Next Article in Journal
Age-Related Changes in Attentional Refocusing during Simulated Driving
Next Article in Special Issue
Erratum: Macchitella et al. Sleepiness, Neuropsychological Skills, and Scholastic Learning in Children. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 529
Previous Article in Journal
Evaluation of Cerebral Microvascular Regulatory Mechanisms with Transcranial Doppler in Fabry Disease
 
 
Erratum published on 6 July 2021, see Brain Sci. 2021, 11(7), 892.
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sleepiness, Neuropsychological Skills, and Scholastic Learning in Children

Department of History, Society and Human Studies, Lab of Applied Psychology and Intervention, University of Salento, 73100 Lecce, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2020, 10(8), 529; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080529
Submission received: 30 June 2020 / Revised: 3 August 2020 / Accepted: 3 August 2020 / Published: 7 August 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sleep Disturbance and Cognition)

Abstract

:
Excessive daytime sleepiness is a frequent condition among children and adolescents that may lead to several and significant daytime consequences, including impaired neurocognitive skills and scholastic performance. Here, we evaluated in one hundred and ninety-one unselected primary school children, the relationship between sleepiness and a wide range of cognitive and academic skills through a standardized neuropsychological test battery. In order to assess the statistical relationship, we performed a partial least squares path modelling, a non-parametrical approach which combined a model of paths between latent variables and the coefficients between indicators and dimensions. Results were validated through the bootstrap approach and suggest that sleepiness is not associated with all cognitive and scholastic abilities, but only with those relying on verbal abilities and complex cognitive functions (i.e., reading comprehension, oral/syntactic comprehension, spelling, and mathematic skills). Our data suggest the idea that sleepiness in children is associated mostly with “higher” (mainly verbal) cognitive function(s), while the visuospatial domain was not affected.

1. Introduction

Several lines of research point out that sleep may influence (both positively and negatively) neurocognitive development as well as cognitive skills and scholastic achievement in children and adolescents [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8]. Sleepiness is the difficulty in maintaining a desired level of wakefulness or arousal [2,5], while excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is defined as an increased tendency to fall asleep in a setting where an individual would be expected to stay awake and alert [9]. “Excessive daytime sleepiness represents a common but often under-recognized phenomenon in children” [10]. Indeed, many studies reported a high rate of EDS among children and adolescents, ranging from 10% to 47% [11]. At the same time, however, EDS is underreported by parents and underdiagnosed by physicians, possibly due to lack of recognition [11]. EDS may increase risky behaviours (such as motor vehicle accidents in adolescents) and it may be associated to impaired cognitive and scholastic performance [12,13]. There are numerous and complex biopsychosocial conditions that may cause (independently, jointly or through their interactions) excessive daytime sleepiness [9,10,11,12,14]. They may be conceptualized under the following broad categories: Insufficient sleep duration (due, for instance, to poor sleep hygiene as well as biological factors; see also below), fragmented/disturbed sleep (caused by, for example, medical problems such as gastroesophageal reflux as well as sleep disorders such as sleep-disordered breathing (SDB)), circadian misalignment (e.g., delay sleep-wake phase syndrome), primary disorders that increase sleep needs (e.g., depression, narcolepsy, hypothalamic lesions) [9,11] (Morse and Kothare, 2019; Owens et al., 2020), and intrinsic development related to changes in biological systems (i.e., homeostasis and circadian systems) that regulate timing of sleep and awakening [15,16,17]. Finally, there are several “environmental and lifestyle factors” that may induce EDS, such as, for example, school starting times and staying awake until late to finish homework (that may induce a reduction of sleep duration) [18,19], excessive consumption of food and drink containing caffeine [20,21,22], pre-sleep activities (e.g., the use of television, tablet, and video games may induce both delay in bedtime and shortens sleeping hours as well as impaired sleep latency and sleep architecture) [22,23,24,25,26,27] as well as more trivial but still important factors (environmental stimuli such as noise, light, temperature during sleep time) (Morse and Kothare, 2019). Notably, sleepiness associated with insufficient sleep duration may arise from complex interactions between environmental factors (e.g., school starting time) [9] and intrinsic development related to changes in biological systems that regulate sleep time [14,17,28]
Considering that EDS is a frequent condition among children and adolescents that may have several and important daytime consequences, EDS has become an important international health and societal concern. The scientific and media attention on this problem has already influenced some public policy, school, and clinical practice. For example, pediatric health care professionals were required to inquire about sleep and the biopsychosocial causes underling EDS in their patients [11]. Moreover, considering the “environmental and lifestyle factors” that may induce EDS, Minges and Redeker stressed that “given the public advocacy for delayed school start time, the recent policy statement from American Academy of Paediatrics to delay the start of class to 8:30 a.m. or later and some estimates that over 80 U.S. school district have already adopted later school start time” [18]. Finally, the lockdown during COVID-19 pandemic has made EDS a very topical issue. Indeed, the lockdown has affected several habits in children and adolescents, some of which are related to sleep habits, such as an excessive use of video games, computer or mobile telephones that may increase daytime sleepiness [29,30,31,32]. For example, in Italy a 70% increase in Fortnite-gaming related internet traffic was reported [33].
What are the predictable effects of sleepiness on cognitive and scholastic performance in children and adolescents? Several reviews stressed the impact of ESD on cognition and scholastic performance in children [5,10,13]. However, in some cases, assumptions about the relationship between sleepiness and both cognition and school performances arise indirectly. Most of the studies investigated the effects of primary conditions leading to sleepiness (e.g., sleep disorders, poor sleep quality or quantity assessed, for instance, via actigraphy) on cognitive skills and scholastic achievement. Accordingly, these studies found that sleep disorders and poor sleep quality or quantity may adversely affect intelligence, executive functions, attention, visuo-motor skills, working memory, long term memory and learning, as well as the academic skills such as reading, reading comprehension, spelling, and arithmetic skills [4,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46]. Furthermore, studies that experimentally manipulated sleep duration in children and adolescents, introducing some form of restrictions/deprivation, highlighted decreased cognitive performances [5,47,48,49,50,51,52] as well as increased school difficulties [5,53,54]. In sum, in many of such studies it was found that children with insufficient or impaired sleep were sleepier during the day and displayed poorer cognitive and scholastic performances. This evidence suggested a “direct” association between sleepiness and decremented cognitive and/or school skills [5,10,12,13]. Crucially, however, in many of these studies the relationship between children sleepiness and cognitive performance was taken for granted and not evaluated. For example, the study of Randazzo et al. [48] is one of the studies frequently cited in the reviews addressing the impact of sleepiness on cognition. Randazzo et al. [48] found that experimental sleep restriction induced both sleepiness and poorer cognitive performance, however the association between sleepiness and cognitive impairment was not assessed. This is an important methodological question that, in turn, suggests caution when affirming a relationship between sleepiness and cognitive/academic skills. Indeed, despite insufficient/disturbed sleep that induce both sleepiness and cognitive sequelae, it is not possible to take for granted that decremented cognitive performances are due to sleepiness. Both sleep loss and sleep disorders may affect cognition independently from sleepiness [2,39,42,51,55]. For example, Jiang et al. [51] investigated the effect of sleep restriction on both sleepiness and working memory and found that sleep loss affects both subjective sleepiness and working memory. At the same time, however, the authors failed to find a correlation between sleepiness and the working memory performance [51]. Moreover, children with SDB may exhibit impairment in executive functions, independently from their sleepiness levels [42]. In other words, their deficits may extend beyond those associated with sleepiness. Indeed, the causal mechanism of these cognitive effects remains open to debate.
Thus, it seems reasonable that, in order to support the idea that there is a relationship between sleepiness and cognition, studies assessing directly and specifically the association between sleepiness and cognitive skills are needed [44]. There are relatively few studies and data which seem to support the relationship between sleepiness and cognition and between sleepiness and scholastic achievements in different samples, i.e., non-selected population of children, healthy children, or children with sleep disorders [56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63]. However there are also some studies which failed to find any association [39,42,51]. Conflicting results regarding which of the cognitive skills is the most affected by sleepiness were also reported. Buckhalt et al [57,60,61,64] found that subjective sleepiness correlated with several cognitive functions (including intellectual ability, verbal comprehension, and working memory) and with mathematics, language, and reading achievements. Calhoun et al. [65] showed a correlation between sleepiness not only with some cognitive skills (processing speed and working memory), but also with school learning problems (e.g., failure to complete school work or the school year), as reported by parents. Notably, other data indicate that there is no association between sleepiness and working memory [51] or intelligence [65]. Similarly, inconsistent findings also regard the relationship between sleepiness and executive functions. Anderson et al. [59] found that adolescents who reported high levels of sleepiness had poor executive functioning. Moreover, Cerasuolo et al. [63] found a significant correlation between sleepiness and executive functions in children (assessed via a Go/NO GO task). Other studies, however, failed to find a correlation between sleepiness and other tests of executive function (i.e., Stroop Test and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; Calhoun et al., 2012; Esposito et al., 2013). In summary, even if a link between sleepiness and cognitive functions was documented, data are still relatively few and results controversial. Moreover, previous studies focused mainly on some cognitive abilities (attention, executive functions, and verbal related ability such as verbal working memory and verbal comprehension), while the relationship between sleepiness and other cognitive abilities (e.g., visual and spatial abilities) was studied less thoroughly. Interestingly, taking together the sparse evidence, it seems that the association between sleepiness and verbal related tasks (e.g., phonological working memory and verbal comprehension underlying crystallized intelligence) is consistent across studies [57,65], while less evidence regarding non-verbal domains of cognition (e.g., visuo-motor integration sills) was reported.
Finally, an important note of caution in interpreting previous findings regards the procedure used for the assessment of scholastic outcomes. Almost all studies did not assess school achievements through standardized tests. For example, school learning outcomes were assessed mainly via self-, parent- or teacher- reports [56,58,61,65,66,67] or through scholastic report cards [68]. Only some studies on children suffering from sleep disorders (mainly SDB) used standardized tests and found that sleep disorders, usually associated with sleepiness, affected scholastic learning outcomes (such as spelling, reading, and arithmetic skills) [36,38,41,43]. This, in turn, suggested the hypothesis that sleepiness could affect scholastic learning. However, the primary effects of sleep disorders on scholastic outcomes deserves caution in interpreting the data, since (as stressed above) sleep disorders may adversely impact cognitive functioning independently from sleepiness (e.g., via hypoxia, sleep fragmentation, and related impact on brain restorative mechanisms [42].
The purpose of this study is to evaluate the relationship between sleepiness and several cognitive skills and scholastic learning abilities, assessed with an extensive neuropsychological battery of standardized tests and not through teachers’ and parents’ reports. We evaluated, in a large sample of unselected primary school children, the association between sleepiness and cognitive abilities, in both verbal (e.g., syntactic comprehension skill) and non-verbal (i.e., visuo-spatial ability) domains, as well as academic skills (e.g., reading decoding, reading comprehension, spelling, mathematical, and handwriting skills). Finally, we wanted to explore if sleepiness may be more related to verbal than to non-verbal domains of cognition and which subdomains may be more influenced. We used a very suited statistical technique, partial least squares path modelling (PLS-PM), the non-parametrical extension of structural equation models (SEM), which allows to suggest causal links between sleepiness and the various academic skills, each interdependently from the other. The additional benefit of this statistical modelling is the combination of dependence and prediction through regression coefficients between latent variables, and interdependence, through the different items of the tests synthetized in one or more common dimensions by performing a factor analysis.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

One hundred and ninety-one children (91 female and 100 male), ranging in age from 7.8 to 11.2 years (average age: 9.61 ± 0.90), were recruited from three primary schools in the south of Italy. In particular, 60 3rd grade children (32 F, 28 M, average age: 8.56 ± 0.32), 57 4th grade children (29 F, 28 M, average age: 9.50 ± 0.32), and 74 5th grade children (30 F, 44 M, average age: 10.56 ± 0.31) participated in the study. All children performed within the norm at an intelligence test [69].
With the guide of an expert psychologist, all children completed the questionnaire assessing sleepiness and then were evaluated through several neuropsychological tests (see below). Participants were tested individually in the morning in a quiet room during school time. The tests were administered through a pseudo-random sequence across participants.
The parents were informed of the research activities and authorized their child’s participation by signing the appropriate informed consent. The study was conducted according to the principles of the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the school authorities when it started. It was also approved by the Ethic committee of Psychological Research of the Department of Hystory, Society and Human Studies -University of Salento (Prot. 101206 -29th July 2020).

2.2. Sleepiness Measures

We evaluated sleepiness through the Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scales [56]. PDSS is a self-reported likert-type questionnaire that measures daytime sleepiness thought related behaviours (e.g., falling asleep or getting drowsy during class periods or homework; difficulty getting up in the morning) in scholastic populations, with possible scores ranging from 0 to 32. Higher PDSS scores indicate greater daytime sleepiness.
PDSS has a high (0.80) internal consistency (assessed through the split-half) and acceptable factor loadings [56]. Moreover, the PDSS scores correlate with total sleep time and sleep disorders [56,58,70].

2.3. Neuropsychological Assessment

2.3.1. Reading and Comprehension Skills

The participants’ reading level was assessed through a standard reading achievement test widely used for Italian children [71]. The MT consists of a series of meaningful texts (short stories taken from children books) of increasing difficulty from Grade level 1 to 5: Syntactic and semantic complexity increases progressively; the letter size decreases (from a medium width of approximately 3.8 to 1.6 mm), and the number of words increases (from 66 to 281 words) progressing from Grade 1 to Grade 5. Children were asked to read a single text depending on their grade and school-year period. Each story was printed in black on a white cardboard. Participants were asked to read the text aloud within a 4-min time limit. Reading speed (number of syllables read/sec) and accuracy (number of errors, adjusted for the amount of text read) were considered. A second passage evaluated reading comprehension: Children read it without a time limit and responded to 10-multiple-choice questions. Children could check the text while answering so that memory load did not affect performance. Correct answers were collected.

2.3.2. Spelling Skills

The participants’ spelling abilities were tested with a standard dictation test [72]. We considered two sections, both requiring the application of transcribing processes based on one-sound-to-one-letter correspondence:
Section A: Regular words with complete one-sound-to-one-letter correspondence (N = 70). Words were selected with different sources of phonetic-phonological complexity: (i) Words made up of continuant sounds only (fricative, liquid, or nasal consonants) vs. words also containing non-continuant (plosive) consonants; (ii) words made up only of consonant-vowel (CV) syllables vs. words also containing consonant clusters and doubled consonants; (iii) disyllabic vs. polysyllabic words. The presence of different sources of phonetic-phonological complexity influences both segmentation and identification of phonemic string to be converted (for instance, continuant phones are, by nature, easier to segment, and hence to identify, than non-continuant phones).
Section B (that in the test corresponds to Section D): Pseudowords—with one-sound-to-one-letter correspondence (N = 25). Items were controlled for different sources of phonetic-phonological complexity, as were words in Section A: (i) Continuance of sounds (non-words with continuant vs. non-continuant consonants); syllabic structure (non-words with consonant-vowel (CV) syllables vs. non-words also containing doubled consonants; and length (disyllabic vs. 3–4 syllable non-words). Similarly to Section A, phonetic/phonological variables are introduced in order to account for variables influencing acoustic-to-phonological analysis that is preliminary to an effective phonological-to-orthographic conversion procedure.
Words and pseudowords were given in separate blocks and in a single quasi-randomized order. The examiner read each item aloud in a neutral tone. The children were asked to repeat each item, before writing, by spelling it down (so that the examiner could ensure that they had perceived the item). They were permitted to write in either capital or lower case letters. No feedback was provided on the accuracy of the written response. The number of stimuli spelled correctly in each section was computed. Self-corrections were accepted.

2.3.3. Mathematical Skills

The AC-MT battery [73] was used to assess the students’ mathematical skills. AC-MT is a battery composed by several subtasks that are derived from the neuropsychological model of number processing and calculation [73,74,75]. The subtests used in the present work are described below:
  • Computation. This subtest assesses the child’s ability to complete written computational operations (additions, subtractions, multiplications, and divisions).
  • Number ordering. This task requires understanding semantics of number and thus evaluate number sense. A series of four numbers are presented, and the child must be able to place them in the correct order (from greatest to smaller one, and vice versa).
  • Number spelling. This task assesses students’ ability to lexical retrieval as well as to elaborate the syntactic structure of a number. Students listen to six numbers and then are asked to spell them.
  • Arithmetical Facts subtest. This task is used to investigate if children have stored arithmetical facts and were able to retrieve automatically the results of basic and simple operations from the memory. Children are asked to recall several arithmetic facts, each one within 5 s of time.
In all subtests, correct answers were collected.

2.3.4. Syntactic Comprehension Skill

Syntactic comprehension test [76] assesses oral comprehension and in particular the ability to identify the stimulus listened between syntactic distractors. Participants had to choose one picture among four alternatives that represented the sentence read by the examiner. Correct answers were collected.

2.3.5. Handwriting Speed

The Concise Assessment Scale for Children’s Handwriting [77] is a standardized norm-referenced test to assess handwriting skills. Children were required to copy in their usual style a standard text on an unlined A4 sheet in a maximum 5 min (for slow writers, children had to spell at least the first five sentences) Copying speed (BHK-speed) was determined by counting the number of letters spelled in 5 min.

2.3.6. Visuospatial Working Memory

Visuospatial working memory (VSWM) was assessed through the Visual Pattern test, Active Version (VPTA) of the Visuospatial Working Memory Test Battery [78]. This test assesses the active simultaneous component of visuo-spatial working memory [79,80]. Note that active processing requires the ability to integrate and modify previous stored visuo-spatial information. The task requires memorizing a pattern of positions in a matrix; after that the children are required to reproduce it on an empty matrix, but shifting the original pattern one line below. The total accuracy was scored.

2.3.7. Sustained and Selective Attention

The bell test [81] evaluates sustained and selective attention. The test consists of four different sheets, each of which contain 35 bells among many other stimuli (animate and inanimate objects, such as houses, trees, fish, horses, etc.) of the same size and orientation in space. The task consists of finding and ticking only the bells within a time limit (2 min per sheet). We calculated the total number of bells identified in the first 30 s on each sheet of paper (selective attention score).

2.3.8. Visuo-Spatial Constructive Skills

The Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test [82] is a neuropsychological test extensively used in neuropsychological research and clinical practice. Through the use of different scoring systems, the ROCF task allows evaluating several cognitive abilities, including visual memory, visuo- spatial constructional ability, as well as executive functions such as planning strategies [83,84,85]. Children were asked to copy a complex geometrical figure as accurately as possible. The score was computed according to Ferrara-Mori [86].

3. Data Analyses

Partial Least Squares Path Modelling (PLS-PM)

For each test, raw scores were converted to z scores according to proper standard reference data. Preliminarily, we looked for differences in the rate of subjective sleepiness as a function of gender and school age. Non-parametrical comparisons of mean, or Welch’s Test and ANOVA (due to the different standard deviations of the groups), were performed with gender (two levels: Male vs. female) and school age (three levels: 3rd, 4th, 5th graders) as independent variables and values of sleepiness as dependent ones.
In order to test the relationship between sleepiness and the various academic skills, we performed a partial least squares path modelling [87], a multivariate variance based on the structural equation modelling technique whose application can be found in business, management, and in social sciences, as well as recently in psychology [88,89,90]. Structural equation modelling (SEM) involves the analysis of multiple variables taken into account simultaneously and represents a second-generation technique, due to the sophisticated methods used. This technique allows modelling latent variables based on behavioural research, composite constructs, and different measurement scales. This technique allows modelling latent variables based on behavioural research, composite constructs, and different measurement scales. The aim of SEM is, therefore, to investigate the relations between directly non-observable variables (i.e., latent variables), which are, in turn, measured by indicators (i.e., manifest variables), provided by scales. We used PLS-SEM, which is a primarily explorative technique, because it does not imply assumptions on distributional data, nor on sample size. Furthermore, PLS-SEM is a robust method when there are missing values (at a reasonable level) within data. The model in PLS-SEM can be defined by using constructs formed by single and multi-item measures. Finally, PLS-SEM can incorporate formative and reflective models [91] minimizing the amount of unexplained variance and is more efficient, since the convergence takes place after few iterations [92].
The aim of this research was to inquire the existing relations between sleepiness and other neuropsychological abilities [93], as mathematical skills, attention, spelling, oral and reading comprehension, reading, visuo-constructive skills, visuo-spatial working memory, and handwriting. In order to achieve this objective, we performed a PLS-PM model with ten latent variables measured through 18 different indicators, as reported in Table 1. We used the reflective approach to define the blocks of indicators (Figure 1).
In the reflective approach (right side of Figure 1), manifest variables are supposed to be caused by the latent variables. In the formative way (left side of Figure 1), instead, the latent construct is constituted by its indicators. The difference between the two approaches is based on causal-effect relationships between the indicators and the constructs. In a reflective block, a change in the latent variable corresponds to a change in its indicators.
The goodness of measures reliability was assessed through three different methods, Cronbach’s alpha, Dillon-Goldstein’s rho, and the eigenvalues criterion. Cronbach’s alpha and Dillon-Goldstein’s rho values must be bigger than 0.70 in order to be considered good indices of reliability. In terms of eigenvalues, the first one must be bigger than the value one while the second smaller than one. Latent variables measured by only one indicator have a subsequent value of 1.
With regards to the outer model, guidelines [87,92] affirmed that indicators must have a loading of at least 0.708 to be considered good reflective blocks of latent variables. This precondition is fundamental to explain an account of variance at least at 50%, or communality, which is the variation in an item (i.e., a test) explained by the construct (variance extracted from the item).
We determined the average variance extracted (AVE): This is an index which allows to evaluate the amount of variance of the indicators explained by the latent variables. A good AVE index is more than 0.50.

4. Results

The explorative analysis on differences in sleepiness due to gender distribution showed no significant effect (more specifically MEAN FEMALE = 13.8 (SD = 6.55) and MEAN MALE = 13.9 (DS = 5.49), ∆ = 0.0839; p-value = 0.933). Similarly, no significant differences emerged as a function of school age (MEAN THIRD GRADE = 15.5 (SD = 6.13); MEAN FOURTH GRADE = 13.6 (SD = 5.58), MEAN FIFTH GRADE = 12.7 (SD = 5.94), p-value = 0.062). Overall, we can assume that, in our sample of primary school children, gender and age did not modulate levels of subjective sleepiness.
Partial least squares path modelling (PLS-PM) was performed through the plspm package [87] by using the R Studio software (version 1.2.5033). The hypotheses were tested by performing a PLS-PM model, as shown in Figure 2.
Table 2 reported results of goodness of measures reliability. All latent variables present good Cronbach’s alpha and Dillon-Goldstein’s rho indexes. Only the latent construct mathematical skills had a slightly lower measure of 0.70 (0.69). Nevertheless, other indices were acceptable, therefore we considered all the non-observable constructs as appropriately reliable.
With regards to the outer model, we found only two indicators which did not reach a loading of 0.70, computation and number spelling on mathematical skills (respectively 0.69 and 0.68) but their measures were very near to that threshold, so we included them in our model without losing quality (Table 3).
Table 4 presented cross-loadings values. These measures are important to identify indicators with the higher correlation with the latent variable, which should be the one to which they are associated. The cross-loadings approach assesses the discriminant validity. In our study, the biggest correlations of the indicators corresponded to their non-observable constructs. The final model is presented in Figure 3.
Regarding the AVE index, we found that AVE SLEEPINESS = 1, AVE MATHEMATICAL SKILLS = 0.515, AVE ATTENTION = 0.672, AVE SPELLING = 0.845, AVE ORAL COMPREHENSION = 1, AVE READING = 0.804, AVE VISUOCONSTRUCTIVE SKILLS = 1, AVE VISUO SPATIALWORKING MEMORY = 1, AVE HANDWRITING = 1, and AVE READING COMPREHENSION = 1. According to these AVE outcomes, each latent variable explains more than 50% of the variance, therefore the remaining error is less than the information explained.
Based on our results, sleepiness had negative coefficients with all latent variables used. In particular, the statistical relations were negative and significant (α = 0.05) in four causal effects, attention (β2 = −0.2177, p-value = 0.007), mathematical skills (β1 = −0.1759, p-value = 0.03), spelling (β3 = −0.1828, p-value = 0.024), and oral comprehension (β4 = −0.251, p-value = 0.002). In the other cases, sleepiness did not affect in a significant way reading-decoding (β5 = −0.113, p-value = 0.154), visuo-constructive skills (β6 = −0.024, p-value = 0.786), visuo-spatial working memory (β7 = −0.063, p-value = 0.439), and handwriting (β8 = −0.117, p-value = 0.149). The relation between sleepiness and reading comprehension is negative (β9 = −0.138) and not significant but p-value was very close to the threshold of 0.05 (p-value = 0.09), so this result must be considered as a borderline significant trend. In order to assess the validity of the model, we performed different resampling through the bootstrap method in path coefficients (n = 1000). Results were confirmed in terms of polarity and significance (Table 5). Furthermore, the bootstrap validation showed that all the indicators had significant loadings according to the lower and upper confidence intervals (Table 6): For this reason, we considered all of them a good proxy of latent variables.

5. Discussion

The hypothesis that sleepiness affects cognition and school performance arises mainly from studies investigating the impact of sleep deprivation/restriction or sleep disorders on cognitive and academic skills. However, despite the fact that sleep disorders and sleep loss induce both sleepiness and cognitive deficits, it is not possible to directly assume that decremented cognitive performances are due to sleepiness. Indeed, sleep loss and sleep disorder may affect brain maturation, cognitive functions, and related brain mechanisms through many different processes (e.g., disruption of sleep related restorative brain processes, oxidative stress, and hypoxia in prefrontal cortex and other brain regions) [2,42,94,95]. Thus, it is possible to stress that, in order to support the idea that there is a relationship between sleepiness and cognition, there is a need of studies that assess directly and specifically the association between sleepiness and cognitive skills [44]. In the present paper, we examined this issue considering many standardized measures of cognitive and academic abilities, also including non-verbal domains, and used also a statistical technique allowing us to explore causal relationships.
The results showed that sleepiness seems to affect several, but not all, cognitive and literacy skills. First of all, we found that sleepiness is related to attention processes. Daytime sleepiness has a detrimental effect on attention and limits the ability to focus and attend to salient information. This result may be considered in accordance with data from other studies showing that sleepiness is associated with processing speed [65] and selective attention in the GO/NO-GO task [63].
Moreover, it seems that sleepiness (perhaps because of reduced attention capacity) may slow learning and may decrease the efficiency of some cognitive and academic skills. With regards to cognitive skills, we found that sleepiness is related only to verbal abilities. Specifically, sleepiness had a negative impact on syntactic comprehension but did not influence visuo-spatial working memory and visuo-constructive skills. Several studies showed that sleepiness is related to verbal ability and verbal comprehension [57], however, the focus on non-verbal visuo-spatial and visuo-constructive abilities is relatively new [65]. Present data show an unequal vulnerability of verbal vs. non-verbal abilities by sleepiness.
We also found that sleepiness had significant and detrimental effects on some, but not all, literacy skills. In particular, it negatively influenced the orthographic competency, the mathematical abilities, and tended to significantly affect also reading comprehension. At the same time, results showed that sleepiness did not interfere with the efficiency of reading, decoding, and handwriting fluency.
Overall results deserve several comments. Firstly, data clearly show that sleepiness is not related to all cognitive and academic performances. As stated by Beebe and Gonzal [94], though it is tempting to attribute all the daytime difficulties to excessive sleepiness, doing so requires expansion of the already multidimensional construct of sleepiness to include conceptually distinct cognitive function.
Interestingly, we found more profound effects on verbal than non-verbal abilities and on functions requiring executive control with respect to automatized ones. Specifically, sleepiness had a negative impact on oral and written comprehension and orthographic competency but did not influence visuo-spatial working memory, visuo-constructive skills, and handwriting speed. Although the dissociation between verbal and visuo-spatial domains merits some caution, since we did not extensively test all verbal competencies of our children (e.g., verbal working memory and measures of vocabulary, verbal memory are absent), it clearly emerges that sleepiness is unrelated to visual-spatial, constructional, and motor domains of cognition.
With respect to the impact of differences of tasks in terms of automaticity and resource demanding, several studies indicating that sleep loss affects only “higher” cognitive functions related to prefrontal cortex (e.g., creativity, verbal fluency, divergent thinking ability, executive functions, problem solving, and learning new abstract concepts), but it does not influence other automatic and less-complex cognitive processes [4,34,48,96,97,98,99]. For instance, a recent meta-analysis stressed that sleep duration is linked mainly with deficits in higher-order and complex executive functions [4]. Indeed, we found a relation between sleepiness and verbal comprehension (i.e., written and oral/syntactic comprehension), spelling, and such cognitive skills involving complex frontal lobe related verbal, controlled, and meta-cognitive skills, while it did not hamper efficient word decoding and the fast motor activity implied in handwriting.
With respect to reading, after sleep loss and increased sleepiness, the ability such as word decoding may be carried out efficiently through automatic decoding processes [100]. Italian children acquire the decoding process very early and read with a very small rate of errors yet after one year of schooling [101,102,103,104]. In fact, with increasing experience and practice, children progressively pass from the application of an effortful and serial algorithm to a less demanding process based on the fast and automatic retrieval of a memory trace [105,106,107]. With practice, children also automatize the process of grapheme-to-phoneme mapping in a given orthography. This finding could explain why sleepiness is not related to the decoding skills in our sample. Sleep loss may leave automatic cognitive processes (such as reading decoding) unchanged but, at the same time, it continues to affect complex cognitive functions, as highlighted by the partial influence of sleepiness on the reading comprehension skill, that is not an automatic process involving prefrontal controlled skills and meta-cognitive processes and monitoring [108,109,110,111]. Notably, it is important to consider that brain imaging and cognitive studies showed that reading decoding and reading comprehension are dissociable functions [108,111,112,113]. Consistent with our results, Kuroishi et al. [43] found that children with mouth breathing syndrome had poorer performance than controls on the reading comprehension task. However, Ellis et al. [114] found contrasting results: Poor sleepers performed better on the reading comprehension test than medium quality sleepers. To explain their result, Ellis et al. [114] suggested that a compensatory effort can temporarily permit coping with the effect of sleep loss and sleepiness. More generally, Ellis et al. suggested that sleep loss and sleepiness may induce an increase of compensative brain and psychological mechanisms that cope with deficits and, in turn, improve some cognitive skills, thus the performance of sleepy children may be normal or higher [114]. If this was the case, we should have found no relationship between sleepiness and any other skills, while we found that sleepiness affected several cognitive abilities. Of course, we realize that there is a need of more studies that assess the relationship between sleepiness and reading comprehension.
With respect to writing, while spelling is a very demanding and sensitive task, handwriting is considered a fast motor activity. Efficient spelling requires a fine online phonetic-phonological analysis of the acoustic string and correct phoneme-to-grapheme sequential mapping and/or lexical access. As described in the method section, the spelling task used maximized the difficulties of both the mentioned processes, since stimuli were varied for different sources of sublexical spelling difficulties. Coherently, in the literature there are reports of a different sensitivity of spelling vs. reading processes in detecting deficits of automation. For example, adults with compensated dyslexia, as well as adult relatives of dyslexic children, may show spelling errors as a result of their residual learning difficulties [115,116]. Therefore, it seems that spelling, but not reading decoding, may reveal minor learning problems that might otherwise go undetected. Handwriting, instead, became very early fully automated. Handwriting is a rhythmic activity that can reach a frequency of about 5 Hz when accounting for the successive ascending and descending strokes produced by the pen. The high frequency limits the possibility of online control based on sensory information and imposes a mode of control, based on the execution of motor programs. Automation refers to the fact that writing is produced with minimal conscious “effort”, i.e., with minimal attentional and frontal involvement [117,118]. Automation is crucial in handwriting as it allows the allocation of cognitive resources to other processes [119,120].
With regards to the relationships between sleepiness and mathematical skills, our results suggest that sleepiness is linked with all the domains of mathematical competence. One would wonder why also the verbal recall of arithmetical facts, an ability that seems to be more automated, was found to be implied. Accordingly with the neuroscientific and neuropsychological model developed by Dehaene and Cohen [75], the ability of recalling arithmetical facts involves the domain of verbal ability (i.e., not specific for number) implemented within classical frontal and perisylvian language areas of the left hemisphere [75]. In our study, all mathematical domains are involved, confirming the major susceptibility of the verbal component of some mathematical tasks (i.e., arithmetical facts) and probably also the higher reliance on prefrontal related executive attention control of the others [121].
Our study has several limitations. Some studies point out that the relationship between sleepiness and cognitive and scholastic skills is influenced by socioeconomic variables [57]. Moreover, sleepiness may be measured via several methods, including objective methods (such as the multiple sleep latency test, psychomotor vigilance performance, and measures of spontaneous oscillations in pupil diameter). Notably, some studies have stressed that subjective and objective measures of sleepiness are not strictly associated and thus may represent distinct entities that should not be assumed to be equivalent [122]. Thus, other studies are needed to corroborate our data. Nevertheless, the present study clearly showed a relationship between subjective experience of sleepiness and objective neuropsychological measures. In conclusion, we extended the finding about the association between sleepiness and both cognitive and scholastic outcomes. It is highlighted that the presence of subjective sleepiness can be considered a risk factor for cognitive and scholastic difficulties in children, at least in abilities not automatized and in the verbal domain. The present results have clinical and educational implications. In fact, clinicians and educators may consider that sleepy children may not respond optimally to tests or scholastic verifications: It would be useful, if possible, to test children at a different time of day, or different days, when the child is not sleepy and consider sleepiness in formulating conclusions about core reasons for a child′s poor performance. Moreover, comorbidity of sleep problems should be evaluated. Many children with concurrent diagnoses should be reevaluated taking into account the role of sleep in aggravating their performance.
Furthermore, it is important to remember that medical and/or behavioural treatment on sleep disorders (that are related to sleepiness) and, in particular, on sleep habits and hygiene may be in some cases effective and recommended in order to reduce the cognitive difficulties due to sleepiness [5,123,124,125]. Moreover, teachers and health professionals should sensitize and inform the population in order to promote an adequate sleep in children to reduce the impact of sleepiness on cognitive and academic achievement.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: L.M., C.V.M. and P.A.; Data Curation: C.V.M.; Formal Analysis: F.S. and E.C.; Writing: L.M., C.V.M., F.S., E.C. and P.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by University of Salento, through the grant “5 x mille” to Chiara Valeria Marinelli.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Curcio, G.; Ferrara, M.; De Gennaro, L. Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance. Sleep Med. Rev. 2006, 10, 323–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Sadeh, A. Consequences of Sleep Loss or Sleep Disruption in Children. Sleep Med. Clin. 2007, 2, 513–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Beebe, D.W. Cognitive, behavioral, and functional consequences of inadequate sleep in children and adolescents. Pediatr. Clin. 2011, 58, 649–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Astill, R.G.; Van der Heijden, K.B.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Van Someren, E.J. Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: A century of research meta-analyzed. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Hershner, S.D.; Chervin, R.D. Causes and consequences of sleepiness among college students. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2014, 6, 73–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. De Bruin, E.J.; Van Run, C.; Staaks, J.; Meijer, A.M. Effects of sleep manipulation on cognitive functioning of adolescents: A systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2017, 32, 45–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Tham, E.; Schneider, N.; Broekman, B.F.P. Infant sleep and its relation with cognition and growth: A narrative review. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2017, 9, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Reynaud, E.; Vecchierini, M.-F.; Heude, B.; Charles, M.A.; Plancoulaine, S. Sleep and its relation to cognition and behaviour in preschool-aged children of the general population: A systematic review. J. Sleep Res. 2018, 27, e12636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Morse, A.M.; Kothare, S.V. Evaluation and Management of a Sleepy Child. In Allergy and Sleep; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2019; pp. 87–104. [Google Scholar]
  10. Blunden, S.; Hoban, T.F.; Chervin, R.D. Sleepiness in children. Sleep Med. Clin. 2006, 1, 105–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Owens, J.A.; Babcock, D.; Weiss, M. Evaluation and Treatment of Children and Adolescents with Excessive Daytime Sleepiness. Clin. Pediatr. 2020, 59, 340–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Moore, M.; Meltzer, L.J. The sleepy adolescent: Causes and consequences of sleepiness in teens. Paediatr. Respir. Rev. 2008, 9, 114–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Fallone, G.; Owens, J.A.; Deane, J. Sleepiness in children and adolescents: Clinical implications. Sleep Med. Rev. 2002, 6, 287–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Crowley, S.J.; Wolfson, A.R.; Tarokh, L.; Carskadon, M.A. An update on adolescent sleep: New evidence informing the perfect storm model. J. Adolesc. 2018, 67, 55–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Carskadon, M.A.; Wolfson, A.R.; Acebo, C.; Tzischinsky, O.; Seifer, R. Adolescent sleep patterns, circadian timing, and sleepiness at a transition to early school days. Sleep 1998, 21, 871–881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Jenni, O.G.; LeBourgeois, M.K. Understanding sleep-wake behavior and sleep disorders in children: The value of a model. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2006, 19, 282–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Carskadon, M.A. Sleep in Adolescents: The Perfect Storm. Pediatr. Clin. N. Am. 2011, 58, 637–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Minges, K.E.; Redeker, N.S. Delayed school start times and adolescent sleep: A systematic review of the experimental evidence. Sleep Med. Rev. 2016, 28, 86–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Marx, R.; Tanner-Smith, E.; Davison, C.M.; Ufholz, L.-A.; Freeman, J.; Shankar, R.; Newton, L.; Brown, R.S.; Parpia, A.S.; Cozma, I.; et al. Later school start times for supporting the education, health, and well-being of high school students. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2017, 7, 1–57. [Google Scholar]
  20. Gibson, E.S.; Powles, A.C.P.; Thabane, L.; O’Brien, S.; Molnar, D.S.; Trajanovic, N.; Ogilvie, R.; Shapiro, C.M.; Yan, M.; Chilcott-Tanser, L. “Sleepiness” is serious in adolescence: Two surveys of 3235 Canadian students. BMC Public Health 2006, 6, 116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Roehrs, T.; Roth, T. Caffeine: Sleep and daytime sleepiness. Sleep Med. Rev. 2008, 12, 153–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Calamaro, C.J.; Mason, T.B.; Ratcliffe, S.J. Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: Effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics 2009, 123, 1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Higuchi, S.; Motohashi, Y.; Liu, Y.; Maeda, A. Effects of playing a computer game using a bright display on presleep physiological variables, sleep latency, slow wave sleep and REM sleep. J. Sleep Res. 2005, 14, 267–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Joo, S.; Shin, C.; Kim, J.; Yi, H.; Ahn, Y.; Park, M.; Kim, J.; Lee, S. Prevalence and correlates of excessive daytime sleepiness in high school students in Korea. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2005, 59, 433–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Foley, L.S.; Maddison, R.; Jiang, Y.; Marsh, S.; Olds, T.S.; Ridley, K. Presleep Activities and Time of Sleep Onset in Children. Pediatrics 2013, 131, 276–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Liu, Y.; Zhang, J.; Li, S.X.; Chan, N.Y.; Yu, M.W.M.; Lam, S.P.; Chan, J.; Li, A.M.; Wing, Y.-K. Excessive daytime sleepiness among children and adolescents: Prevalence, correlates, and pubertal effects. Sleep Med. 2019, 53, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Beyens, I.; Nathanson, A.I. Electronic media use and sleep among preschoolers: Evidence for time-shifted and less consolidated sleep. Health Commun. 2019, 34, 537–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Crowley, S.J.; Acebo, C.; Carskadon, M.A. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and delayed phase in adolescence. Sleep Med. 2007, 8, 602–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Altena, E.; Baglioni, C.; Espie, C.A.; Ellis, J.; Gavriloff, D.; Holzinger, B.; Schlarb, A.; Frase, L.; Jernelöv, S.; Riemann, D. Dealing with sleep problems during home confinement due to the COVID-19 outbreak: Practical recommendations from a task force of the European CBT-I Academy. J. Sleep Res. 2020, 29, e13052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Buheji, M.; Hassani, A.; Ebrahim, A.; da Costa Cunha, K.; Jahrami, H.; Baloshi, M.; Hubail, S. Children and Coping During COVID-19: A Scoping Review of Bio-Psycho-Social Factors. Int. J. Appl. 2020, 10, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Xiang, M.; Zhang, Z.; Kuwahara, K. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on children and adolescents’ lifestyle behavior larger than expected. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2020, 63, 531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. King, D.L.; Delfabbro, P.H.; Billieux, J.; Potenza, M.N. Problematic online gaming and the COVID-19 pandemic. J.Behav. Addict. 2020, 9, 184–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Lepido, D.; Rolander, N. Housebound Italian Kids Strain Network with Fortnite Marathon. 2020. Available online: https://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2020-03-12/housebound-italian-kids-strain-networkwith-fortnite-marathon (accessed on 12 March 2020).
  34. Sadeh, A.; Gruber, R.; Raviv, A. Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 405–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Steenari, M.R.; Vuontela, V.; Paavonen, E.J.; Carlson, S.; Fjallberg, M.; Aronen, E.T. Working memory and sleep in 6- to 13-year-old schoolchildren. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2003, 42, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Gottlieb, D.J.; Chase, C.; Vezina, R.M.; Heeren, T.; Corwin, M.J.; Auerbach, S.; Weese-Mayer, D.E.; Lesko, S.M. Sleep-disordered breathing symptoms are associated with poorer cognitive function in 5-year-old children. J. Pediatr. 2004, 145, 458–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. O’Brien, L.M.; Mervis, C.B.; Holbrook, C.R.; Bruner, J.L.; Smith, N.H.; McNally, N.; McClimment, M.C.; Gozal, D. Neurobehavioral correlates of sleep-disordered breathing in children. J. Sleep Res. 2004, 13, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gradisar, M.; Terrill, G.; Johnston, A.; Douglas, P. Adolescent sleep and working memory performance. Sleep Biol. Rhythm. 2008, 6, 146–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Goodlin-Jones, B.; Tang, K.; Liu, J.; Anders, T.F. Sleep problems, sleepiness and daytime behavior in preschool-age children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2009, 50, 1532–1540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Kopasz, M.; Loessl, B.; Hornyak, M.; Riemann, D.; Nissen, C.; Piosczyk, H.; Voderholzer, U. Sleep and memory in healthy children and adolescents-a critical review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2010, 14, 167–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Bourke, R.; Anderson, V.; Yang, J.S.; Jackman, A.R.; Killedar, A.; Nixon, G.M.; Davey, M.J.; Walker, A.M.; Trinder, J.; Horne, R.S.C. Cognitive and academic functions are impaired in children with all severities of sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep Med. 2011, 12, 489–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Esposito, M.; Antinolfi, L.; Gallai, B.; Parisi, L.; Roccella, M.; Marotta, R.; Lavano, S.M.; Mazzotta, G.; Precenzano, F.; Carotenuto, M. Executive dysfunction in children a_ected by obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: An observational study. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2013, 9, 1087–1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Kuroishi, R.C.S.; Garcia, R.B.; Valera, F.C.P.; Anselmo-Lima, W.T.; Fukuda, M.T.H. Deficits in working memory, reading comprehension and arithmetic skills in children with mouth breathing syndrome: Analytical cross-sectional study. São Paulo Med. J. 2015, 133, 78–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Liu, J.; Liu, X.; Ji, X.; Wang, Y.; Zhou, G.; Chen, X. Sleep disordered breathing symptoms and daytime sleepiness are associated with emotional problems and poor school performance in children. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 242, 218–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Hunter, S.J.; Gozal, D.; Smith, D.L.; Philby, M.F.; Kaylegian, J.; Kheirandish-Gozal, L. Effect of Sleep-disordered Breathing Severity on Cognitive Performance Measures in a Large Community Cohort of Young School-aged Children. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2016, 194, 739–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Ruberto, M.; Precenzano, F.; Parisi, L.; Salerno, M.; Maltese, A.; Messina, G.; Roccella, M. Visuomotor integration skills in children a_ected by obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: A case-control study. Acta Med. Mediterr. 2016, 32, 1659–1663. [Google Scholar]
  47. Randazzo, A.C.; Schweitzer, P.K.; Walsh, J.K. Cognitive function following 3 nights of sleep restriction in home monitoring. Sleep 1998, 21, s249. [Google Scholar]
  48. Randazzo, A.C.; Muehlbach, M.J.; Schweitzer, P.K.; Waish, J.K. Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10–14. Sleep 1998, 21, 861–868. [Google Scholar]
  49. Sadeh, A.; Gruber, R.; Raviv, A. The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: What a difference an hour makes. Child Dev. 2003, 74, 444–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Beebe, D.W.; Fallone, G.; Godiwala, N.; Flanigan, M.; Martin, D.; Schaffner, L.; Amin, R. Feasibility and behavioral effects of an at-home multi-night sleep restriction protocol for adolescents. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 2008, 49, 915–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Jiang, F.; VanDyke, R.D.; Zhang, J.; Li, F.; Gozal, D.; Shen, X. Effect of chronic sleep restriction on sleepiness and working memory in adolescents and young adults. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2011, 33, 892–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lo, J.C.; Ong, J.L.; Leong, R.L.; Gooley, J.J.; Chee, M.W. Cognitive Performance, Sleepiness, and Mood in Partially Sleep Deprived Adolescents: The Need for Sleep Study. Sleep 2016, 39, 687–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Fallone, G.; Acebo, C.; Arnedt, J.T.; Seifer, R.; Carskadon, M.A. Effects of acute sleep restriction on behavior, sustained attention, and response inhibition in children. Percept. Mot. Skills 2001, 93, 213–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Fallone, G.; Acebo, C.; Seifer, R.; Carskadon, M.A. Experimental restriction of sleep opportunity in children: Effect on teacher rating. Sleep 2005, 28, 1561–1567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Chuah, L.Y.M.; Chee, M.W.L. Cholinergic Augmentation Modulates Visual Task Performance in Sleep-Deprived Young Adults. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 11369–11377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Drake, C.L.; Nickel, C.; Burduvali, E.; Roth, T.; Jefferson, C.; Badia, P. The Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scale (PDSS): Sleep Habits and School Outcomes in Middle-school Children. Sleep 2003, 26, 455–458. [Google Scholar]
  57. Buckhalt, J.A.; Keller, P.; El-Sheikh, M. Children’s Sleep and Cognitive Functioning: Race and Socioeconomic Status as Moderators of Effects. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 213–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Perez-Chada, D.; Perez-Lloret, S.; Videla, A.J.; Cardinali, D.; Bergna, M.A.; Fernández-Acquier, M.; Larrateguy, L.; Zabert, G.E.; Drake, C.L. Sleep Disordered Breathing and Daytime Sleepiness Are Associated with Poor Academic Performance In Teenagers. A Study Using the Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scale (PDSS). Sleep 2007, 30, 1698–1703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Anderson, B.; Storfer-Isser, A.; Taylor, H.G.; Rosen, C.L.; Redline, S. Associations of executive function with sleepiness and sleep duration in adolescents. Pediatrics 2009, 123, e701–e707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Buckhalt, J.A.; Keller, P.S.; Kelly, R.J.; El-Sheikh, M. Concurrent and Longitudinal Relations between Children’s Sleep and Cognitive Functioning: The Moderating Role of Parent Education. Child Dev. 2009, 80, 875–892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Dewald, J.F.; Meijer, A.M.; Oort, F.J.; Kerkhof, G.A.; Bögels, S.M. The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2010, 14, 179–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Buckhalt, J.A. Children’s Sleep, Sleepiness, and Performance on Cognitive Tasks. WMF Press Bull. 2011, 2, 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  63. Cerasuolo, M.; Giganti, F.; Conte, F.; Costanzo, L.M.; Della Monica, C.; Arzilli, C.; Marchesano, R.; Perrella, A.; Ficca, G. Schooltime subjective sleepiness and performance in Italian primary school children. Chronol. Int. 2016, 33, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Bub, K.L.; Buckhalt, J.A.; El-Sheikh, M. Children’s sleep and cognitive performance: A cross-domain analysis of change over time. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 1504–1514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Calhoun, S.L.; Fernandez-Mendoza, J.; Vgontzas, A.N.; Mayes, S.D.; Tsaoussoglou, M.; Rodriguez-Muñoz, A.; Bixler, E.O. Learning, Attention/Hyperactivity, and Conduct Problems as Sequelae of Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in a General Population Study of Young Children. Sleep 2012, 35, 627–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Giannotti, F.; Cortesi, F.; Ottaviano, S. Sleep pattern daytime functioning and school performance in adolescence: Preliminary data on an Italian representative sample. Sleep Res. 1997, 26, 196. [Google Scholar]
  67. Bruni, O.; Ferini-Strambi, L.; Russo, P.M.; Antignani, M.; Innocenzi, M.; Ottaviano, P.; Valente, D.; Ottaviano, S. Sleep disturbances and teacher ratings of school achievement and temperament in children. Sleep Med. 2006, 7, 43–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Shin, C.; Kim, J.; Lee, S.; Ahn, Y.; Joo, S. Sleep habits, excessive daytime sleepiness and school performance in high school students. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2003, 57, 451–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Pruneti, C. Aggiornamento della standardizzazione italiana del test delle Matrici Progressive Colorate di Raven. Boll. Psicol. Appl. 1996, 217, 51–57. [Google Scholar]
  70. Lewandowski, A.S.; Toliver-Sokol, M.; Palermo, T.M. Evidence-Based Review of Subjective Pediatric Sleep Measures. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2011, 36, 780–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Cornoldi, C.; Carretti, B. Prove MT-3- Clinica. Florence; Organizzazioni Speciali: Florence, Italy, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  72. Angelelli, P.; Marinelli, C.V.; Iaia, M.; Notarnicola, A.; Costabile, D.; Judica, A.; Zoccolot-ti, P.; Luzzatti, C. DDO 2: Diagnosi dei Disturbi Ortografici in età Evolutiva; Edizioni Centro Studi Erickson: Trento, Italy, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  73. Cornoldi, C.; Lucangeli, D.; Bellina, M. AC-MT 6-11. Test per la Valutazione delle Abilità di Calcolo e Risoluzione dei Problem; Edizioni Centro Studi Erickson: Trento, Italy, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  74. McCloskey, M.; Caramazza, A.; Basili, A. Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: Evidence from dyscalculia. Brain Cogn. 1985, 4, 171–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L. Cerebral pathways for calculation: Double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex 1997, 33, 219–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Bisiacchi, P.S.; Cendron, M.; Gugliotta, M.; Tressoldi, P.; Vio, C. BVN5-11. Batteria di Valutazione Neuropsicologica per l’età Evolutiva; Edizioni Erickson: Trento, Italy, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  77. Hamstra-Bletz, L.; Blöte, A.W. A Longitudinal Study on Dysgraphic Handwriting in Primary School. J. Learn. Disabil. 1993, 26, 689–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Mammarella, I.C.; Toso, C.; Pazzaglia, F.; Cornoldi, C. BVS-Corsi: The Corsi Blocks Task and the BVS Battery for Visuospatial Memory Assessment; Edizioni Centro Studi Erickson: Trento, Italy, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  79. Cornoldi, C.; Rigoni, F.; Venneri, A.; Vecchi, T. Passive and active processes in visuo-spatial memory: Double dissociation in developmental learning disabilities. Brain Cogn. 2000, 43, 17–20. [Google Scholar]
  80. Mammarella, I.C.; Cornoldi, C.; Pazzaglia, F.; Toso, C.; Grimoldi, M.; Vio, C. Evidence for a double dissociation between spatial-simultaneous and spatial-sequential working memory in visuospatial (nonverbal) learning disabled children. Brain Cogn. 2006, 62, 58–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Gauthier, L.; Dehaut, F.; Joanette, Y. The Bells test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. Int. J. Clin. Psychol. 1989, 11, 49–54. [Google Scholar]
  82. Rey, A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encephalopathie traumatique. Arch. Psychol. 1941, 28, 286–340. [Google Scholar]
  83. Akshoomo, N.A.; Stiles, J. Developmental trends in visuospatial analysis and planning: I. copying a complex figure. Neuropsychology 1995, 9, 364–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Shin, M.S.; Park, S.-Y.; Park, S.-R.; Seol, S.-H.; Kwon, J.S. Clinical and empirical applications of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. Nat. Protoc. 2006, 1, 892–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Strauss, E.; Sherman, E.M.; Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  86. Ferrara-Mori, G. Traduzione Italiana del Manuale del Centre de Psychologie Appliquèe; Organizzazioni Speciali: Florence, Italy. Available online: https://opac.sbn.it/opacsbn/opaclib?db=solr_iccu&resultForward=opac/iccu/brief.jsp&from=1&nentries=10&searchForm=opac/iccu/error.jsp&do_cmd=search_show_cmd&item:5032:Nomi::@frase@=IT%5CICCU%5CSBLV%5C000277 (accessed on 7 August 2020).
  87. Sanchez, G. PLS Path Modeling with R; Trowchez Editions: Berkeley, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  88. Ciavolino, E. General distress as second order latent variable estimated through PLS-PM approach. Electron. J. Appl. Stat. Anal. 2012, 5, 458–464. [Google Scholar]
  89. Ciavolino, E.; Carpita, M.; Nitti, M. High-order PLS path model with qualitative external information. Qual. Quant. 2015, 49, 1609–1620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Signore, F.; Catalano, A.; De Carlo, E.; Madaro, A.; Ingusci, E. The role of employability in students during academic experience: A preliminary study through PLS-PM technique. Electron. J. Appl. Stat. Anal. 2019, 12, 720–747. [Google Scholar]
  91. Cheah, J.H.; Ting, H.; Ramayah, T.; Memon, M.A.; Cham, T.H.; Ciavolino, E. A comparison of five reflective–formative estimation approaches: Reconsideration and recommendations for tourism research. Qual. Quant. 2018, 53, 1421–1458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Hair, J.F., Jr.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.; Sarstedt, M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM); Sage Publications: Sauzend Oaks, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  93. Svingos, A.; Greif, S.; Bailey, B.; Heaton, S. The Relationship between Sleep and Cognition in Children Referred for Neuropsychological Evaluation: A Latent Modeling Approach. Children 2018, 5, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  94. Beebe, D.W.; Gozal, D. Obstructive sleep apnea and the prefrontal cortex: Towards a comprehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral deficits. J. Sleep Res. 2002, 11, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Alkadhi, K.; Zagaar, M.; Alhaider, I.; Salim, S.; Aleisa, A. Neurobiological consequences of sleep deprivation. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 2013, 11, 231–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  96. Horne, J.A. Sleep loss and divergent thinking ability. Sleep 1988, 11, 528–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Harrison, Y.; Horne, J. Sleep loss impairs short and novel language tasks having a prefrontal focus. J. Sleep Res. 1998, 7, 95–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Harrison, Y.; Horne, J.; Rothwell, A. Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults-a model for healthy aging? Sleep 2000, 23, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Jennings, J.R.; Monk, T.H.; Van der Molen, M.W. Sleep deprivation influences some but not all processes of supervisory attention. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 473–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Swann, C.E.; Yelland, G.W.; Redman, J.R.; Rajaratnam, S.M. Chronic partial sleep loss increases the facilitatory role of a masked prime in a word recognition task. J. Sleep Res. 2006, 15, 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Tressoldi, P.; Stella, G.; Faggella, M. The Development of Reading Speed in Italians with Dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 2001, 34, 414–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Orsolini, M.; Fanari, R.; Serra, G.; Cioce, R.; Rotondi, A.; Dassisti, A.; Maronato, C. Primi progressi nell’apprendimento della lettura: Una riconsiderazione del ruolo della consapevolezza fonologica. Psicol. Clin. Sviluppo. 2003, 7, 403–436. [Google Scholar]
  103. Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Di Pace, E.; Gasperini, F.; Judica, A.; Spinelli, D. Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain Lang. 2005, 93, 369–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Marinelli, C.V.; Romani, C.; Burani, C.; McGowan, V.A.; Zoccolotti, P. Costs and Benefits of Orthographic Inconsistency in Reading: Evidence from a Cross-Linguistic Comparison. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0157457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  105. Logan, G.D. Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychol. Rev. 1988, 95, 492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Marinelli, C.V.; Cellini, P.; Zoccolotti, P.; Angelelli, P. Lexical processing and distributional knowledge in sound–spelling mapping in a consistent orthography: A longitudinal study of reading and spelling in dyslexic and typically developing children. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2017, 34, 163–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Marinelli, C.V.; Spinelli, D. Predicting individual differences in reading, spelling, and maths in a sample of typical developing children: A study in the perspective of comorbidity. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0231937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Cain, K.; Oakhill, J. Reading Comprehension Difficulties. In Handbook of Children’s Literacy; Springer Science and Business Media LLC: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2004; pp. 313–338. [Google Scholar]
  109. Van Kraayenoord, C.E. The role of metacognition in reading comprehension. J. Pendidik. Bhs. 2010, 6, 277–302. [Google Scholar]
  110. Moss, J.; Schunn, C.D.; Schneider, W.; McNamara, D.S.; VanLehn, K. The neural correlates of strategic reading comprehension: Cognitive control and discourse comprehension. NeuroImage 2011, 58, 675–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Patael, S.Z.; Farris, E.A.; Black, J.M.; Hancock, R.; Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Cutting, L.E.; Hoeft, F. Brain basis of cognitive resilience: Prefrontal cortex predicts better reading comprehension in relation to decoding. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0198791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Di Pace, E.; Judica, A.; Orlandi, M.; Spinelli, D. Markers of developmental surface dyslexia in a language (Italian) with high grapheme–phoneme correspondence. Appl. Psycholinguist. 1999, 20, 191–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Judica, A.; De Luca, M.; Spinelli, D.; Zoccolotti, P. Training of developmental surface dyslexia improves reading performance and shortens eye fixation duration in reading. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2002, 12, 177–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Ellis, S.K.; Walczyk, J.J.; Buboltz, W.; Felix, V. The relationship between self-reported sleep quality and reading comprehension skills. Sleep Sci. 2014, 7, 189–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  115. Bruck, M. Component spelling skills of college students with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Learn. Disabil. Q. 1993, 16, 171–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Wolff, P.H.; Melngailis, I.; Kotwica, K. Family patterns of developmental dyslexia part III: Spelling errors as behavioral phenotype. Am. J. Med. Genet. 1996, 67, 378–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Hasher, L.; Zacks, R.T. Automatic and effortful processes in memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1979, 108, 356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Tucha, O.; Tucha, L.; Lange, K.W. Graphonomics, automaticity and handwriting assessment. Literacy 2008, 42, 145–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Berninger, V.W.; Swanson, H.L. Modifying Hayes and Flower’s model of skilled writing to explain beginning and developing writing. Adv. Cogn. Educ. Pract. 1994, 2, 57–81. [Google Scholar]
  120. Jones, D.; Christensen, C.A. Relationship between automaticity in handwriting and students’ ability to generate written text. J. Educ. Psychol. 1999, 91, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Arsalidou, M.; Taylor, M.J. Is 2 + 2 = 4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. Neuroimage 2011, 54, 2382–2393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Franzen, P.L.; Siegle, G.J.; Buysse, D.J. Relationships between affect, vigilance, and sleepiness following sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 2008, 17, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Owens, L.J.; France, K.G.; Wiggs, L. Review Article: Behavioural and cognitive-behavioural interventions for sleep disorders in infants and children: A review. Sleep Med. Rev. 1999, 3, 281–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  124. Marcus, C.L.; Radcliffe, J.; Konstantinopoulou, S.; Beck, S.E.; Cornaglia, M.A.; Traylor, J.; DiFeo, N.; Karamessinis, L.R.; Gallagher, P.R.; Meltzer, L.J. Effects of Positive Airway Pressure Therapy on Neurobehavioral Outcomes in Children with Obstructive Sleep Apnea. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2012, 185, 998–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Taylor, H.G.; Bowen, S.R.; Beebe, D.W.; Hodges, E.; Amin, R.; Arens, R.; Chervin, R.D.; Garetz, S.L.; Katz, E.S.; Moore, R.H.; et al. Cognitive Effects of Adenotonsillectomy for Obstructive Sleep Apnea. Pediatrics 2016, 138, e20154458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Formative or reflective blocks.
Figure 1. Formative or reflective blocks.
Brainsci 10 00529 g001
Figure 2. The inner and outer model.
Figure 2. The inner and outer model.
Brainsci 10 00529 g002
Figure 3. The model with path coefficients and loadings. ** p-value < 0.05, *** p-value < 0.001.
Figure 3. The model with path coefficients and loadings. ** p-value < 0.05, *** p-value < 0.001.
Brainsci 10 00529 g003
Table 1. Latent variables and indicators of the model.
Table 1. Latent variables and indicators of the model.
Latent VariablesLabelNo. of Indicators
SleepinessSleep1
Mathematical SkillsMath4
AttentionAtt4
SpellingSpe2
Oral ComprehensionOr Comp1
ReadingRead2
Visuo-Constructive AbilitiesVisc1
Working MemoryWM1
HandwritingHandw1
Reading ComprehensionRead Comp1
Table 2. Reliability analysis of latent variables.
Table 2. Reliability analysis of latent variables.
Cronbach αDillon-Goldstein ρ1st Eigenvalue2nd Eigenvalue
Sleepiness1.001.001.000.00
Mathematical Skills0.690.812.080.79
Attention 0.840.892.720.52
Spelling 0.820.921.690.31
Oral Comprehension 1.001.001.000.00
Read 0.760.891.610.39
Visuo-Constructive Abilities 1.001.001.000.00
Working Memory 1.001.001.000.00
Handwriting 1.001.001.000.00
Reading Comprehension1.001.001.000.00
Table 3. Outer model with loadings and communalities.
Table 3. Outer model with loadings and communalities.
IndicatorsLatent VariablesLoadingsCommunality
SleepinessSleepiness1.001.00
ComputationMathematical Skills0.690.47
Number SpellingMathematical Skills0.680.47
Number OrderingMathematical Skills0.720.51
Arithmetical FactsMathematical Skills0.780.61
Bell (30 s) 1Attention0.850.72
Bell (30 s) 2Attention0.760.57
Bell (30 s) 3Attention0.890.80
Bell (30 s) 4Attention0.770.60
RegularSpelling0.930.87
PseudowordsSpelling0.870.76
Oral ComprehensionOral Comprehension1.001.00
Text ComprehensionReading Comprehension1.001.00
Reading SpeedReading0.920.85
Reading AccuracyReading0.870.76
ReyVisuo-Constructive Abilities1.001.00
VPTAWorking Memory1.001.00
Handwriting SpeedHandwriting1.001.00
Table 4. Cross-loadings of the indicators. Indicators of each latent variable were reported in bold.
Table 4. Cross-loadings of the indicators. Indicators of each latent variable were reported in bold.
IndicatorsSLEMATATTSPEOR COMPREADVISCWMHANDWREAD COMP
Sleepiness1.00−0.18−0.22−0.18−0.25−0.11−0.02−0.06−0.12−0.14
Computation−0.100.690.300.410.380.350.360.220.260.16
Number Spelling−0.140.680.060.210.310.270.210.290.130.06
Number Ordering−0.110.720.090.350.330.310.440.240.210.07
Arithmetical Facts−0.140.780.180.280.350.400.400.450.260.21
Bell (30 s) 1−0.220.180.850.060.240.200.040.070.28−0.03
Bell (30 s) 2−0.100.140.760.100.130.140.160.040.26−0.03
Bell (30 s) 3−0.210.190.900.100.170.260.100.030.260.09
Bell (30 s) 4−0.140.160.770.120.090.120.150.040.16−0.10
Regular−0.180.350.090.930.160.330.300.120.160.12
Pseudowords−0.150.430.110.900.200.380.230.200.160.07
Oral Comprehension−0.250.470.200.201.000.300.240.260.190.29
Reading Speed−0.110.360.240.280.250.920.060.130.340.29
Reading Accuracy−0.090.500.160.430.290.870.070.310.290.31
Rey−0.020.480.120.290.240.071.000.290.260.01
VPTA−0.060.430.050.170.260.230.291.000.170.07
Handwriting Speed−0.120.290.290.170.190.350.260.171.000.23
Text Comprehension−0.140.18−0.010.110.290.340.010.070.231.00
Table 5. Bootstrap validation of inner model paths coefficient.
Table 5. Bootstrap validation of inner model paths coefficient.
RelationOriginalMean BootstrapStd. ErrorLower CIUpper CI
SLE ≥ MATH−0.1759−0.20230.0801−0.337−0.0626
SLE ≥ ATT−0.2177−0.23630.0768−0.373−0.1079
SLE ≥ SPE−0.1828−0.18610.0665−0.304−0.0603
SLE ≥ OR COMP−0.2510−0.25010.0703−0.376−0.1122
SLE ≥ Read−0.1132−0.12330.0929−0.2750.1083
SLE ≥ VISC−0.0240−0.02190.0803−0.1780.1388
SLE ≥ WM−0.0631−0.06140.0786−0.2120.0928
SLE ≥ HANDW−0.1173−0.11760.0784−0.2670.0338
SLE ≥ READ COMP−0.1376−0.13820.0927−0.3150.0352
Table 6. Bootstrap validation of loadings between indicators and latent variables (outer model).
Table 6. Bootstrap validation of loadings between indicators and latent variables (outer model).
OriginalMean BootstrapStd. ErrorLower CIUpper CI
SLE-Sleepiness1.0001.0000.0001.0001.000
MATH-Computation0.6860.6300.0210.0610.897
MATH-Number spelling0.6820.6470.0190.1990.912
MATH-Number ordering0.7160.6530.0180.1320.871
MATH-Arithmetical facts0.7820.7210.0170.3040.914
ATT-Bell (30 s) 10.8460.8350.0090.6880.929
ATT-Bell (30 s) 10.7560.7200.0130.3500.851
ATT-Bell (30 s) 10.8950.8760.0080.7270.939
ATT-Bell (30 s) 10.7740.7530.0100.5210.871
SPE-Regular0.9340.9140.0140.7450.994
SPE-Pseudowords0.9040.8800.0130.6310.955
OR COMP -Oral comprehension1.0001.0000.0001.0001.000
READ-Reading speed0.9210.8350.0270.0480.999
READ-Reading accuracy0.8720.7960.0270.0940.994
VISC-Rey1.0001.0000.0001.0001.000
WM-VPTA1.0001.0000.0001.0001.000
HANDW-Handwriting Speed1.0001.0000.0001.0001.000
READ COMP-Text Comprehension1.0001.0000.0001.0001.000

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Macchitella, L.; Marinelli, C.V.; Signore, F.; Ciavolino, E.; Angelelli, P. Sleepiness, Neuropsychological Skills, and Scholastic Learning in Children. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 529. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080529

AMA Style

Macchitella L, Marinelli CV, Signore F, Ciavolino E, Angelelli P. Sleepiness, Neuropsychological Skills, and Scholastic Learning in Children. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(8):529. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080529

Chicago/Turabian Style

Macchitella, Luigi, Chiara Valeria Marinelli, Fulvio Signore, Enrico Ciavolino, and Paola Angelelli. 2020. "Sleepiness, Neuropsychological Skills, and Scholastic Learning in Children" Brain Sciences 10, no. 8: 529. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080529

APA Style

Macchitella, L., Marinelli, C. V., Signore, F., Ciavolino, E., & Angelelli, P. (2020). Sleepiness, Neuropsychological Skills, and Scholastic Learning in Children. Brain Sciences, 10(8), 529. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080529

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop