubauer, A.C. To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage 2014, 88, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tian, F.; Chen, Q.; Zhu, W.; Wang, Y.; Yang, W.; Zhu, X.; Tian, X.; Zhang, Q.; Cao, G.; Qiu, J. The association between visual creativity and cortical thickness in healthy adults. Neurosci. Lett. 2018, 683, 104–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beaty, R.E. The neuroscience of musical improvisation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 51, 108–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Boccia, M.; Piccardi, L.; Palermo, L.; Nori, R.; Palmiero, M. Where do bright ideas occur in our brain? Meta-analytic evidence from neuroimaging studies of domain-specific creativity. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Boasen, J.; Takeshita, Y.; Kuriki, S.; Yokosawa, K. Spectral-spatial differentiation of brain activity during mental imagery of improvisational music performance using MEG. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Aziz-Zadeh, L.; Liew, S.L.; Dandekar, F. Exploring the neural correlates of visual creativity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013, 8, 475–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sack, A.T.; Jacobs, C.; De Martino, F.; Staeren, N.; Goebel, R.; Formisano, E. Dynamic premotor-to-parietal interactions during spatial imagery. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 8417–8429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kimmel, M.A. Cognitive theory of joint improvisation: The case of tango argentino. In Handbook of Dance Improvisation; Midgelow, V., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Finke, R.A. Imagery, Creativity, and Emergent Structure. Conscious. Cogn. 1996, 5, 381–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finke, R.A.; Ward, T.M.; Smith, S.M. Creative Cognition: Theory, Research, and Applications; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Palmiero, M.; Nori, R.; Piccardi, L. Visualizer cognitive style enhances visual creativity. Neurosci. Lett. 2016, 615, 98–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palmiero, M.; Cardi, V.; Olivetti Belardinelli, M. The Role of vividness of visual mental imagery on different dimensions of creativity. Creat. Res. J. 2011, 23, 372–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palmiero, M.; Nakatani, C.; Raver, D.; Olivetti Belardinelli, M.; Van Leeuwen, C. Abilities within and across visual and verbal domains: How specific is their influence on creativity? Creat. Res. J. 2010, 22, 369–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shepard, R.N. Externalization of mental images and the act of creation. In Visual Learning, Thinking, and Communication; Randhawa, B.S., Coffman, W.E., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Sawyer, R.K. Explaining Creativity: The Science of Human Innovation, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Van Someren, M.W.; Barnard, Y.F.; Sandberg, J.A.C. Think-Aloud Method: A Practical Guide to Modelling Cognitive Processes; Academic Press: London, UK, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Ericsson, K.A.; Simon, H. Verbal reports as data. Psychol. Rev. 1980, 87, 215–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ericsson, K.A.; Simon, H.A. Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Gilhooly, K.J.; Fioratou, E.; Anthony, S.H.; Wynn, V. Divergent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. Br. J. Psychol. 2007, 98, 611–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dorst, K.; Cross, N. Creativity in the design process: Co-evolution of problem–solution. Design Stud. 2001, 22, 425–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pringle, A.; Sowden, P.T. Unearthing the creative thinking process: Fresh insights from a think-aloud study of garden design. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2017, 11, 344–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boldt, G. Artistic creativity beyond divergent thinking: Analyzing sequences in creative subprocesses. Think. Skills Creat. 2019, 34, 100606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finke, R.A. Creative Imagery: Discoveries and Inventions in Visualization; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Gu, Y. To code or not to code: Dilemmas in analyzing think-aloud protocols in learning strategies research. System 2014, 43, 74–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amabile, T.M. Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 43, 997–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hair, J.F.; Black, W.C.; Babin, B.J.; Anderson, R.E.; Tatham, R.L. Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th ed.; Pearson: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Sheather, S. A Modern Approach to Regression With R; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Palmiero, M.; Nori, R.; Aloisi, V.; Ferrara, M.; Piccardi, L. Domain-specificity of creativity: A study on the relationship between visual creativity and visual mental imagery. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kim, J. Physical activity benefits creativity: Squeezing a ball for enhancing creativity. Creat. Res. J. 2015, 27, 328–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stevens, J.A. Interference effects demonstrate distinct roles for visual and motor imagery during the mental representation of human action. Cognition 2005, 95, 329–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guillot, A.; Collet, C.; Nguyen, V.A.; Malouin, F.; Richards, C.; Doyon, J. Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: An fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 2157–2172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Mean | S.D. | Min | Max | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Originality | 1.3 | 1.1 | 0 | 4.7 |
Appropriateness | 3.3 | 0.90 | 0.50 | 5 |
Pre-motor | 0.53 | 0.29 | 0.08 | 1.6 |
Pre-spatial | 0.81 | 0.56 | 0.06 | 2.4 |
Pre-visual | 3.3 | 1.2 | 1.1 | 7.6 |
Post-motor | 0.54 | 0.34 | 0.08 | 1.3 |
Post-spatial | 1.4 | 0.76 | 0 | 4 |
Post-visual | 3.8 | 1.1 | 1 | 6 |
Pre-Motor | Pre-Spatial | Pre-Visual | Post-Motor | Post-Spatial | Post-Visual | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pre-motor | 1 | |||||
Pre-spatial | 0.467 ** | 1 | ||||
Pre-visual | 0.370 * | 0.522 ** | 1 | |||
Post-motor | 0.094 | 0.152 | −0.054 | 1 | ||
Post-spatial | 0.212 | 0.281 | 0.016 | 0.257 | 1 | |
Post-visual | 0.340 * | 0.416 ** | 0.434 ** | 0.151 | 0.368 * | 1 |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Palmiero, M.; Piccardi, L. Is Visual Creativity Embodied? Thinking Aloud While Performing the Creative Mental Synthesis Task. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 455. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10070455
Palmiero M, Piccardi L. Is Visual Creativity Embodied? Thinking Aloud While Performing the Creative Mental Synthesis Task. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(7):455. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10070455
Chicago/Turabian StylePalmiero, Massimiliano, and Laura Piccardi. 2020. "Is Visual Creativity Embodied? Thinking Aloud While Performing the Creative Mental Synthesis Task" Brain Sciences 10, no. 7: 455. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10070455
APA StylePalmiero, M., & Piccardi, L. (2020). Is Visual Creativity Embodied? Thinking Aloud While Performing the Creative Mental Synthesis Task. Brain Sciences, 10(7), 455. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10070455