Effects of Oxytocin on Social Comparisons in Intergroup Situations
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.2.1. Treatment
2.2.2. Manipulation for Group Membership
2.2.3. Social Comparison Task
2.2.4. Protocol after Social Comparison Task
2.3. Data Analysis
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Taylor, S.E.; Lobel, M. Social comparison activity under threat: Downward evaluation and upward contacts. Psychol. Rev. 1989, 96, 569–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wheeler, L.; Miyake, K. Social comparison in everyday life. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 62, 760–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, A.E.; Oswald, A.J. Satisfaction and comparison income. J. Public Econ. 1996, 61, 359–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tenbrunsel, A.E.; Diekmann, K.A. Job-decision inconsistencies involving social comparison information: The role of dominating alternatives. J. Appl. Psychol. 2002, 87, 1149–1158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sheskin, M.; Bloom, P.; Wynn, K. Anti-equality: Social comparison in young children. Cognition 2014, 130, 152–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Corcoran, K.; Crusius, J.; Mussweiler, T. Social comparison: Motives, standards, and mechanisms. In Theories in Social Psychology; Chadee, D., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 119–139. [Google Scholar]
- Festinger, L. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum. Relat. 1954, 7, 117–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leach, C.W.; Spears, R. “A vengefulness of the impotent”: The pain of in-group inferiority and schadenfreude toward successful out-groups. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 1383–1396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoogland, C.E.; Schurtz, D.R.; Cooper, C.M.; Combs, D.J.; Brown, E.G.; Smith, R.H. The joy of pain and the pain of joy: In-group identification predicts schadenfreude and gluckschmerz following rival groups’ fortunes. Motiv. Emot. 2015, 39, 260–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sidanius, J.; Cotterill, S.; Sheehy-Skeffington, J.; Kteily, N.; Carvacho, H. Social dominance theory: Explorations in the psychology of oppression. In Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice; Sibley, C.G., Barlow, F.K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016; pp. 149–187. [Google Scholar]
- Cikara, M.; Botvinick, M.M.; Fiske, S.T. Us versus them: Social identity shapes neural responses to intergroup competition and harm. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 306–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- McDonald, M.M.; Navarrete, C.D.; Van Vugt, M. Evolution and the psychology of intergroup conflict: The male warrior hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2012, 367, 670–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Riek, B.M.; Mania, E.W.; Gaertner, S.L. Intergroup threat and outgroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 10, 336–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hammond, R.A.; Axelrod, R. The evolution of ethnocentrism. J. Conflict Resolut. 2006, 50, 926–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fiske, S.T. Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination at the seam between the centuries: Evolution, culture, mind, and brain. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 30, 299–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caldwell, H.K. Oxytocin and vasopressin: Powerful regulators of social behavior. Neuroscientist 2017, 23, 517–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feng, C.; Hackett, P.D.; Demarco, A.C. Oxytocin and vasopressin effects on the neural response to social cooperation are modulated by sex in humans. Brain Imaging Behav. 2014, 9, 754–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heinrichs, M.; von Dawans, B.; Domes, G. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2009, 30, 548–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Shamay-Tsoory, S.G.; Fischer, M.; Dvash, J.; Harari, H.; Perach-Bloom, N.; Levkovitz, Y. Intranasal administration of oxytocin increases envy and schadenfreude (gloating). Biol. Psychiatry 2009, 66, 864–870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Dreu, C.K.W. Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: An integrative review and research agenda. Horm. Behav. 2012, 61, 419–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Dreu, C.K.W.; Greer, L.L.; Van Kleef, G.A.; Shalvi, S.; Handgraaf, M.J.J. Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 1262–1266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Luo, S.; Li, B.; Ma, Y.; Zhang, W.; Rao, Y.; Han, S. Oxytocin receptor gene and racial ingroup bias in empathy-related brain activity. NeuroImage 2015, 110, 22–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sheng, F.; Liu, Y.; Zhou, W.; Han, S. Oxytocin modulates the racial bias in neural responses to others’ suffering. Biol. Psychol. 2013, 92, 380–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Dreu, C.K.W.; Greer, L.L.; Handgraaf, M.J.J.; Shalvi, S.; Van Kleef, G.A.; Baas, M.; Feith, S.W.W. The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 2010, 328, 1408–1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kosfeld, M.; Heinrichs, M.; Zak, P.J.; Fischbacher, U.; Fehr, E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 2005, 435, 673–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Benenson, J.F.; Markovits, H.; Emery Thompson, M.; Wrangham, R.W. Strength determines coalitional strategies in humans. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 276, 2589–2595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Dreu, C.K.W.; Greer, L.L.; Handgraaf, M.J.J.; Shalvi, S.; Van Kleef, G.A. Oxytocin modulates selection of allies in intergroup conflict. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2012, 279, 1150–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shamay-tsoory, S.G.; Abu-akel, A. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol. Psychiatry 2016, 79, 194–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartz, J.A.; Zaki, J.; Bolger, N.; Ochsner, K.N. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: Context and person matter. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 301–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olff, M.; Frijling, J.L.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Bradley, B.; Ellenbogen, M.A.; Cardoso, C.; Bartz, J.A.; Yee, J.R.; van Zuiden, M. The role of oxytocin in social bonding, stress regulation and mental health: An update on the moderating effects of context and interindividual differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013, 38, 1883–1894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Marsh, N.; Scheele, D.; Feinstein, J.S.; Gerhardt, H.; Strang, S.; Maier, W.; Hurlemann, R. Oxytocin-enforced norm compliance reduces xenophobic outgroup rejection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 9314–9319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Garcia, S.M.; Tor, A.; Schiff, T.M. The psychology of competition: A social comparison perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 8, 634–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, P.; Lee, Y.; Choi, I.; Kim, H. Neural evidence for individual and cultural variability in the social comparison effect. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 16200–16208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shin, N.Y.; Park, H.Y.; Jung, W.H.; Park, J.W.; Yun, J.Y.; Jang, J.H.; Kim, S.N.; Han, H.J.; Kim, S.; Kang, D. Effects of oxytocin on neural response to facial expressions in patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015, 40, 1919–1927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marsh, A.A.; Yu, H.H.; Pine, D.S.; Blair, R.J.R. Oxytocin improves specific recognition of positive facial expressions. Psychopharmacology 2010, 209, 225–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Born, J.; Lange, T.; Kern, W.; McGregor, G.P.; Bickel, U.; Fehm, H.L. Sniffing neuropeptides: A transnasal approach to the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2002, 5, 514–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tajfel, H. Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Sci. Am. 1970, 223, 96–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sul, S.; Lee, M.; Kim, H. Measuring individual differences in altruism with altruistic learning task. Korean J. Psychol. Gen. 2014, 33, 467–492. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Y.; Kim, H. Social comparison influences risky financial decisions. Korean J. Consum. Advert. Psychol. 2011, 12, 589–613. [Google Scholar]
- Wayment, H.A.; Taylor, S.E. Self-evaluation processes: Motives, information use, and self-esteem. J. Personal. 1995, 63, 729–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, R.H.; Parrott, W.G.; Diener, E.F.; Hoyle, R.H.; Kim, S.H. Dispositional envy. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1999, 25, 1007–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenberg, M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1965; pp. 16–36. [Google Scholar]
- Levin, S.; Sidanius, J. Social dominance and social identity in the United States and Israel: Ingroup favoritism or outgroup derogation? Political Psychol. 1999, 20, 99–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhawuk, D.P.S. Individualism and collectivism. In The International Encyclopedia of Intercultural Communication; Kim, Y.Y., Ed.; Wiley & Sons: Malden, MA, USA, 2018; Volume 2, pp. 920–929. [Google Scholar]
- Sturmer, S.; Snyder, M.; Omoto, A.M. Prosocial emotions and helping: The moderating role of group membership. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 88, 532–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pratto, F.; Sidanius, J.; Stallworth, L.M.; Malle, B.F. Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 741–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singelis, T.M.; Triandis, H.C.; Bhawuk, D.; Gelfand, M.J. Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cult. Res. 1995, 29, 240–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, M.H. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Cat. Sel. Doc. Psychol. 1980, 10, 85. [Google Scholar]
- De Dreu, C.K.W.; Kret, M.E. Oxytocin conditions intergroup relations through upregulated in-group empathy, cooperation, conformity, and defense. Biol. Psychiatry 2016, 79, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stallen, M.; De Dreu, C.K.W.; Shalvi, S.; Smidts, A.; Sanfey, A.G. The herding hormone: Oxytocin stimulates in-group conformity. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 1288–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ten Velden, F.S.; Daughters, K.; De Dreu, C.K.W. Oxytocin promotes intuitive rather than deliberated cooperation with the in-group. Horm. Behav. 2017, 92, 164–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Israel, S.; Weisel, O.; Ebstein, R.P.; Bornstein, G. Oxytocin, but not vasopressin, increases both parochial and universal altruism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012, 37, 1341–1344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Shamay-Tsoory, S.G.; Abu-Akel, A.; Palgi, S.; Sulieman, R.; Fischer-Shofty, M.; Levkovitz, Y.; Decety, J. Giving peace a chance: Oxytocin increases empathy to pain in the context of the Israeli-Palestinian conflict. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013, 38, 3139–3144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Terris, E.T.; Beavin, L.E.; Barraza, J.A.; Schloss, J.; Zak, P.J. Endogenous oxytocin release eliminates in-group bias in monetary transfers with perspective-taking. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hart, A.J.; Whalen, P.J.; Shin, L.M.; McInerney, S.C.; Fischer, H.; Rauch, S.L. Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs. ingroup face stimuli. Neuroreport 2000, 11, 2351–2355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Roozendaal, B.; McEwen, B.S.; Chattarji, S. Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 423–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salam, A.P.; Rainford, E.; van Vugt, M.; Ronay, R. Acute stress reduces perceived trustworthiness of male racial outgroup faces. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2017, 3, 282–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spengler, F.B.; Schultz, J.; Scheele, D.; Essel, M.; Maier, W.; Heinrichs, M.; Hurlemann, R. Kinetics and dose dependency of intranasal oxytocin effects on amygdala reactivity. Biol. Psychiatry 2017, 82, 885–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salonia, A.; Nappi, R.E.; Pontillo, M.; Daverio, R.; Smeraldi, A.; Briganti, A.; Fabbri, F.; Zanni, G.; Rigatti, P.; Montorsi, F. Menstrual cycle-related changes in plasma oxytocin are relevant to normal sexual function in healthy women. Horm. Behav. 2005, 47, 164–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Acevedo-Rodriguez, A.; Mani, S.; Handa, R.J. Oxytocin and estrogen receptor β in the brain: An overview. Front. Endocrinol. 2015, 6, 160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kret, M.E.; De Dreu, C.K.W. Oxytocin-motivated ally selection is moderated by fetal testosterone exposure and empathic concern. Front. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Houston, J.M.; Faresse, D.M.; La Du, T.J. Assessing competitiveness: A validation study of the Competitiveness Index. Pers. Individ. Differ. 1992, 13, 1153–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duckitt, J.; Sibley, C. A dual-process motivational model of ideology, politics, and prejudice. Psychol. Inq. 2009, 20, 98–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rimmele, U.; Hediger, K.; Heinrichs, M.; Klaver, P. Oxytocin makes a face in memory familiar. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 38–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
OXT | Placebo | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Congruence | Incongruence | Congruence | Incongruence | |
In-group | 0.91 (0.09) | 0.15 (0.14) | 0.88 (0.15) | 0.24 (0.26) |
Out-group | 0.89 (0.13) | 0.29 (0.26) | 0.87 (0.15) | 0.26 (0.24) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kim, E.Y.; Sul, S.; Lee, M.W.; Lim, K.-O.; Shin, N.Y.; Kim, S.N.; Kwon, J.S.; Kim, H. Effects of Oxytocin on Social Comparisons in Intergroup Situations. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1227. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091227
Kim EY, Sul S, Lee MW, Lim K-O, Shin NY, Kim SN, Kwon JS, Kim H. Effects of Oxytocin on Social Comparisons in Intergroup Situations. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(9):1227. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091227
Chicago/Turabian StyleKim, Eun Young, Sunhae Sul, Min Woo Lee, Kyung-Ok Lim, Na Young Shin, Sung Nyun Kim, Jun Soo Kwon, and Hackjin Kim. 2021. "Effects of Oxytocin on Social Comparisons in Intergroup Situations" Brain Sciences 11, no. 9: 1227. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091227
APA StyleKim, E. Y., Sul, S., Lee, M. W., Lim, K. -O., Shin, N. Y., Kim, S. N., Kwon, J. S., & Kim, H. (2021). Effects of Oxytocin on Social Comparisons in Intergroup Situations. Brain Sciences, 11(9), 1227. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091227