Domain-General Cognitive Skills in Children with Mathematical Difficulties and Dyscalculia: A Systematic Review of the Literature
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Identify the cognitive abilities most impaired in school-age children with MD, independent of the degree of severity and the instruments used for assessment;
- Define which cognitive areas might be helpful to assess to support the potential diagnosis of DD;
- Pinpoint future research areas to enable a more accurate identification of children with mathematical difficulties at risk of DD.
2. Method
2.1. Research Strategies
2.2. Eligibility Criteria
2.3. Data Collection and Quality Assessment
3. Results
3.1. Studies Selection
3.2. Quality Assessment
3.3. Characteristics of Selected Studies
3.3.1. Characteristics of Cross-Sectional Studies (N = 31)
3.3.2. Characteristics of Longitudinal Studies (N = 15)
3.4. Results on Cognitive Functioning (N = 46)
3.4.1. Processing Speed (N = 22)
Synthesis of Results and Comments
3.4.2. Short- and Long-Term Memory (N = 12)
Synthesis of Results and Comments
3.4.3. Attention (N = 9)
Synthesis of Results and Comments
3.4.4. Executive Functions
Working Memory (N = 32)
Inhibition and Interference Control (N = 8)
Cognitive Flexibility (N = 7)
Synthesis of Results and Comments
3.4.5. Phonological Processing and Phonological Awareness (N = 4)
Synthesis of Results and Comments
4. Discussion
5. Limitations
6. Conclusions
7. Open Challenges and Research Directions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Author, Year of Publication | Sample Groups (N 1, Gender, Mean Age) | Intelligence Assesment | Screening Measures for Mathematics (Test) and Criteria for MD 2 Group | Cognitive Domain | Task | Outcome |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Webster, 1980 [55] | MD-2 = 26 11.4 (0.26) MD-1 = 24 11.39 (0.29) CG 3 = 26 11.53 (0.25) | PPVT 4 | WRAT 5 MD-2 (severe) = WRAT < 2 sd MD-1 (mildly) = WRAT ≤ 1 sd | - Verbal STM 6 | - Letters and Digits Forward Span | Verbal STM MD-2 < CG MD-1 = CG |
Geary et al., 1999 [76] | MD = 15 (8 M 7; 7 F 8) 6.92 CG = 35 (13 M; 22 F) 6.75 | WISC 9 III OR Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale | WIAT 10 MD = WIAT < 30° | 1. Verbal STM 2. Verbal WM 11 | 1. Digit Forward Span (WISC III) 2. Digit Backward Span (WISC III) | 1. Verbal STM MD = CG 2. Verbal WM MD < CG |
Keeler & Swanson, 2001 [56] | Study #2 MD = 20 (13 M; 7 F) 11.78 (0.47) CG = 18 (11 M; 7 F) 11.65 (0.34) | Raven Progressive Matrices | WJ 12 (calculation subtest) MD = WJ < 25° (90 standard score) | 1. Verbal WM 2. Visuospatial WM | 1. Digit Sentence Span task 2. Mapping and Directions task | 1. Verbal WM MD < CG 2. Visuospatial WM MD < CG |
Geary et al., 2004 [75] | MD = 58 (32 M; 26 F) CG = 91 (36 M; 55 F) | Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales | WIAT MD = WIAT < 30° | - Verbal WM | - Counting Span | Verbal WM MD < CG |
Censabella & Noël, 2007 [77] | STUDY #1 MD = 20 (10 M; 10 F) 10.48 (0.54) CG = 20 (10 M; 10 F) 10.33 (1.18) STUDY #2 Only Fact retrievals Deficit MD = 12 CG = 12 | WISC-III | Arithmetic Skills: standardized pedagogic task (Simonart,1998) + Arithmetic fact retrieval (ad hoc) MD = ICD-10 13 criteria | - Inhibition | - Listening Span test (suppression of irrelevant information) - Color–Word Stroop test (inhibition of prepotent response) - Flanker task (interference control) | - Suppression of irrelevant information: Intrusion errors: #1 MD = CG #2 CG < MD Span score: #1 MD < CG #2 MD = CG - Inhibition of prepotent response (RTs and Accuracy): #1 and #2 MD = CG - Interference Control (RTs and Accuracy): #1 and #2 MD = CG |
Cirino et al., 2007 [64] | MD = 51 (33 M; 18 F) 9.55 (0.7) CG = 85 (43 M; 42 F) 9.3 (0.7) | WASI 14 | WRAT-3 MD = WRAT-3 ≤ 30° | - Attention | - SWAN 15 4th ed. (inattention scale) | Attention: MD < CG |
Rousselle & Noël, 2007 [53] | MD = 29 (8 M; 21 F) 7.32 (0.25) CG-MD = 29 (16 M; 13 F) 7.58 (0.23) MD/RD 16 = 16 (6 M; 10 F) 7.41 (0.35) CG-MD/RD = 16 (7 M; 9 F) 7.5 (0.25) MD + MD/RD = 45 (14 M; 31 F) CG = 45 (23 M; 22 F) | WISC-III | TEDI-MATH + Transcoding tokens into Arabic numerals. Untimed addition and subtraction test; Timed addition test (ad hoc) MD = Composite Score < 15° + Teacher reports | - Inhibition | - Numerical Stroop task | Inhibition (RTs and Accuracy) MD + MD/RD = CG |
Fuchs et al., 2008 [31] | CG = 415 (201 M; 214 F) MD = 132 (63 M; 69 F) MD- Computational and Problem-Solving (MD-CP) = 64 (32 M; 32 F) MD-Computational (MD-C) = 35 (17 M; 18 F) MD-Problem-solving (MD-P) = 33 (14 M; 19 F) | WASI | WJ-III + Computational skills + Problem-solving MD-CP ≤15° on both computational and problem-solving factors MD- C ≤15° on computational skills MD-P ≤ 15° on problem-solving task | 1. Processing Speed 2. verbal WM 3. Verbal LTM 17 4. Attention | 1. Visual Matching (WJ-III) 2. Listening Recall (WMTB-C 18) 2. Numbers Reversed (WJ-III) 3. Retrieval Fluency (WJ-III) 4. SWAN | 1. Processing Speed (accuracy): MD < CG 2. Listening Recall: MD < CG 2. Numbers Reversed: MD-P = MD-CP = MD-C MD-C = CG MD-P = MD-CP < CG 3. Verbal LTM: MD-P = MD-CP < MD-C = CG 4. Attention: MD < CG |
Raghubar et al., 2009 [65] | #1 MD = 51 (33 M; 18 F) 9.55 (0.7) CG = 85 (43 M; 42 F) 9.30 (0.7) #2 MD = 36 (17 M; 19 F) 9.88 (0.08) CG = 85 (43 M; 42 F) 9.3 (0.7) | WASI | WRAT-3 #1 MD = WRAT-3 < 30° #2 MD = WRAT < 16° LA = WRAT < 30° | - Attention | - SWAN | Attention: #1 MD < CG #2 MD < CG |
Chan & Ho, 2010 [60] | MD = 49 9 (0.08) CG = 76 9 (0.08) | RSPM 19 | Hong Kong Attainment test on Mathematics + Arithmetic subtest Grades 2 or 3: MD ≤ 25° at screening math tasks Grade 4: MD ≤ 20° at screening math tasks | 1. Processing Speed 2. Verbal WM 3. Visual WM | 1. Visual Matching (WJ-III) 1. Digit Rapid Naming (HKT-SpLD 20) 2. Digit Span Backward (WISC) 3. Visual Memory Subtest (TVPS-NM 21) | 1. Processing Speed MD = CG 2. Verbal WM MD < CG 3. Visual WM MD = CG |
Costa et al., 2011 [93] | MD = 14 (6 M; 8 F) 10.18 (1.07) CG = 84 (30 M; 54 F) 10.17 (1.09) | CPM 22 | TDE 23 MD = TDE < 1 sd | 1. Processing speed 2. Verbal STM 3. Verbal WM 4. Visuospatial STM 5. Visuospatial WM | 1. Simple Reaction Time 2. Digit span Forward (WISC III) 3. Digit Span Backward (WISC III) 4. Corsi Block Forward 5. Corsi Block Backward | All cognitive domains: MD = CG |
Passolunghi, 2011 [66] | MD = 18 (7 M; 11 F) 9.6 (5) CG = 18 (10 M; 8 F) 9.7 (5) | PMA 24 | AC-MT 25 MD = AC-MT < 25° + Teacher report | 1. Processing speed 2. Verbal STM 3. Verbal WM | 1. Symbol Search (WAIS 26) 2. Digit span Forward (WISC-R) 2. Word Forward Span task 3. Listening Span task | 1. Processing Speed (Accuracy) MD < CG 2. Verbal STM (Digit and Word) MD = CG 3. Verbal WM Correct Recall MD < CG Intrusion Error MD > CG |
Passolunghi & Mammarella, 2012 [67] | MD = 35 (15 M; 20 F) 9.96 (0.77) CG = 35 (14 M; 21 F) 9.94 (0.73) | PMA | Standardized Mathematics test (Word problem solving; Amoretti et al., 1994) MD = Math test < 25° + teacher report | 1. Spatial WM 2. Visual WM | 1. Simple Matrix task 1. Complex Matrix task 2. Simple House Recognition 2. Complex House Recognition | 1. Spatial WM MD < CG 2. Visual WM MD = CG |
Peng et al., 2012 [57] | MD = 18 (11 M; 7 F) 11 (0.52) CG = 30 (13 M; 17 F) 10.97 (0.36) | RSPM | WRAT 4 (computation) <25° Math Problem-solving (adapted from WISC IV) <15° | 1. Verbal STM 2. Attention 3. Inhibition 4. Updating (Verbal WM) | 1. Word Span Forward 1. Digit Span Forward (WISC-IV) 2. Dual-task (Complex reading and computation span) 3. Stroop Color–Word 3. Number Inhibition 4. 2-Back task (word and Digit) | 1. Verbal STM Word Span: MD = CG Digit Span: MD < CG 2. Dual-Task MD < CG 3. Inhibition Stroop Color–Word: MD = CG Number inhibition: MD < CG 4. Updating MD = CG |
Cai et al., 2013 [44] | N = 111 (48 M; 63 F) 11.97 CG = 56 MD = 55 | RSPM | Standardized mathematics test (Dong, 2011) MD = −2ds at the standardized test + three recent math tests ≤ 20° of the class | 1. Processing speed 2. Planning 3. Attention 4. Successive Processing 5. Simultaneous Processing 6. Phonological Loop 7. Central Executive 8. Visuospatial sketchpad | 1. Reaction time 2. Matching Numbers; Planned Codes; Planed Connection (CAS 27) 3. Expressive attention; number detection; receptive attention (CAS) 4. Word series; sentence repetition; sentence questions (CAS) 5. Non-verbal matrices; verbal spatial relations; figure memory (CAS) 6. Number Span task 6. Sentence Span task 7. Stop-Signal task 7. Flanker task 8. N-Back task 8. Nine-gride graphical spatial location task | 1. Processing Speed MD = CG 2. Planning (all tasks): MD < CG 3: Attention (all tasks): MD < CG 4. Successive processing Sentence questions and repetition: MD < CG Word series: MD = CG 5. Simultaneous Processing (all Tasks): MD < CG 6. Phonological Loop MD < CG 7. Central Executive: MD < CG 8. Visuospatial Sketchpad: MD < CG |
De Weerdt et al., 2013a [68] | MD = 22 (6 M; 16 F) 9.79 (0.75) CG = 45 (19 M; 26 F) 12.07 (0.86) | Short form WISC-III | Clinical diagnosis + TTR 28 + KRT-R 29 (mental arithmetic and number knowledge) MD ≤ 10° on test from clinical diagnosis and at least one standardized math test (TTR and/or KTR-R) | - Inhibition | - Go/No Go task (picture, letter or digit modality) | Inhibition: Commission errors: MD = CG RTs (Go trials) MD = CG |
De Weerdt et al., 2013b [69] | MD = 22 (6 M; 16 F) 9.79 (0.75) CG = 45 (19 M; 26 F) 12.07 (0.86) | Short form WISC-III | TTR + KRT-R (mental arithmetic and number knowledge) MD ≤ 11° on at least one standardized math test (TTR and/or KTR-R) | 1. Verbal STM 2. Verbal WM 3. Visuospatial STM 4. Visuospatial WM | 1. Digit Span Forward 1. Word List Forward 2. Digit Span Backward 2. Word List Backward 2. Listening Span task 3. Corsi Block Recall Forward 4. Corsi Block Recall Backward 4. Spatial Span | 1. Verbal STM Digit Span: MD < CG Word Span: MD = CG 2. Verbal WM (All tasks): MD < CG 3. Visuospatial STM: MD = CG 4. Visuospatial WM: Block recall: MD = CG Spatial Span: MD < CG |
Moura et al., 2013 [70] | MD = 28 (10 M; 18 F) 9.6(1.1)y.o CG = 81 (36 M; 45 F) 9.4 (1.3) | CPM | TDE MD = TDE < 25° | 1. Verbal WM 2. Visuospatial WM | 1. Digit Span Backward (WISC-III) 2. Corsi Block Backward | 1. Verbal WM MD < CG 2. Visuospatial WM MD = CG |
Reimann et al., 2013 [80] | MD = 48 (16 M; 33 F) 8.5 (1.6) CG = 783 (399 M; 384 F) 7.11(1.8) | IDS 30 | IDS MD IDS < 15° | 1. Attention 2. Verbal STM 3. Verbal LTM 4. Visuospatial WM | 1. Cross-Out (IDS) 2. Digit and Letter Span Forward (IDS) 3. Story Recall (IDS) 4. Shape Recognition (IDS) | 1. Attention MD < CG 2. Verbal STM MD = CG 3. Verbal LTM MD = CG 4. Visuospatial WM MD = CG |
Szucs et al., 2013 [71] | DD 31 = 12 (8 M; 4 F) 9.2 y.o CG = 12 (5 M; 7 F) 9.1 y.o | CPM + WISC-III- short form | MaLT 32 + WIAT-II (numerical Operations subtest) DD ≤ 1 ds (<16°) at both math ability tests | 1. Switching 2. Inhibition 3. Verbal STM 4. Verbal WM 5. Visuospatial STM 6. Visuospatial WM | 1. Trial Making test 2. Animal Stroop; Numerical Magnitude Comparison Stroop task; Physical Size Comparison Stroop task; Stop-Signal task 3. Digit Span Forward (AWMA 33) 3. Word Recall (AWMA) 4. Listening Span test (AWMA) 5. Dot Matrix (AWMA) 6. Odd One Out (AWMA) | 1. Switching: accuracy: MD = CG RTs: MD < CG 2. Inhibition 3. Verbal STM MD = CG 4. Verbal WM MD = CG 5. Visuospatial STM MD < CG 6. Visuospatial WM MD < CG |
Willcutt et al., 2013 [58] | MD = 183 (82 M; 101 F) 11.4 (2.4) CG = 419 (202 M; 217 F) 11.1 (2.2) | WISC-R | PIAT 34 and WRAT subtest MD = −1.25 sd at the composite measure of math calculation | 1. Processing speed 2. Phonological Awareness 3. Attention 4. Inhibition 5. Shifting 6. Verbal WM | 1. Symbol Search (WISC-III); Coding (WISC-R); Colorado Perceptual Speed test; Identical Pictures (Educational Testing Service) 1. RAN (objects, numbers, letters and colors) 2. Phoneme Deletion task; Pig Latin task 3. Gordon Diagnostic System 4. Stop-Signal task; Gordon Diagnostic system; Stroop Color–Word test 5. WCST 35 6. Sentence Span task; Counting Span task | 1. Processing Speed (all tasks): MD < CG 2. Phonological Awareness: MD < CG 3. Attention (omission) MD < CG 4. Inhibition (all tasks) MD < CG 5. Shifting: MD < CG 6. Verbal WM: MD < CG |
Attout & Majerus, 2015 [95] | DD 36 = 16 (6 M; 10 F) 10.04 (1.38) CG = 16 (6 M; 10 F) 9.47 (1.19) | CPM + EVIP 37 | Clinical diagnosis of DD + TTR = more than 1 year of delay + NUMERICAL + TEDI-MATH + ZAREKI-R = −2 sd | - Verbal WM | - Item Working Memory task -Order Working Memory task | Verbal WM - Item Working Memory task: MD = CG - Order Working Memory Task: MD < CG |
Cirino et al., 2015 [54] | N#1 = MD = 105 (46 M; 59 F) 7.74 (0.48) CG = 403 (186 M; 217 F) 7.48 (0.32) LowMD = 56 CG = 530 * age as cov | WASI | WRAT-3 #1 MD = WRAT-3 < 25° #2 LowMD = WRAT-3 < 10° | 1. Processing speed 2. Phonological Awareness 3. Verbal WM | 1. Cross-Out (WJ III) 1. RAN 38 (letters and digits, CTOPP 39) 2. Elision (CTOPP) 3. Listening Span task; Digit Span Backward; Counting Recall (WMBT-C) | 1. Processing Speed (Cross-Out and RAN) MD < CG 2. 3. Verbal WM: MD < CG #1 Average of all cognitive variables: MD < CG |
Kroesbergen & Van Dijk, 2015 [78] | N = 154 (70 M; 84 F) 8.65 (0.87) CG = 94 MD = 26 | If IQ not available, measured with four subtest of the WISC-III | Cito Math test MD = Cito Math test < 15° | - Visuospatial WM | - Dot Matrix; Odd One Out; Spatial Span (AWMA) | Visuospatial WM: MD < CG |
Donker et al., 2016 [47] | MD = 31 (7 M; 24 F) 8.95 (1.02) CG = 34 (12 M; 22 F) 8.57 (.75) * groups differed on age | WISC-III Full-version for children assessed in the past two years (37/133) Short version (96/133) | TTR + Math standardized test—problem solving MD = TTR < 1sd + Math standardized test < 25° | - Processing speed | - RAN Alphanumeric (Digits and letters) - RAN Non-Alphanumeric (colors and pictures) | Processing Speed: Alphanumeric: MD = CG Non-alphanumeric: MD < CG |
Kuhn et al., 2016 [79] | DD = 33 (6 M; 27 F) 8.78 (.96) CG = 40 (17 M; 23 F) 8.95 (0.63) | WISC-IV | ZAREKI-R 40 DD = ZAREKI-R ≤ 80 (raw score) | 1. Processing speed 2. Attention 3. Flexibility 4. Verbal WM 5. Visual WM | 1. Choice Reaction Time task 2. FBB-ADHS (inattention Scale) 2. Alertness; Sustained Attention (KITAP) 3. Flexibility (KITAP) 4. Verbal Span task 5. Visual Matrix task | 1. Processing Speed: DD = CG 2. Attention -FBB-ADHS: MD = CG -Alertness: MD = CG -Sustained Attention (errors): DD > CG (omission): DD = CG 3. Flexibility (accuracy and RTs) DD = CG 4. Verbal WM: DD < CG 5. Visual WM: DD < CG |
Slot et al., 2016 [61] | MD = 26 8.89 (0.99) CG = 32 8.61 (0.73) | WISC- III | TTR + Cito Math test MD = TTR < 1 sd + Cito Math test ≤ 25° | 1. Processing Speed 2. Phonological Awareness 3. Verbal STM 4. Visuospatial WM | 1. RAN (letters, digits, pictures and colors) 2. Phoneme Deletion; Phoneme Manipulation (FAT 41) 3. Digit Span Forward; Word Recall (AWMA) 4. Dot Matrix; Spatial Span; Odd One Out (AWMA) | 1. Processing Speed: MD = CG 2. Phonological Awareness: MD = CG 3. Verbal STM: MD = CG 4. Visuospatial WM: MD = CG |
Lafay & St-Pierre, 2017 [59] | DD = 24 (4 M; 20 F) 9.02(0.36) CG = 37 (16 M; 21 F) 8.9 (0.32) * Groups differ for gender and SES | CPM (Raw score) | ZAREKI-R DD = ZAREKI-R= −1.5 ds | 1. Processing speed 2. Verbal STM 3. Verbal WM 4. Visuospatial STM 5. Visuospatial WM | 1. Coding (WISC-IV) 2. Digit Span Forward (ZAREKI-R) 3. Digit Span Backward (ZAREKI-R) 4. Corsi Block Forward 5. Corsi Block backward | 1. Processing Speed (accuracy) DD < CG 2. Verbal STM DD = CG 3. Verbal WM DD < CG 4. Visuospatial STM DD = CG 5. Visuospatial WM DD = CG |
Lambert & Spinath, 2018 [72] | #1 N = 229 (119 M; 110 F) 9.18 (1.09) MD = 43 CG = 186 #2 N = 120 (50 M; 70 F) 9.16 (0.72) MD = 27 CG = 60 | 2nd grade CFT 1 3rd–4th grade CFT 20-R + CPM * IQ as cov | DEMAT 1 MD ≤ 25° on DEMAT 1 | - Processing Speed | - Numerosity Processing Speed | Processing Speed: MD < CG |
Mammarella et al., 2018 [73] | MD = 24 (14 M; 10 F) 9.78 (0.61) CG = 24 (10 M; 14 F) 9.8 (0.58) | WISC-IV (Block Design and Vocabulary subtest) | AC-MT + AC-FL 42 (Math Fluency) MD ≤ 10° in at least two mathematical tasks ≤16° at the total mathematical score LA ≤ 20° in at least two mathematical tasks ≤30° at the total mathematical score | - Visuospatial WM | - Visual Working Memory task (houses and balloons) - Spatial-Simultaneous Matrices task (grid, no grid) - Spatial-Sequential Matrices task (grid, no grid) | Visuospatial WM (al tasks): MD < CG |
McDonald & Berg, 2018 [74] | MD = 20 (7 M; 13 F) 9.7 (0.56) CG = 20 (11 M; 9 F) 9.8 (0.56) CG-Y = 20 (11 M; 9 F) 7.83 (0.63) | CPM | WIAT- II MD = WIAT-II < 25° | 1. Inhibition 2. Shifting 3. Verbal WM 4. Visuospatial WM | 1. Quantity–Digits Inhibition task. 1. Color–Word Stroop task 2. Making Trials task (letters and digits) 3. Auditory Digit Sequence; Semantic Categorization (S-CPT 43) 4. Visual Matrix task; Mapping and Direction task (S-CPT) | 1. Inhibition -Quantity–Digits: MD < CG -Color–Word: MD = CG 2. Shifting (all task) MD < CG 3. Verbal WM (al tasks) MD = CG 4. Visuospatial WM MD < CG |
Author, Year of Publication | Sample Groups (N 1, Gender, Mean Age) | Intelligence Assessment | Screening Measures For Mathematics (Test) and Criteria for MD 2 Group | Cognitive Domain | Task | Follow-Up | Outcome |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Geary et al., 2000 [84] | T1 MD = 12 (7 M 3; 5 F 4) 6.92 CG 5 = 26 (12 M; 14 F) 6.75 | T2 WISC 6- III (vocabulary and Matrix reasoning subtests) or Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale. * IQ as Cov | T2 and T4 WIAT 7 | T1 and T3 1. Verbal STM 8 2. Verbal WM 9 | 1. Digit Span Forward (WISC III) 2. Digit Span Backward (WISC III) | T1-1st grade Fall T2-1st grade Spring T3-2nd grade Fall T4-2nd grade Spring | 1. Verbal STM: MD = CG 2. Verbal WM MD = CG |
Passolunghi & Siegel, 2004 [91] | T1 MD = 22 (14 M; 13 F) 10.4 CG = 27 (11 M; 11 F) 10.4 | T1 PMA 10 | T1 and T2 Standard math test (Amoretti et al., 1994) T2 WRAT 11-3 (arithmetic subtest) | T2 1. Verbal STM 2. Simple Verbal WM 3. Complex WM | 1. Digit/Word Span Forward 2. Digit/Word Span Backward 3. Listening Span task; Listening Completion task; Counting Span task | T1-4th grade T2-5th grade | 1. Verbal STM MD = CG 2. Simple Verbal WM - Word: MD = CG - Digit: MD < CG 3. Complex WM - Listening Span and Completion task (correct recall; order): MD < CG (intrusion error): MD > CG - Counting Span task (correct Recall; order): MD < CG (intrusion): MD > CG |
Geary et al., 2007 [88] | MD = 15 T1 = 6.08 ± 0.34 CG = 46 (29 M; 17 F) T1 = 6.34 ± 0.34 | T1-CPM 12 T2-WASI 13 * IQ as cov | T1 and T2 WIAT-II-A | T2 1. Central Executive 2. Visuospatial Sketchpad 3. Phonological Loop T3 4. Processing Speed | 1. Listening Recall; Counting Recall; Digit Recall Backward (WMTB-C 14) 2. Block Recall; Mazes Memory task (WMTB-C) 3. Digit Recall; Word List Recall; Non-Word List Recall; Word List Matching task (WMTB-C) 4. RAN 15 (digit and letters) | T1-K 16 T2-1st grade Fall T3-1st grade Spring | 1. Central Executive: MD < CG 2. Visuospatial Sketchpad: MD < CG 3. Phonological Loop: MD < CG 4. Processing Speed (composite score) MD < CG |
Mazzocco & Kover, 2007 [86] | T1 N = 178 (85 M; 93) M = 6.83 (0.30) F = 6.71 (0.30) MD = 10 CG = 168 | T2 WASI | T1; T2 and T3 TEMA 17 + WJ-R 18 MD = TEMA < 10° in all assessment | T1; T2 and T3 - Executive Functions | CNT 19 1. Baseline task: naming items by color or shape 2. One attribute switching task (alternating between naming color or shape based on one attribute) 3.Two-attribute switching task (alternating between naming color or shape based on two attribute) | T1-1st grade T2-3rd grade T3-5th grade | 1. Baseline task Self-corrections MD > CG Efficiency (speed–accuracy trade-off): MD < CG 2. One attribute task MD = CG 3. Two-attributes task MD group omitted from analysis (1/10 child completed the task on the 1st assessment) |
Murphy et al., 2007 [38] | T1 MD-10° = 22 (15 M; 7 F) 5.77 (0.49) MD-25° = 42 (16 M; 26 F) 5.86 (0.32) CG = 146 (72 M; 74 F) 5.77 (0.32) | T4 WASI | From T1 to T4: TEMA-2 | All grades 1. Processing Speed T2 and T4 2. WM T4 3. Reactive flexibility | 1. RAN (digit and colors) 2. One attribute (CNT) 3. Two attributes (CNT) | T1 = K T2 = 1st grade T3 = 2nd grade T4 = 3rd grade | 1. Processing Speed —(Digit) From 1st to 3rd grade MD10° < CG 1st and 2nd grade MD25° < CG 3rd grade MD25° = CG - (Colors) K to 2nd grade MD10° < CG 3rd grade MD10° = CG K and 1st grade MD25°< CG 2nd and 3rd grade MD25° = CG 2. WM 1st grade Efficiency: MD10° < CG 3rd grade MD10° < CG MD25° < CG 3. Flexibility: 4th grade MD10° < CG MD25°< CG |
Geary et al., 2008 [87] | T1 MD = 19 (9 M; 10 F) 7.83 ±. 25 CG = 50 (24; 26 F) 7.25 ± 0.34 | K CPM T1 WISC | T1 and T2 WIAT-II- A | T1 and T2 1. Processing Speed T1 2. Central Executive 3. Phonological Loop; 4. Visuospatial Sketchpad) | 1. RAN (digit and letters) 2. Listening Recall; Counting Recall; Digit Recall Backward (WMTB-C) 3. Digit Recall; Word List Recall; Non-Word List Recall; Word List Matching task (WMTB-C) 4. Block Recall; Mazes Memory task (WMTB-C) | T0-K T1-1st grade T2-2nd grade | 1. Processing speed: MD < CG 2. Central Executive: MD < CG 3. Phonological Loop: MD < CG 4. Visuospatial Sketchpad: MD = CG |
Geary et al., 2012a [89] | T1 = 7 MD = 16 (6 M; 10 F) CG = 132 (68 M; 64 F) | K -CPM T1 -WASI | T2 to T6 -Addition strategy choices; -Number sets; -Number line estimation | T2 to T6 1. Processing Speed T2 and T6 2. Central Executive 3. Phonological Loop 4. Visuospatial Sketchpad 5. Attention | 1. RAN (digit and letters) 2. Listening Recall; Counting Recall; Digit Recall Backward (WMTB-C) 3. Digit Recall; Word List Recall; Non-Word List Recall; Word List Matching task (WMTB-C) 4. Block Recall; Mazes Memory task (WMTB-C) 5. SWAN | T1-K T2-1st grade T3-2nd grade T4-3rd grade T5-4th grade T6-5th grade | 1. Processing speed MD < CG 2. Central Executive MD < CG 3. Phonological Loop: MD < CG 4. Visual Sketchpad: MD < CG 5. Attention MD < CG |
Geary et al., 2012b [90] | MD = 15 (10 M; 5 F) CG = 101 (52 M 49 F) | T1 - CPM T2 - WASI * IQ as cov | From T1 to T4 WIAT-II-A From T3 to T5 - Fact Retrieval (Choice task & Forced Retrieval task) | From T2 to T5 1. Processing Speed T2 2. Central Executive 3. Phonological Loop 4. Visuospatial Sketchpad | 1. RAN (digit and letter) 2. Listening Recall; Counting Recall; Digit Recall Backward (WMTB-C) 3. Digit Recall; Word List Recall; Non-Word List Recall; Word List Matching task (WMTB-C) 4. Block Recall; Mazes Memory task (WMTB-C) | T1-K T2-1st grade T3-2nd grade T4-3rd grade T5-4th grade | 1. Processing Speed: MD < CG 2. Central Executive: MD < CG 3. Phonological Loop: MD < CG 4. Visuospatial Sketchpad: MD < CG |
Mazzocco & Grimm, 2013 [92] | T1 = 249 (120 M; 129 F) T4 = 213 T9 = 161 | IQ > 80 | All grades TEMA-2 + WJ-R MD = WJ-R < 10° | All grades - Processing speed | - RAN Alphanumeric (letter and digit) - RAN Non-Alphanumeric (colors) | T1-K T2-1st grade T3-2nd grade T4-3rd grade T5-4th grade T6-5th grade T7-6th grade T8-7th grade T9-8th grade | Processing Speed: K (letters, digits, colors) MD < CG 8th grade: (letters and colors): MD < CG (Digits) MD = CG |
Cowan & Powell, 2014 [81] | T2 N = 199 (99 M; 100 F) 8.92 ± 0.42 MD = 11 (2 M; 7 F) CG = 166 (91 M; 75 F) | T2 CPM * MD < CG | T2 -WIAT-II T1 and T2 (different task, same domain) - Numerical factors -Arithmetic Skills | T1 and T2 1. Processing Speed (visual: T1; Verbal T2) T1 2. Central Executive 3. Phonological Loop 4. Visuospatial Sketchpad | 1. Symbol Matching (WISC) 1. Pair Cancellation (WJ III) 1. RAN (letters) 2. Listening Recall (WMTB-C) 3. Word List Recall (WMTB-C) 4. Block Recall; Mazes Memory task (WMTB-C) | T1-1st grade T2-3rd grade | 1. Processing Speed (all task): MD < CG 2. Central Executive: MD < CG 3. Phonological Loop: MD < CG 4. Visuospatial Sketchpad: MD = CG |
Wong & Chan, 2019 [82] | MD = 79 (45 M; 34 F) 7 CG = 466 | T1 CPM * IQ as cov | All grades LAMK-3.0 | All grades 1. Verbal WM 2. Visuospatial WM | 1. Digit Span Backward 2. Corsi Block Backward | T1 = 1st grade T2 = 2nd grade | 1. Verbal WM: MD < CG 2. Visuospatial WM MD = CG |
Chu et al., 2019 [62] | MD = 14 Recovered MD = 23 CG = 35 | T3 WPPSI 20-III * MD < CG | T1 and T2 TEMA T3 WIAT-II (Numerical Operations) | T1 and T2 - Conflict Executive Function scale (Beck et al., 2011) | Inhibition and shifting | T1-K 1st year T2-K 2nd year T3-1st grade | Inhibition and shifting: MD < CG (MD recovered = CG) |
Chan & Wong, 2020 [83] | T5 N = 101 (56 M; 45 F) 12.08 (±0.33) MD = 14 (7 M; 7 F) 12.17 (±0.33) CG = 87 | T2-RSPM (short form, series A–C) | T3, T4 and T5 LAMK | T1 -Verbal WM | - Digit Span Backward | T1 = K T2 = 1st grade T3 = 2nd grade T4 = 2nd grade T5 = 6th grade | Verbal WM MD = CG |
Zhang et al., 2020 [63] | N = 1471 MD = 6.14 LA = 6.12 CG = 6.17 |
T6- CPM (short form) | T3 and T7 BAT 21 | T1 and T2 1. Phonological awareness T2 2. Processing speed | 1. Initial Phoneme Identification 2. RAN (objects) | T1 = K fall T2 = K spring T3 = 1st grade fall T4 = 1st grade spring T5 = 2nd grade spring T6 = 3rd grade spring T7 = 4th | 1. Phonological Awareness: MD = CG 2. Processing Speed: MD < CG |
Träff et al., 2020 [85] | T4 MD = 27 (14 M; 13 F) CG = 81 (51 M; 20 F) 12.95 (±0.27) | T1 to T4 Matrix Reasoning (WISC-IV) | T5 Standardized mathematics test (Swedish National Agency for Education) + overall math accomplishment MD = grade E on both criteria | T1 to T4 1. Verbal WM 2. Processing Speed | 1. Word Sequence with Interference 2. RAN (Time Color) | T1 = K T2 = 1st grade T3 = 2nd grade T4 = 3rd grade T5 = 6th grade | 1. Verbal WM MD = CG 2. Processing Speed MD = CG |
References
- McCloskey, M. Quantitative Literacy and Developmental Dyscalculias. In Why is Math so Hard for Some Children? The Nature and Origins of Mathematical Learning Difficulties and Disabilities; Berch, D.B., Mazzocco, M.M.M., Eds.; Paul H. Brookes Publishing: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2007; pp. 415–429. [Google Scholar]
- Reyna, V.F.; Brainerd, C.J. The importance of mathematics in health and human judgment: Numeracy, risk communication, and medical decision making. Learn. Ind. Diff. 2007, 17, 147–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ojose, B. Mathematics literacy: Are we able to put the mathematics we learn into everyday use. J. Math. Educ. 2011, 4, 89–100. [Google Scholar]
- Jansen, B.R.; Schmitz, E.A.; van der Maas, H.L. Affective and motivational factors mediate the relation between math skills and use of math in everyday life. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Parsons, S.; Bynner, J. Numeracy and Employment. Educ. Train. 1997, 39, 43–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dowker, A. Early identification and intervention for students with mathematics difficulties. J. Learn. Disabil. 2007, 38, 324–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Geary, D.C. Consequences, characteristics, and causes of mathematical learning disabilities and persistent low achievement in mathematics. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2011, 32, 250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kosc, L. Developmental dyscalculia. J. Learn. Disabil. 1974, 7, 164–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, C.; Hitch, G.J.; Walker, P. The prevalence of specific arithmetic difficulties and specific reading difficulties in 9-to 10-year-old boys and girls. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1994, 35, 283–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shalev, R.S.; Manor, O.; Gross-Tsur, V. Developmental dyscalculia: A prospective six-year follow-up. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2005, 47, 121–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Swanson, H.L.; Jerman, O.; Zheng, X. Math disabilities and reading disabilities: Can they be separated? J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2009, 27, 175–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dehaene, S. Précis of the number sense. Mind Lang. 2001, 16, 16–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aunola, K.; Leskinen, E.; Lerkkanen, M.K.; Nurmi, J.E. Developmental dynamics of math performance from preschool to grade 2. J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 96, 699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krajewski, K.; Schneider, W. Early development of quantity to number-word linkage as a precursor of mathematical school achievement and mathematical difficulties: Findings from a four-year longitudinal study. Learn. Instr. 2009, 19, 513–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simms, V.; Gilmore, C.; Cragg, L.; Marlow, N.; Wolke, D.; Johnson, S. Mathematics difficulties in extremely preterm children: Evidence of a specific deficit in basic mathematics processing. Pediatr. Res. 2013, 73, 236–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Smedt, B.; Noël, M.P.; Gilmore, C.; Ansari, D. The relationship between symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills and the typical and atypical development of mathematics: A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2013, 2, 48–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Piazza, M. Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 542–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mazzocco, M.M.M.; Feigenson, L.; Halberda, J. Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (Dyscalculia): Impaired numerical acuity contributes to MLD. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 1224–1237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Butterworth, B. A head for figures. Science 1999, 284, 928–929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Butterworth, B. Developmental dyscalculia. Child Neuropsychol. 2008, 357–374. [Google Scholar]
- Geary, D.C. Mathematics and learning disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 2004, 37, 4–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hassinger-Das, B.; Jordan, N.C.; Glutting, J.; Irwin, C.; Dyson, N. Domain-general mediators of the relation between kindergarten number sense and first-grade mathematics achievement. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 118, 78–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Purpura, D.J.; Daya, E.; Napolia, A.R.; Hartbc, S.A. Identifying Domain-General and Domain-Specific Predictors of Low Mathematics Performance: A Classification and Regression Tree Analysis. J. Numer. Cogn. 2017, 3, 365–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Passolunghi, M.C.; Lanfranchi, S. Domain-specific and domain-general precursors of mathematical achievement: A longitudinal study from kindergarten to first grade. Brit. J. Educ. Psychol. 2012, 82, 42–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Andersson, U.; Östergren, R. Number magnitude processing and basic cognitive functions in children with mathematical learning disabilities. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 2012, 22, 701–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vanbinst, K.; Ghesquiere, P.; De Smedt, B. Arithmetic strategy development and its domain-specific and domain-general cognitive correlates: A longitudinal study in children with persistent mathematical learning difficulties. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014, 35, 3001–3013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Case, R.; Kurland, D.M.; Goldberg, J. Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1982, 33, 386–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, D.C. Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychol. Bull. 2003, 114, 345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, P.; Wang, C.; Namkung, J. Understanding the cognition related to mathematics difficulties: A meta-analysis on the cognitive deficit profiles and the bottleneck theory. Educ. Res. Rev. 2018, 88, 434–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bull, R.; Johnston, R.S. Children’s arithmetical difficulties: Contributions from processing speed, item identification, and short-term memory. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1997, 65, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fuchs, L.S.; Fuchs, D.; Stuebing, K.; Fletcher, J.M.; Hamlett, C.L.; Lambert, W. Problem solving and computational skill: Are they shared or distinct aspects of mathematical cognition? J. Educ. Psychol. 2008, 100, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dehaene, S.; Dehaene-Lambertz, G.; Cohen, L. Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain. Trends Neurosci. 1998, 21, 355–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCloskey, M. Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia. Cognition 1992, 44, 107–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Andersson, U. Mathematical competencies in children with different types of learning difficulties. J. Educ. Psychol. 2008, 100, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bull, R.; Scerif, G. Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2001, 19, 273–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, C.A.; Pritchard, V.E.; Woodward, L.J. Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, M.M.; Mazzocco, M.M.; Hanich, L.B.; Early, M.C. Cognitive characteristics of children with mathematics learning disability (MLD) vary as a function of the cutoff criterion used to define MLD. J. Learn. Disabil. 2007, 40, 458–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fletcher, J.M.; Francis, D.J.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Lyon, G.R.; Foorman, B.R.; Stuebing, K.K.; Shaywitz, B.A. Intelligent testing and the discrepancy model for children with learning disabilities. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 1998, 14, 186–203. [Google Scholar]
- Sörqvist, P.; Stenfelt, S.; Rönnberg, J. Working memory capacity and visual–verbal cognitive load modulate auditory–sensory gating in the brainstem: Toward a unified view of attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012, 24, 2147–2154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sörqvist, P. On interpretation and task selection in studies on the effects of noise on cognitive performance. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1249. [Google Scholar]
- Neath, I.; Surprenant, A.M. Set size and long-term memory/lexical effects in immediate serial recall: Testing the impurity principle. Mem. Cognit. 2019, 47, 455–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cai, D.; Li, Q.W.; Deng, C.P. Cognitive processing characteristics of 6th to 8th grade Chinese students with mathematics learning disability: Relationships among working memory, PASS processes, and processing speed. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2013, 27, 120–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacLeod, C.M. The Stroop task: The “gold standard” of attentional measures. J. Exp. Psychol. 1992, 121, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, M.; Bowers, P.G. The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. J. Educ. Psychol. 1999, 91, 415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donker, M.; Kroesbergen, E.; Slot, E.; Van Viersen, S.; De Bree, E. Alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric Rapid Automatized Naming in children with reading and/or spelling difficulties and mathematical difficulties. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2016, 47, 80–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, M.; Bowers, P.G.; Biddle, K. Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. J. Learn. Disabil. 2000, 33, 387–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shanahan, M.A.; Pennington, B.F.; Yerys, B.E.; Scott, A.; Boada, R.; Willcutt, E.G.; De Fries, J.C. Processing speed deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2006, 34, 584–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liberati, A.; Altman, D.G.; Tetzlaff, J.; Mulrow, C.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Ioannidis, J.P.; Moher, D. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2009, 62, e1–e34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; Prisma Group. Reprint—Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Phys. Ther. 2009, 89, 873–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, J.P.; Altman, D.G.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Jüni, P.; Moher, D.; Oxman, A.D.; Sterne, J.A. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. Br. Med. J. 2011, 343, d5928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rousselle, L.; Noël, M.P. Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs. non-symbolic number magnitude processing. Cognition 2007, 102, 361–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cirino, P.T.; Fuchs, L.S.; Elias, J.T.; Powell, S.R.; Schumacher, R.F. Cognitive and mathematical profiles for different forms of learning difficulties. J. Learn. Disabil. 2015, 48, 156–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Webster, R.E. Short-Term Memory in Mathematics-Proficient and Math Ematics-Disabled Students as a Function of Input-Modality/Output-Modality Pairings. J. Spec. Educ. 1980, 14, 67–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keeler, M.L.; Swanson, H.L. Does strategy knowledge influence working memory in children with mathematical disabilities? J. Learn. Disabil. 2001, 34, 418–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, P.; Congying, S.; Beilei, L.; Sha, T. Phonological storage and executive function deficits in children with mathematics difficulties. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2012, 112, 452–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willcutt, E.G.; Petrill, S.A.; Wu, S.; Boada, R.; DeFries, J.C.; Olson, R.K.; Pennington, B.F. Comorbidity between reading disability and math disability: Concurrent psychopathology, functional impairment, and neuropsychological functioning. J. Learn. Disabil. 2013, 46, 500–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lafay, A.; St-Pierre, M.C.; Macoir, J. The Mental Number Line in Dyscalculia: Impaired Number Sense or Access from Symbolic Numbers? J. Learn. Disabil. 2017, 50, 672–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chan, B.M.Y.; Ho, C.S.H. The cognitive profile of Chinese children with mathematics difficulties. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 107, 260–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Slot, E.M.; van Viersen, S.; de Bree, E.H.; Kroesbergen, E.H. Shared and unique risk factors underlying mathematical disability and reading and spelling disability. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chu, F.W.; Hoard, M.K.; Nugent, L.; Scofield, J.E.; Geary, D.C. Preschool deficits in cardinal knowledge and executive function contribute to longer-term mathematical learning disability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 188, 104668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, X.; Räsänen, P.; Koponen, T.; Aunola, K.; Lerkkanen, M.K.; Nurmi, J.E. Early cognitive precursors of children’s mathematics learning disability and persistent low achievement: A 5-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2020, 91, 7–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cirino, P.T.; Fletcher, J.M.; Ewing-Cobbs, L.; Barnes, M.A.; Fuchs, L.S. Cognitive arithmetic differences in learning difficulty groups and the role of behavioral inattention. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2007, 22, 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raghubar, K.; Cirino, P.; Barnes, M.; Ewing-Cobbs, L.; Fletcher, J.; Fuchs, L. Errors in multi-digit arithmetic and behavioral inattention in children with math difficulties. J. Learn. Disabil. 2009, 42, 356–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Passolunghi, M.C. Cognitive and emotional factors in children with mathematical learning disabilities. Int. J. Disabil. Dev. Educ. 2011, 58, 61–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Passolunghi, M.C.; Mammarella, I.C. Selective spatial working memory impairment in a group of children with mathematics learning disabilities and poor problem-solving skills. J. Learn. Disabil. 2012, 45, 341–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- De Weerdt, F.; Desoete, A.; Roeyers, H. Behavioral inhibition in children with learning disabilities. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2013, 34, 1998–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- De Weerdt, F.; Desoete, A.; Roeyers, H. Working memory in children with reading disabilities and/or mathematical disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 2013, 46, 461–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Moura, R.; Wood, G.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Lonnemann, J.; Krinzinger, H.; Willmes, K.; Haase, V.G. Transcoding abilities in typical and atypical mathematics achievers: The role of working memory and procedural and lexical competencies. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 116, 707–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szucs, D.; Devine, A.; Soltesz, F.; Nobes, A.; Gabriel, F. Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex 2013, 49, 2674–2688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lambert, K.; Spinath, B. Conservation abilities, visuospatial skills, and numerosity processing speed: Association with math achievement and math difficulties in elementary school children. J. Learn. Disabil. 2018, 51, 223–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mammarella, I.C.; Caviola, S.; Giofrè, D.; Szűcs, D. The underlying structure of visuospatial working memory in children with mathematical learning disability. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 36, 220–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDonald, P.A.; Berg, D.H. Identifying the nature of impairments in executive functioning and working memory of children with severe difficulties in arithmetic. Child Neuropsychol. 2018, 24, 1047–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, D.C.; Hoard, M.K.; Byrd-Craven, J.; DeSoto, M.C. Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2004, 88, 121–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, D.C.; Hoard, M.K.; Hamson, C.O. Numerical and arithmetical cognition: Patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1999, 74, 213–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Censabella, S.; Noël, M.P. The inhibition capacities of children with mathematical disabilities. Child Neuropsychol. 2007, 14, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroesbergen, E.H.; van Dijk, M. Working Memory and Number Sense as Predictors of Mathematical (Dis-) Ability. Z. Psychol. 2015, 223, 102–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, J.T.; Ise, E.; Raddatz, J.; Schwenk, C.; Dobel, C. Basic Numerical Processing, Calculation, and Working Memory in Children with Dyscalculia and/or ADHD Symptoms. Z. Kinder Jugendpsychiatr. Psychother. 2016, 44, 365–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Reimann, G.; Gut, J.; Frischknecht, M.C.; Grob, A. Memory abilities in children with mathematical difficulties: Comorbid language difficulties matter. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2013, 23, 108–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, R.; Powell, D. The contributions of domain-general and numerical factors to third-grade arithmetic skills and mathematical learning disability. J. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 106, 214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wong, T.T.Y.; Chan, W.W.L. Identifying children with persistent low math achievement: The role of number-magnitude mapping and symbolic numerical processing. Learn. Instr. 2019, 60, 29–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chan, W.W.L.; Wong, T.T.Y. Subtypes of mathematical difficulties and their stability. J. Educ. Psychol. 2020, 112, 649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geary, D.C.; Hamson, C.O.; Hoard, M.K. Numerical and arithmetical cognition: A longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with learning disability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2000, 77, 236–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Träff, U.; Olsson, L.; Östergren, R.; Skagerlund, K. Development of early domain-specific and domain-general cognitive precursors of high and low math achievers in grade 6. Child Neuropsychol. 2020, 26, 1065–1090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Mazzocco, M.M.; Kover, S.T. A longitudinal assessment of executive function skills and their association with math performance. Child Neuropsychol. 2007, 13, 18–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Geary, D.C.; Hoard, M.K.; Nugent, L.; Byrd-Craven, J. Development of number line representations in children with mathematical learning disability. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2008, 33, 277–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geary, D.C.; Hoard, M.K.; Byrd-Craven, J.; Nugent, L.; Numtee, C. Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Devel. 2007, 78, 1343–1359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geary, D.C.; Hoard, M.K.; Nugent, L.; Bailey, D.H. Mathematical cognition deficits in children with learning disabilities and persistent low achievement: A five-year prospective study. J. Educ. Psychol. 2012, 104, 206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geary, D.C.; Hoard, M.K.; Bailey, D.H. Fact retrieval deficits in low achieving children and children with mathematical learning disability. J. Learn. Disabil. 2012, 45, 291–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Passolunghi, M.C.; Siegel, L.S. Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2004, 88, 348–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mazzocco, M.M.; Grimm, K.J. Growth in rapid automatized naming from grades K to 8 in children with math or reading disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 2013, 46, 517–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Costa, A.J.; Silva, J.B.L.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Krinzinger, H.; Lonnemann, J.; Willmes, K.; Haase, V.G. A hand full of numbers: A role for offloading in arithmetics learning? Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 368. [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse, T.A. Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol. Psychol. 2000, 54, 35–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Attout, L.; Majerus, S. Working memory deficits in developmental dyscalculia: The importance of serial order. Child Neuropsychol. 2015, 21, 432–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. Working memory. Science 1992, 255, 556–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaufmann, L.; von Aster, M. The diagnosis and management of dyscalculia. Dtsch. Ärztebl. Int. 2012, 109, 767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kucian, K.; von Aster, M. Developmental dyscalculia. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2015, 174, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kulp, M.T.; Earley, M.J.; Mitchell, G.L.; Timmerman, L.M.; Frasco, C.S.; Geiger, M.E. Are visual perceptual skills related to mathematics ability in second through sixth grade children? Focus Learn. Probl. Math. 2004, 26, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sortor, J.M.; Kulp, M.T. Are the results of the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration and its subtests related to achievement test scores? Optom. Vis. Sci. 2003, 80, 758–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pieters, S.; Desoete, A.; Roeyers, H.; Vanderswalmen, R.; Van Waelvelde, H. Behind mathematical learning disabilities: What about visual perception and motor skills? Learn. Individ. Differ. 2012, 22, 498–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haist, F.; Wazny, J.H.; Toomarian, E.; Adamo, M. Development of brain systems for nonsymbolic numerosity and the relationship to formal math academic achievement. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015, 36, 804–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Swanson, H.L.; Jerman, O. Math disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Rev. Educ. Res. 2006, 76, 249–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szűcs, D. Subtypes and comorbidity in mathematical learning disabilities: Multidimensional study of verbal and visual memory processes is key to understanding. Prog. Brain Res. 2016, 227, 277–304. [Google Scholar]
- Zentall, S.S.; Smith, Y.N.; Lee, Y.B.B.; Wieczorek, C. Mathematical outcomes of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Learn. Disabil. 1994, 27, 510–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lucangeli, D.; Cabrele, S. Mathematical difficulties and ADHD. Exceptionality 2006, 14, 53–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Platt, A. ADHD and math disabilities: Cognitive similarities and instructional interventions. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Raghubar, K.P.; Barnes, M.A.; Hecht, S.A. Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2010, 20, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrouillet, P.; Fayol, M.; Lathulière, E. Selecting between competitors in multiplication tasks: An explanation of the errors produced by adolescents with learning difficulties. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1997, 21, 253–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilmore, C.; Keeble, S.; Richardson, S.; Cragg, L. The role of cognitive inhibition in different components of arithmetic. Zent.bl. Didakt. Math. 2015, 47, 771–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lemaire, P.; Lecacheur, M. Age-related changes in children’s executive functions and strategy selection: A study in computational estimation. Cogn. Dev. 2011, 26, 282–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koponen, T.; Salmi, P.; Eklund, K.; Aro, T. Counting and RAN: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 105, 162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roelofs, A. Context effects of pictures and words in naming objects, reading words, and generating simple phrases. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. 2006, 59, 1764–1784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bruffaerts, R.; Tyler, L.K.; Shafto, M.; Tsvetanov, K.A.; Clarke, A. Perceptual and conceptual processing of visual objects across the adult lifespan. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kruk, R.S.; Luther Ruban, C. Beyond phonology: Visual processes predict alphanumeric and nonalphanumeric rapid naming in poor early readers. J. Learn. Disabil. 2018, 51, 18–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poulsen, M.; Elbro, C. What’s in a name depends on the type of name: The relationships between semantic and phonological access, reading fluency, and reading comprehension. Sci. Stud. Read. 2013, 17, 303–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Butterworth, B. The development of arithmetical abilities. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2005, 46, 3–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szűcs, D.; Goswami, U. Developmental dyscalculia: Fresh perspectives. Trends Neurosc. Educ. 2013, 2, 33–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhang, J.; Fan, X.; Cheung, S.K.; Meng, Y.; Cai, Z.; Hu, B.Y. The role of early language abilities on math skills among Chinese children. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0181074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vukovic, R.K.; Lesaux, N.K. The language of mathematics: Investigating the ways language counts for children’s mathematical development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 115, 227–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petersen, S.E.; Posner, M.I. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 35, 73–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Marotta, A.; Casagrande, M.; Lupiáñez, J. Object-based attentional effects in response to eye-gaze and arrow cues. Acta Psychol. 2013, 143, 317–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marotta, A.; Casagrande, M.; Rosa, C.; Maccari, L.; Berloco, B.; Pasini, A. Impaired reflexive orienting to social cues in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2014, 23, 649–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spagna, A.; Martella, D.; Sebastiani, M.; Maccari, L.; Marotta, A.; Casagrande, M. Efficiency and interactions of alerting, orienting and executive networks: The impact of imperative stimulus type. Acta Psychol. 2014, 148, 209–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Federico, F.; Marotta, A.; Martella, D.; Casagrande, M. Development in attention functions and social processing: Evidence from the Attention Network Test. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2017, 35, 169–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casagrande, M.; Marotta, A.; Martella, D.; Volpari, E.; Agostini, F.; Favieri, F.; Giovannoli, J. Assessing the three attentional networks in children from three to six years: A child-friendly version of the Attentional Network Test for Interaction. Behav. Res. Methods 2021, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casagrande, M.; Martella, D.; Ruggiero, M.C.; Maccari, L.; Paloscia, C.; Rosa, C.; Pasini, A. Assessing attentional systems in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2012, 27, 30–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Domain (N of Studies) | MD-CG Difference (N of Studies) |
---|---|
Processing speed (22) | MD < CG (17/22) |
STM verbal (12) | MD < CG (9/12) |
STM visuospatial (4) | MD < CG (1/4) |
LTM verbal (2) | MD < CG (1/2) |
Attention (9) | MD < CG (9/9) |
WM verbal (21) | MD < CG (16/21) |
WM visuospatial (14) | MD < CG (9/14) |
Inhibition (8) | MD < CG (6/8) |
Cognitive Flexibility (7) | MD < CG (4/7) |
Phonological processing and awareness (4) | MD < CG (2/4) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Agostini, F.; Zoccolotti, P.; Casagrande, M. Domain-General Cognitive Skills in Children with Mathematical Difficulties and Dyscalculia: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 239. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020239
Agostini F, Zoccolotti P, Casagrande M. Domain-General Cognitive Skills in Children with Mathematical Difficulties and Dyscalculia: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(2):239. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020239
Chicago/Turabian StyleAgostini, Francesca, Pierluigi Zoccolotti, and Maria Casagrande. 2022. "Domain-General Cognitive Skills in Children with Mathematical Difficulties and Dyscalculia: A Systematic Review of the Literature" Brain Sciences 12, no. 2: 239. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020239
APA StyleAgostini, F., Zoccolotti, P., & Casagrande, M. (2022). Domain-General Cognitive Skills in Children with Mathematical Difficulties and Dyscalculia: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Brain Sciences, 12(2), 239. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020239