Numerical Training Videos and Early Numerical Achievement: A Study on 3-Year-Old Preschoolers
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Domain-Specific Precursors
1.2. Domain-General Precursors
1.3. Numeracy Training Studies
1.4. The Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.2.1. Pretest Phase
2.2.2. Training Phase
2.2.3. Posttest Phase
2.2.4. Follow-Up Phase
2.3. Pretest Control Measures
2.3.1. Receptive Vocabulary
2.3.2. Selective Attention
2.3.3. Visuospatial Low-Control WM
2.3.4. Verbal Low-Control WM
2.4. Pretest and Posttest Numeracy Measures
2.4.1. Counting
2.4.2. Cardinality
2.4.3. Number Line Knowledge
2.5. Characteristics of the Intervention
2.5.1. Numerical Training Group Intervention
2.5.2. Control Group Intervention
3. Results
3.1. Pretest Assessment
3.2. Posttest Assessment
3.3. Follow-Up Assessment
4. Discussion
Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Bynner, J.M. Basic skills in adolescents’ occupational preparation. Career Dev. Q. 1997, 45, 305–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rivera-Batiz, F.L. Quantitative literacy and the likelihood of employment among young adults in the United States. J. Hum. Resour. 1992, 27, 313–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dougherty, C. Numeracy, literacy and earnings: Evidence from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Econ. Educ. Rev. 2003, 22, 511–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gerardi, K.; Goette, L.; Meier, S. Numerical ability predicts mortgage default. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 11267–11271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gross, J.; Hudson, C.; Price, D. The Long Term Costs of Numeracy Difficulties; Every Child a Chance Trust and KPMG: East Sussex, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Barbaresi, W.J.; Katusic, S.K.; Colligan, R.C.; Weaver, A.L.; Jacobsen, S.J. Math learning disorder: Incidence in a population-based birth cohort, 1976–82, Rochester. Minn. Ambul. Pediatr. 2005, 5, 281–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Butterworth, B. Foundational numerical capacities and the origins of dyscalculia. In Space, Time and Number in the Brain; Academic Press: London, UK, 2011; pp. 249–265. [Google Scholar]
- Shalev, R.S. Prevalence of developmental dyscalculia. In Why Is Math so Hard for Some Children? The Nature and Origins of Mathematical Learning Difficulties and Disabilities; Brookes Publishing: Baltimore, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 49–60. [Google Scholar]
- Shalev, R.S.; Manor, O.; Gross-Tsur, V. Developmental dyscalculia: A prospective six-year follow-up. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2005, 47, 121–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Duncan, G.J.; Dowsett, C.J.; Claessens, A.; Magnuson, K.; Huston, A.C.; Klebanov, P.; Pagani, L.S.; Feinstein, L.; Engel, M.; Brooks-Gunn, J.; et al. School readiness and later achievement. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 1428–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gathercole, S.E.; Pickering, S.J.; Knight, C.; Stegmann, Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2004, 18, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroesbergen, E.H.; Van Luit, J.E.H.; Van Lieshout, E.C.D.M.; Van Loosbroek, E.; Van de Rijt, B.A.M. Individual differences in early numeracy: The role of executive functions and subitizing. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2009, 27, 226–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Träff, U. The contribution of general cognitive abilities and number abilities to different aspects of mathematics in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 116, 139–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aunola, K.; Leskinen, E.; Lerkkanen, M.K.; Nurmi, J.E. Developmental dynamics of math performance from preschool to grade 2. J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 96, 699–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aunio, P.; Niemivirta, M. Predicting children’s mathematical performance in grade one by early numeracy. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2010, 20, 427–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jordan, N.C.; Kaplan, D.; Locuniak, M.N.; Ramineni, C. Predicting first-grade math achievement from developmental number sense trajectories. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2007, 22, 36–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geary, D.C. Mathematics and learning disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 2004, 37, 4–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Passolunghi, M.C.; Vercelloni, B.; Schadee, H. The precursors of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological ability and numerical competence. Cogn. Dev. 2007, 22, 165–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jordan, N.C.; Kaplan, D.; Ramineni, C.; Locuniak, M.N. Early math matters: Kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 2009, 45, 850–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Martin, R.B.; Cirino, P.T.; Sharp, C.; Barnes, M. Number and counting skills in kindergarten as predictors of grade 1 mathematical skills. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2014, 34, 12–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nguyen, T.; Watts, T.W.; Duncan, G.J.; Clements, D.H.; Sarama, J.S.; Wolfe, C.; Spitler, M.E. Which preschool mathematics competencies are most predictive of fifth grade achievement? Early Child. Res. 2016, 36, 550–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Koponen, T.; Aunola, K.; Nurmi, J.E. Verbal counting skill predicts later math performance and difficulties in middle school. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2019, 59, 101803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gelman, R.; Gallistel, C.R. The Child’s Concept of Number; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Halberda, J.; Taing, L.; Lidz, J. The development of “most” comprehension and its potential dependence on counting ability in preschoolers. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2008, 4, 99–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Le Corre, M.; Carey, S. One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition 2007, 105, 395–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wynn, K. Children’s understanding of counting. Cognition 1990, 36, 155–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuson, K.C. Children’s Counting and Concepts of Number; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Mix, K.S.; Sandhofer, C.M.; Moore, J.A.; Russell, C. Acquisition of the cardinal word principle: The role of input. Early Child. Res. Q. 2012, 27, 274–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paliwal, V.; Baroody, A.J. How best to teach the cardinality principle? Early Child. Res. 2018, 44, 152–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, D.C.; vanMarle, K.; Chu, F.W.; Rouder, J.; Hoard, M.K.; Nugent, L. Early conceptual understanding of cardinality predicts superior school-entry number-system knowledge. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 29, 191–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chu, F.W.; Geary, D.C. Early numerical foundations of young children’s mathematical development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015, 132, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sarnecka, B.W.; Carey, S. How counting represents number: What children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition 2008, 108, 662–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sarnecka, B.W.; Wright, C.E. The idea of an exact number: Children’s understanding of cardinality and equinumerosity. Cogn. Sci. 2013, 37, 1493–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ansari, D.; Donlan, C.; Thomas, M.S.; Ewing, S.A.; Peen, T.; Karmiloff-Smith, A. What makes counting count? Verbal and visuo-spatial contributions to typical and atypical number development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2003, 85, 50–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallistel, C.R.; Gelman, R. Non-verbal numerical cognition: From reals to integers. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 59–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hubbard, E.M.; Piazza, M.; Pinel, P.; Dehaene, S. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 6, 435–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegler, R.S. Magnitude knowledge: The common core of numerical development. Dev. Sci. 2016, 19, 341–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schneider, M.; Merz, S.; Stricker, J.; De Smedt, B.; Torbeyns, J.; Verschaffel, L.; Luwel, K. Associations of number line estimation with mathematical competence: A meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 1467–1484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McLean, J.F.; Hitch, G.H. Working memory impairments in children with specific mathematics learning difficulties. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1999, 74, 240–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Raghubar, K.P.; Barnes, M.A.; Hecht, S.A. Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2010, 20, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A.D. Is working memory still working? Eur. Psychol. 2002, 7, 85–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A.D.; Allen, R.J.; Hitch, G.J. Binding in visual working memory: The role of the episodic buffer. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 1393–1400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baddeley, A.D.; Hitch, G. Working Memory. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1974; Volume 8, pp. 47–89. [Google Scholar]
- Cornoldi, C.; Vecchi, T. Visuo-Spatial Working Memory and Individual Differences; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- De Vita, C.; Costa, H.M.; Tomasetto, C.; Passolunghi, M.C. The contributions of working memory domains and processes to early mathematical knowledge between preschool and first grade. Psychol. Res. 2021, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Smedt, B.; Janssen, R.; Bouwens, K.; Verschaffel, L.; Boets, B.; Ghesquière, P. Working memory and individual differences in mathematics achievement: A longitudinal study from first grade to second grade. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2009, 103, 186–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Menon, V. Working memory in children’s math learning and its disruption in dyscalculia. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 10, 125–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Passolunghi, M.C.; Siegel, L.S. Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2004, 88, 348–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fanari, R.; Meloni, C.; Massidda, D. Visual and spatial working memory abilities predict early math skills: A longitudinal study. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Friso-Van den Bos, I.; Van der Ven, S.H.; Kroesbergen, E.H.; Van Luit, J.E. Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 2013, 10, 29–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rasmussen, C.; Bisanz, J. Representation and working memory in early arithmetic. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2005, 91, 137–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whyte, J.C.; Bull, R. Number games, magnitude representation, and basic number skills in preschoolers. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 588–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kyttälä, M.; Kanerva, K.; Kroesbergen, E. Training counting skills and working memory in preschool. Scand. J. Psychol. 2015, 56, 363–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramani, G.B.; Siegler, R.S. Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children’s numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 375–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Passolunghi, M.C.; Costa, H.M. Working memory and early numeracy training in preschool children. Child Neuropsychol. 2016, 22, 81–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schacter, J.; Jo, B. Improving low-income preschoolers mathematics achievement with Math Shelf, a preschool tablet computer curriculum. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 55, 223–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young-Loveridge, J.M. Effects on early numeracy of a program using number books and games. Early Child. Res. Q. 2004, 19, 82–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sella, F.; Tressoldi, P.; Lucangeli, D.; Zorzi, M. Training numerical skills with the adaptive videogame “The Number Race”: A randomized controlled trial on preschoolers. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2016, 5, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroesbergen, E.H.; Van’t Noordende, J.E.; Kolkman, M.E. Number sense in low-performing kindergarten children: Effects of a working memory and an early math training. In Reading, Writing, Mathematics and the Developing Brain: Listening to Many Voices; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 295–313. [Google Scholar]
- Melby-Lervåg, M.; Hulme, C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 270–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Melby-Lervåg, M.; Redick, T.S.; Hulme, C. Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer” evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 11, 512–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Thorell, L.B.; Lindqvist, S.; Bergman Nutley, S.; Bohlin, G.; Klingberg, T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gade, M.; Zoelch, C.; Seitz-Stein, K. Training of visual-spatial working memory in preschool children. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2017, 13, 177–187. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Siegler, R.S.; Ramani, G.B. Playing linear number board games—But not circular ones—Improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. J. Educ. Psychol. 2009, 101, 545–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Siegler, R.S.; Booth, J. Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 428–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rideout, V.; Robb, M.B. The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Kids Age Zero to Eight; Common Sense Media: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Marsh, J.; Law, L.; Lahmar, J.; Yamada-Rice, D.; Parry, B.; Scott, F.; Robinson, P.; Nutbrown, B.; Scholey, E.; Baldi, P.; et al. Social Media, Television and Children; University of Sheffield: Sheffield, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- DeLoache, J.S.; Chiong, C.; Sherman, K.; Islam, N.; Vanderborght, M.; Troseth, G.L.; Strouse, G.A.; O’Doherty, K. Do babies learn from baby media? Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 1570–1574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, D.R.; Huston, A.C.; Schmitt, K.L.; Linebarger, D.L.; Wright, J.C.; Larson, R. Early childhood television viewing and adolescent behavior: The recontact study. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child. 2001, 66, 1–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fisch, S.M.; Truglio, R.T.; Cole, C.F. The impact of Sesame Street on preschool children: A review and synthesis of 30 years’ research. Media Psychol. 1999, 1, 165–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rice, M.L.; Huston, A.C.; Truglio, R.; Wright, J.C. Words from “Sesame Street”: Learning vocabulary while viewing. Dev. Psychol. 1990, 26, 421–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Troseth, G.L. Is it life or is it Memorex? Video as a representation of reality. Dev. Rev. 2010, 30, 155–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ball, S.; Bogatz, G.A. A Summary of the Major Findings in “The First Year of Sesame Street: An Evaluation”; Educational Testing Service: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1970; pp. 1–33. [Google Scholar]
- Bogatz, G.A.; Ball, S. The Second Year of Sesame Street: A Continuing Evaluation: A Report to the Children’s Television Workshop; Educational Testing Service: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1971; Volume 1, pp. 1–310. [Google Scholar]
- Linebarger, D.L.; Walker, D. Infants’ and toddlers’ television viewing and language outcomes. Am. Behav. Sci. 2005, 48, 624–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mares, M.L.; Pan, Z. Effects of Sesame Street: A meta-analysis of children’s learning in 15 countries. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 34, 140–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lauricella, A.R.; Gola, A.A.H.; Calvert, S.L. Toddlers’ learning from socially meaningful video characters. Media Psychol. 2011, 14, 216–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aladé, F.; Lauricella, A.R.; Beaudoin-Ryan, L.; Wartella, E. Measuring with Murray: Touchscreen technology and preschoolers’ STEM learning. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 62, 433–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schroeder, E.L.; Kirkorian, H.L. When seeing is better than doing: Preschoolers’ transfer of STEM skills using touchscreen games. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geary, D.C.; vanMarle, K. Young children’s core symbolic and nonsymbolic quantitative knowledge in the prediction of later mathematics achievement. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 2130–2144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, 4th ed.; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Gugliotta, M.; Bisiacchi, P.S.; Cendron, M.; Tressoldi, P.M.; Vio, C. BVN 12-18: Batteria di Valutazione Neuropsicologica per L’adolescenza; Erickson: Trento, Italy, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Lanfranchi, S.; Cornoldi, C.; Vianello, R. Verbal and visuospatial working memory deficits in children with Down syndrome. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 2004, 109, 456–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Molin, A.; Poli, S.; Lucangeli, D. BIN 4-6. Batteria per la Valutazione dell’intelligenza Numerica in Bambini dai 4 a 6 anni; Erikson: Trento, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Laski, E.V.; Siegler, R.S. Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 1723–1743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1988; pp. 4–579. [Google Scholar]
- Alloway, T.P.; Bibile, V.; Lau, G. Computerized working memory training: Can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013, 29, 632–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brehmer, Y.; Westerberg, H.; Bäckman, L. Working-memory training in younger and older adults: Training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pellizzoni, S.; Apuzzo, G.M.; De Vita, C.; Agostini, T.; Passolunghi, M.C. Evaluation and training of executive functions in genocide survivors. The case of Yazidi children. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raghubar, K.P.; Barnes, M.A. Early numeracy skills in preschool-aged children: A review of neurocognitive findings and implications for assessment and intervention. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2017, 31, 329–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van De Rijt, B.A.; Van Luit, J.E. Milestones in the development of infant numeracy. Scand. J. Psychol. 1999, 40, 65–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, C.; Burr, S.D.L.; Douglas, H.; Susperreguy, M.I.; LeFevre, J.A. Number line development of Chilean children from preschool to the end of kindergarten. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2021, 208, 105144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burroughs, B. YouTube kids: The app economy and mobile parenting. Soc. Media Soc. 2017, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caviola, S.; Toffalini, E.; Giofrè, D.; Ruiz, J.M.; Szűcs, D.; Mammarella, I.C. Math performance and academic anxiety forms, from sociodemographic to cognitive aspects: A meta-analysis on 906,311 participants. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2021, 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pellizzoni, S.; Cargnelutti, E.; Cuder, A.; Passolunghi, M.C. The interplay between math anxiety and working memory on math performance: A longitudinal study. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2021, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Training Videos | |||
Topic | Duration in Minutes | Content Description | |
1 | Counting | 4 | Characters were involved in counting activities from 1 to 9. Example: puppets had to count mushrooms’ white dots. |
2 | Counting | 4 | Characters were involved in counting activities from 1 to 10. Example: puppets had to count fruit sets that were placed inside a case. |
3 | Counting | 4 | Characters were involved in counting activities from 1 to 10. Example: puppets counted candies placed inside a plastic bottle. |
4 | Number line | 4 | Characters were involved in arranging numbers on a number line from 1 to 5. Example: puppets had to sort some jars with numbers printed on them in a number line. |
5 | Number line | 4 | Characters were involved in arranging numbers on a number line from 1 to 5. Example: puppets had to sort digits on a number line. |
6 | Number line | 4 | Characters were involved in arranging numbers on a number line from 1 to 10. Example: puppets had to sort ten stones with digits printed on them on a number line. |
Active Control Group Videos | |||
Topic | Duration in Minutes | Content Description | |
1 | Colors | 4 | Characters were involved in naming colors. Example: puppets had to name the colors of flower’s petals (red, blue, green, yellow). |
2 | Colors | 4 | Characters were involved in naming colors. Example: puppets had to name the colors of fruits (red, orange, green, purple, yellow). |
3 | Colors | 4 | Characters were involved in naming colors. Example: puppets had to name the colors of candies (red, orange, purple, yellow). |
4 | Animals | 4 | Characters were involved in naming animals. Example: puppets had to name jungle animals (tiger, monkey, elephant, parrot, giraffe). |
5 | Animals | 4 | Characters were involved in naming animals. Example: puppets had to name wood animals (wolf, owl, deer, fox). |
6 | Animals | 4 | Characters were involved in naming animals. Example: puppets had to name sea animals (whale, shrimp, turtle, shark). |
Pretest Assessment | ||||||||
Measures | R | Training Group (n = 42) | Control Group (n = 43) | Min. | Max. | F | ||
M | SD | M | SD | |||||
Receptive vocabulary | 0.94 | 10.7 | 3.24 | 11.3 | 3.07 | 1 | 17 | 0.79 |
Selective attention | 0.92 | 16.2 | 4.30 | 16.2 | 2.85 | 5 | 20 | 0.01 |
Visuospatial WM | 0.80 | 2.44 | 1.18 | 2.51 | 1.03 | 0 | 4 | 0.12 |
Verbal WM | 0.89 | 3.02 | 0.87 | 2.88 | 0.54 | 0 | 4 | 0.80 |
Counting | 0.83 | 7.84 | 5.19 | 8.81 | 4.47 | 0 | 20 | 1.15 |
Cardinality | 0.84 | 2.30 | 2.88 | 3.02 | 2.64 | 0 | 9 | 1.97 |
Number line knowledge | 0.75 | 1.40 | 1.14 | 1.67 | 1.32 | 0 | 5 | 1.42 |
Post-test Assessment | ||||||||
Measures | R | Training Group (n = 42) | Control Group (n = 43) | Min. | Max. | Fgains | ||
M | SD | M | SD | |||||
Visuospatial WM | 0.80 | 2.67 | 0.94 | 3.09 | 0.97 | 0 | 5 | 2.03 |
Verbal WM | 0.89 | 3.38 | 0.76 | 3.12 | 0.50 | 0 | 5 | 0.73 |
Counting | 0.83 | 12.5 | 4.41 | 8.51 | 4.59 | 0 | 20 | 25.8 *** |
Cardinality | 0.84 | 4.58 | 3.51 | 5.02 | 2.76 | 0 | 9 | 0.19 |
Number line knowledge | 0.75 | 1.65 | 1.15 | 1.58 | 1.18 | 0 | 5 | 1.16 |
Follow-Up Assessment | ||||||||
Measures | R | Training Group (n = 33) | Control Group (n = 41) | Min. | Max. | Fgains | ||
M | SD | M | SD | |||||
Counting | 0.83 | 14.2 | 4.86 | 10.8 | 5.81 | 0 | 20 | 14.9 *** |
Cardinality | 0.84 | 6.85 | 2.76 | 5.07 | 2.99 | 0 | 9 | 11.5 ** |
Number line knowledge | 0.75 | 1.24 | 0.94 | 1.71 | 1.19 | 0 | 5 | 0.05 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Cuder, A.; Vidoz, M.; De Vita, C.; Pellizzoni, S.; Passolunghi, M.C. Numerical Training Videos and Early Numerical Achievement: A Study on 3-Year-Old Preschoolers. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12010088
Cuder A, Vidoz M, De Vita C, Pellizzoni S, Passolunghi MC. Numerical Training Videos and Early Numerical Achievement: A Study on 3-Year-Old Preschoolers. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(1):88. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12010088
Chicago/Turabian StyleCuder, Alessandro, Marta Vidoz, Chiara De Vita, Sandra Pellizzoni, and Maria Chiara Passolunghi. 2022. "Numerical Training Videos and Early Numerical Achievement: A Study on 3-Year-Old Preschoolers" Brain Sciences 12, no. 1: 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12010088
APA StyleCuder, A., Vidoz, M., De Vita, C., Pellizzoni, S., & Passolunghi, M. C. (2022). Numerical Training Videos and Early Numerical Achievement: A Study on 3-Year-Old Preschoolers. Brain Sciences, 12(1), 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12010088