Interpreting Developmental Surface Dyslexia within a Comorbidity Perspective
Abstract
:1. Introduction
“When people perform the same task repeatedly, obligatory encoding causes instance representations of the same act to be stored in memory. The more repetitions, the more instances are stored. Obligatory retrieval causes information to become available when familiar situations are encountered once again. The more instances there are in memory, the more will be retrieved (i.e., the response from memory will be stronger). The assumption of instance representation allows one to model the retrieval process as a race in which the fastest trace determines performance (i.e., performance is based on the first instance to be retrieved from memory).”
2. Aims of the Present Report
Modeling Reading within a Co-Morbidity Perspective
3. Interpretations of Surface Dyslexia in Acquired and Developmental Cases
Outcome versus Process Measures of the Individual Ability to Consolidate Specific Instances
4. The Ability to Consolidate Specific Traces: A Route to Understand Surface Dyslexia
Acquisition of Specific Instances
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Pennington, B.F. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition 2006, 101, 385–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coltheart, M.; Rastle, K.; Perry, C.; Langdon, R.; Ziegler, J.C. DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 108, 204–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Plaut, D.C.; McClelland, J.L.; Seidenberg, M.S.; Patterson, K. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 56–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Seidenberg, M.S.; McClelland, J.L. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychol. Rev. 1989, 96, 523–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perry, C.; Ziegler, J.C.; Zorzi, M. Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 114, 273–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dehaene, S. Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition 1992, 44, 1–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L. Towards an Anatomical and Functional Model of Number Processing. Math. Cogn. 1995, 1, 83–120. [Google Scholar]
- Slot, E.M.; van Viersen, S.; de Bree, E.H.; Kroesbergen, E.H. Shared and unique risk factors underlying mathematical disability and reading and spelling disability. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, A.J.; Andrewes, S.G.; Struthers, H.; Rowe, V.M.; Bogdanovic, R.; Waldie, K. Dyscalculia and dyslexia in adults: Cognitive bases of comorbidity. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015, 37, 118–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, D.; Xiao, Q.; Chen, Q.; Cui, J.; Zhou, X. Dyslexia and dyscalculia are characterized by common visual perception deficits. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2018, 43, 497–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willcutt, E.G.; Betjemann, R.S.; McGrath, L.; Chhabildas, N.A.; Olson, R.K.; DeFries, J.C.; Pennington, B.F. Etiology and neuropsychology of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple-deficit models. Cortex 2010, 46, 1345–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Marinelli, C.V.; Spinelli, D. Predicting individual differences in reading, spelling and maths in a sample of typically developing children: A study in the perspective of comorbidity. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0231937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Marinelli, C.V.; Spinelli, D. Testing the Specificity of Predictors of Reading, Spelling and Maths: A New Model of the Association Among Learning Skills Based on Competence, Performance and Acquisition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 573998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denckla, M.B.; Rudel, R.G. Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia 1976, 14, 471–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, N. Topics in the Theory of Generative Grammar; Janua Linguarum. Series Minor; Walter de Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 1966; Volume 56. [Google Scholar]
- Logan, G.D. Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychol. Rev. 1988, 95, 492–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Logan, G.D. Shapes of reaction-time distributions and shapes of learning curves: A test of the instance theory of automaticity. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1992, 18, 883–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bishop, D.V.M. Cognitive neuropsychology and developmental disorders: Uncomfortable bedfellows. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 1997, 50, 899–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blomert, L. The neural signature of orthographic–phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. NeuroImage 2011, 57, 695–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L. Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps. Neuron 2007, 56, 384–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L.; Sigman, M.; Vinckier, F. The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 335–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marsh, E.B.; Hillis, A.E. Cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying reading and naming: Evidence from letter-by-letter reading and optic aphasia. Neurocase 2005, 11, 325–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Marinelli, C.V.; Spinelli, D. Modeling individual differences in text reading fluency: A different pattern of predictors for typically developing and dyslexic readers. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kirby, J.R.; Georgiou, G.K.; Martinussen, R.; Parrila, R.; Bowers, P.; Landerl, K. Naming Speed and Reading: From Prediction to Instruction. Read. Res. Q. 2010, 45, 341–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reichle, E.D.; Rayner, K.; Pollatsek, A. The E-Z Reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behav. Brain Sci. 2003, 26, 445–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Snell, J.; van Leipsig, S.; Grainger, J.; Meeter, M. OB1-reader: A model of word recognition and eye movements in text reading. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 125, 969–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Brown, B.; Haegerstrom-Portnoy, G.; Adams, A.J.; Yingling, C.D.; Galin, D.; Herron, J.; Marcus, M. Predictive eye movements do not discriminate between dyslexic and control children. Neuropsychologia 1983, 21, 121–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanley, G.; Smith, G.A.; Howell, E.A. Eye-movements and sequential tracking in dyslexic and control children. Br. J. Psychol. 1983, 74, 181–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olson, R.K.; Kliegl, R.; Davidson, B.J. Dyslexic and normal readers’ eye movements. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1983, 9, 816–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Luca, M.; Di Pace, E.; Judica, A.; Spinelli, D.; Zoccolotti, P. Eye movement patterns in linguistic and non-linguistic tasks in developmental surface dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 1999, 37, 1407–1420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gagl, B.; Hawelka, S.; Hutzler, F. A similar correction mechanism in slow and fluent readers after suboptimal landing positions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Perfetti, C.A.; Goldman, S.R.; Hogaboam, T.W. Reading skill and the identification of words in discourse context. Mem. Cogn. 1979, 7, 273–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keresztes, A.; Ngo, C.T.; Lindenberger, U.; Werkle-Bergner, M.; Newcombe, N.S. Hippocampal Maturation Drives Memory from Generalization to Specificity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018, 22, 676–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Koponen, T.; Georgiou, G.; Salmi, P.; Leskinen, M.; Aro, M. A meta-analysis of the relation between RAN and mathematics. J. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 109, 977–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szucs, D.; Devine, A.; Soltesz, F.; Nobes, A.; Gabriel, F. Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex 2013, 49, 2674–2688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Huettig, F.; Lachmann, T.; Reis, A.; Petersson, K.M. Distinguishing cause from effect—many deficits associated with developmental dyslexia may be a consequence of reduced and suboptimal reading experience. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 33, 333–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Coltheart, M.; Masterson, J.; Byng, S.; Prior, M.; Riddoch, J. Surface dyslexia. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1983, 35, 469–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Castles, A.; Coltheart, M. Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition 1993, 47, 149–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castles, A. Cognitive Correlates of Developmental Surface Dyslexia: A Single Case Study. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 1996, 13, 25–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patterson, K.; Plaut, D.C.; McClelland, J.L.; Seidenberg, M.S.; Behrmann, M.; Hodges, J.R.; A Reggia, J.; Ruppin, E.; Berndt, R.S. Connections and disconnections: A connectionistic account of surface dyslexia. In Neural Modeling of Brain and Cognitive Disorders; World Scientific: Singapore, 1996; pp. 177–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Manis, F.R.; Seidenberg, M.S.; Doi, L.M.; McBride-Chang, C.; Petersen, A. On the bases of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition 1996, 58, 157–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, K.E.; Siegel, L.S.; Gottardo, A. Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. J. Educ. Psychol. 1997, 89, 114–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woollams, A.M.; Ralph, M.A.L.; Plaut, D.C.; Patterson, K. SD-squared: On the association between semantic dementia and surface dyslexia. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 114, 316–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Blazely, A.M.; Coltheart, M.; Casey, B.J. Semantic impairment with and without surface dyslexia: Implications for models of reading. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2005, 22, 695–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coltheart, M.; Tree, J.J.; Saunders, S.J. Computational Modeling of Reading in Semantic Dementia: Comment on Woollams, Lambon Ralph, Plaut, and Patterson (2007). Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 256–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baayen, R.H.; Wurm, L.H.; Aycock, J. Lexical Dynamics for Low-Frequency Complex Words: A Regression Study across Tasks and Modalities. Ment. Lex. 2007, 2, 419–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Burani, C.; Arduino, L.S.; Barca, L. Frequency, not age of acquisition affects Italian word naming. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2007, 19, 828–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peressotti, F.; Job, R. Reading aloud: Dissociating the semantic pathway from the non-semantic pathway of the lexical route. Read. Writ. 2003, 16, 179–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bryant, P.; Impey, L. The similarities between normal readers and developmental and acquired dyslexics. Cognition 1986, 24, 121–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coltheart, M. Varieties of developmental dyslexia: A comment on Bryant and Impey. Cognition 1987, 27, 97–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, K.E.; Cunningham, A.E. Studying the consequences of literacy within a literate society: The cognitive correlates of print exposure. Mem. Cogn. 1992, 20, 51–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cunningham, A.E.; Stanovich, K.E. Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. J. Educ. Psychol. 1990, 82, 733–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, R.L.; Pennington, B.F.; Olson, R.K. Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Testing the predictions of the dual-route and connectionist frameworks. Cognition 2013, 126, 20–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Exposure to Print and Orthographic Processing. Read. Res. Q. 1989, 24, 402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van den Broeck, W.; Geudens, A. Old and new ways to study characteristics of reading disability: The case of the nonword-reading deficit. Cogn. Psychol. 2012, 65, 414–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zoccolotti, P. The reading level matched design: Limitations and possible alternatives. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2020, 37, 523–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mervis, C.B.; Klein-Tasman, P.B. Methodological Issues in Group-Matching Designs: α Levels for Control Variable Comparisons and Measurement Characteristics of Control and Target Variables. J. Aut. Dev. Dis. 2004, 34, 7–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wybrow, D.P.; Hanley, J.R. Surface developmental dyslexia is as prevalent as phonological dyslexia when appropriate control groups are employed. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2014, 32, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Smedt, B.; Boets, B. Phonological processing and arithmetic fact retrieval: Evidence from developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 3973–3981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Angelelli, P.; Marinelli, C.V.; Zoccolotti, P. Single or dual orthographic representations for reading and spelling? A study of Italian dyslexic–dysgraphic and normal children. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2010, 27, 305–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marinelli, C.V.; Cellini, P.; Zoccolotti, P.; Angelelli, P. Lexical processing and distributional knowledge in sound–spelling mapping in a consistent orthography: A longitudinal study of reading and spelling in dyslexic and typically devel-oping children. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2017, 34, 163–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zoccolotti, P.; Angelelli, P.; Marinelli, C.; Romano, D. A Network Analysis of the Relationship among Reading, Spelling and Maths Skills. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kwok, R.; Ellis, A.W. Visual word learning in skilled readers of English. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2015, 68, 326–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martens, V.E.G.; de Jong, P.F. Effects of repeated reading on the length effect in word and pseudoword reading. J. Res. Read. 2008, 31, 40–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suã¡rez-Coalla, P.; Avdyli, R.; Cuetos, F. Influence of context-sensitive rules on the formation of orthographic representations in Spanish dyslexic children. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pontillo, M.; De Luca, M.; Ellis, A.W.; Marinelli, C.V.; Spinelli, D.; Zoccolotti, P. Failure to learn a new format in developmental dyslexia. Sci. Rep. 2014, 4, 4869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Marinelli, C.V.; Angelelli, P.; Martelli, M.; Trenta, M.; Zoccolotti, P. Ability to Consolidate Instances as a Proxy for the Association among Reading, Spelling, and Math Learning Skills. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 761696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Di Filippo, G.; Judica, A.; Martelli, M. Reading development in an orthographically regular lan-guage: Effects of length, frequency, lexicality and global processing ability. Read. Writ. 2009, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leach, J.M.; Scarborough, H.S.; Rescorla, L. Late-emerging reading disabilities. J. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 95, 211–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Temple, C.M. Surface Dyslexia: Variations Within a Syndrome. In Surface Dyslexia: Neuropsychological and Cognitive Studies of Phonological Reading, 1st ed.; Patterson, K.E., Marshall, J.C., Coltheart, M., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1985; pp. 269–288. [Google Scholar]
- Goulandris, N.K.; Snowling, M. Visual Memory Deficits: A Plausible Cause of Developmental Dyslexia? Evidence from a Single Case Study. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 1991, 8, 127–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanley, J.R.; Hastie, K.; Kay, J. Developmental Surface Dyslexia and Dysgraphia: An Orthographic Processing Impairment. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 1992, 44, 285–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanley, J.R.; Gard, F. A dissociation between developmental surface and phonological dyslexia in two undergraduate students. Neuropsychologia 1995, 33, 909–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Broom, Y.M.; Doctor, E.A. Developmental surface dyslexia: A case study of the efficacy of a remediation programme. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 1995, 12, 69–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friedmann, N.; Lukov, L. Developmental surface dyslexias. Cortex 2008, 44, 1146–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Angelelli, P.; Notarnicola, A.; Judica, A.; Zoccolotti, P.; Luzzatti, C. Spelling impairments in Italian dyslexic children: Phenomenological changes in primary school. Cortex 2010, 46, 1299–1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perfetti, C.A.; Seymour, P.H.K. Cognitive Analysis of Dyslexia. Am. J. Psychol. 1988, 101, 438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seymour, P.H.K.; MacGregor, C.J. Developmental dyslexia: A cognitive experimental analysis of phonological, morphemic, and visual impairments. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 1984, 1, 43–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Di Pace, E.; Judica, A.; Orlandi, M.; Spinelli, D. Markers of developmental surface dyslexia in a language (Italian) with high grapheme-phoneme correspondence. Appl. Psycholing. 1999, 20, 191–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barca, L.; Burani, C.; Di Filippo, G.; Zoccolotti, P. Italian developmental dyslexic and proficient readers: Where are the differences? Brain Lang. 2006, 98, 347–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Level of Analysis | General Function(s) | Main Characteristics | Functions in Reading | Expected Dissociation/Association |
---|---|---|---|---|
Competence | Activation of a specific set of representations and processes | -Domain-dependent -Task-independent -Sensitive to practice | Word reading based on orthographic–phonological binding | Dissociation between deficits in different domains |
Performance | Optimizing behavioral efficiency in everyday conditions | -Task-dependent -Partially domain-dependent -Sensitive to practice | -Left to right scanning -Programming saccadic eye movements for optimal landing position -Integration of decoding with utterance production -Use of contextual information, etc. | Association or dissociation depending on high/low similarity of task characteristics |
Acquisition: learning through practice | Emergence of a competence in a given domain (e.g., language, reading) through exposure and/or explicit teaching | -Domain-dependent | Building reading competence | See competence |
Mastering of typical task formats that are characteristic of a given behavior | -Partially domain-dependent | Tuning reading performance in everyday complex reading tasks | See performance | |
Automatization of performance, through the activation of specific memory traces (instances) | -Item specific -Domain-independent | Building a repertoire of words with immediate and obligatory retrieval and recognition (orthographic lexicon) | Association among deficits in different domains, including learning disturbances (comorbidity) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Marinelli, C.V. Interpreting Developmental Surface Dyslexia within a Comorbidity Perspective. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1568. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121568
Zoccolotti P, De Luca M, Marinelli CV. Interpreting Developmental Surface Dyslexia within a Comorbidity Perspective. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(12):1568. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121568
Chicago/Turabian StyleZoccolotti, Pierluigi, Maria De Luca, and Chiara Valeria Marinelli. 2021. "Interpreting Developmental Surface Dyslexia within a Comorbidity Perspective" Brain Sciences 11, no. 12: 1568. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121568
APA StyleZoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., & Marinelli, C. V. (2021). Interpreting Developmental Surface Dyslexia within a Comorbidity Perspective. Brain Sciences, 11(12), 1568. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121568