Next Article in Journal
Camrelizumab-Induced Isolate Abducens Neuritis: A Rare Ophthalmic Immune-Related Adverse Events
Previous Article in Journal
Experimental Induction of Intracranial Aneurysms in Rats: A New Model Utilizing a Genetic Modification within the EDNRA Gene
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Continuous Neurophysiologic Data Accurately Predict Mood and Energy in the Elderly

Center for Neuroeconomics Studies, Claremont Graduate University, Claremont, CA 91711, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(9), 1240; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12091240
Submission received: 11 August 2022 / Revised: 1 September 2022 / Accepted: 7 September 2022 / Published: 14 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Computational Neuroscience and Neuroinformatics)

Abstract

:
The elderly have an elevated risk of clinical depression because of isolation from family and friends and a reticence to report their emotional states. The present study explored whether data from a commercial neuroscience platform could predict low mood and low energy in members of a retirement community. Neurophysiologic data were collected continuously for three weeks at 1Hz and averaged into hourly and daily measures, while mood and energy were captured with self-reports. Two neurophysiologic measures averaged over a day predicted low mood and low energy with 68% and 75% accuracy. Principal components analysis showed that neurologic variables were statistically associated with mood and energy two days in advance. Applying machine learning to hourly data classified low mood and low energy with 99% and 98% accuracy. Two-day lagged hourly neurophysiologic data predicted low mood and low energy with 98% and 96% accuracy. This study demonstrates that continuous measurement of neurophysiologic variables may be an effective way to reduce the incidence of mood disorders in vulnerable people by identifying when interventions are needed.

1. Introduction

Depression is a primary public health concern globally. The elderly are particularly vulnerable to depression due to age-related neural atrophy, hypertension, and social isolation [1,2,3]. There are a variety of ways to inhibit the onset of depression, including social support, psychological counseling and pharmacotherapy [4,5,6,7]. Such interventions are more effective if a decline in mood can be identified before a major depressive episode occurs [8]. The ability to passively assess mood states using technology would be an important public health advance [9]. Several approaches to predicting low moods are now being investigated. For example, using smartwatches and machine learning to analyze sleep as depressive episodes are associated with disordered sleep patterns [10].
Chronic low mood increases morbidity and mortality, especially in older adults [11,12]. When people experience low moods that last for two weeks or more, they are diagnosed as clinically depressed [13]. The lifetime incidence of depression is 14.6% for adults in developed countries [14], and women are approximately twice as likely as men to have an episode of depression [15]. In the U.S., those aged 65 and older have a one in four depression risk [16,17]. Life events can increase the likelihood of depression in seniors, including declining health, financial straits, losses of loved ones, reduced social interactions, inadequate healthcare, and the inability to participate in activities [18,19,20,21,22]. Depression in old age is also a risk factor for dementia [23].
On the other hand, positive affect has a host of favorable impacts on health in the elderly, including a lower risk of cardiovascular disease [24], a reduction in pain [25,26], increased exercise [27], improved immune function [24,28], and better social relationships [29]. It is likely that the causal flow connecting positive mood to improved health is bidirectional [30,31] and depends in part on one’s genetics [32]. The importance of mood states on healthspan demands a more fundamental understanding of the causes of mood variations [33]. As this research advances, data that quantify activities and physiology using wearable technologies has exploded. These new data may make it possible to predict mood states in the elderly using physiology in order to create interventions to reduce or eliminate the degradation of health from persistent negative affect.
Not only is there a great need to predict moods, but the use of neural data obviates the need to constantly query individuals. Self-reports tend to be inaccurate, especially in the elderly [34,35]. Colloquially, people may be “worried to death,” and indeed, there is an extensive literature relating negative mood states and clinical depression to anxiety [36,37]. Anxious individuals have elevated activity of the sympathetic autonomic nervous system [38]. Typical measures of phasic sympathetic tone include heart rate and electrodermal activity. Most pharmacotherapies for depression reduce sympathetic tone, along with other effects [39]. While basal sympathetic tone varies substantially across individuals [40], measures of sympathetic nervous system activity are a reliable prodrome for depression [41,42,43].
When individuals, including the elderly, are anxious, their ability to enjoy life is inhibited [44]. Indeed, social activities in the elderly reduce anxiety [45,46,47,48] as do supportive relationships [5,49,50]. Seniors can create opportunities for social interactions by volunteering [51], investing in friendships [5], and joining group activities [50]. Socially active seniors are less likely to suffer from anxiety [52].
Depressive symptoms in seniors may arise when individuals no longer enjoy activities (anhedonia). However, even with observation, it may take weeks or months to correctly classify an individual as depressed since variations in moods are common. When depressive symptoms are identified early, the prognosis for patients is substantially improved [8]. The interaction between the quality of social activities and mood has the potential to be measured neurophysiologically [53,54].
Low mood and depression have been predicted by applying artificial intelligence techniques to digital traces [55]. A typical approach employs natural language processing for social media posts and chats [56,57]. Other research has used machine learning to predict moods using song choices [58], street views [59], pictures of faces [60], and images from video conferences [61]. These approaches are convenient because they use publicly available data without the need for direct measurement of participants. However, a shortcoming of this approach is the use of surveys of nonparticipants to assess the moods of participants. This induces bias in the dependent variables as people inconsistently identify others’ moods [62,63]. As a result, the predictive accuracy of these models seldom exceeds the 70–80% range.
The present exploratory study sought to relate self-reported moods to neurophysiologic data collected directly from study participants. This is a difficult task as consciously-filtered self-report measures are typically unrelated to neural activity [35]. Mood was assessed using retrospective daily self-reports, while neural measures were obtained at 1Hz for 8–10 h per day. The first step in creating a potential early detection measure for melancholia is to determine if neurophysiologic measures are associated with mood. The present study used a sample of healthy seniors, rather than a clinical population, in order to test the hypothesis that a combination of neural measures derived from a wearable sensor would predict changes in mood states.

2. Materials and Methods

Participants. Twenty-four participants were recruited from a Texas residential living facility. A flyer was circulated requesting signups for the study after obtaining permission from facility management. Those with serious health conditions were excluded from participation. Residents were provided with Apple Watch 6s loaded an app called Immersion Mobile to collect neurophysiologic data. Data were collected for 20 days between 18 January and 24 February 2021 for up to 10 h each day. All participants who started the study completed it. The initial analysis averaged neurophysiologic data for each day, resulting in 480 observations. Next, we generated average hourly data producing 2478 observations to explore whether higher frequency data improved predictive accuracy.
Procedure. Participants were sent an email every day at 6 a.m. and asked to complete an online survey reporting their mood, health, and energy the day before. If no response was collected by noon, participants were reminded via email and text to complete the survey. Missing data from the self-reports was <4%. Retrospective self-reports were used to capture perceptions of the previous day’s mood because contemporaneous self-reports may induce a bias towards one’s acute mood state, but can reduce accuracy due to poor recall and misattribution of arousal [64,65]. This approach decreases the likelihood of significant associations to physiologic signals associated with retrospective mood reports. In order to reduce the burden of data collection, which required wearing a smartwatch daily and charging it overnight, the only additional information obtained from residents was their biological sex and whether they were ill.
Neurophysiology. A commercial platform (Immersion Neuroscience, Henderson, NV, USA) was used to measure neurophysiologic responses collected at 1Hz. The independent variables obtained from the Immersion platform were average Immersion for each day and average psychological safety. Neurologic Immersion combines signals associated with attention and emotional resonance and measures the value the brain places on social experiences [66,67] The attentional response is associated with dopamine binding to the prefrontal cortex, while emotional resonance is related to oxytocin release from the brainstem [67,68,69]. The Immersion Neuroscience platform infers neural states from the activity of the cranial nerves using the downstream effects of dopamine and oxytocin on cardiac rhythms [66,70]. The data are sent to the cloud continuously, and the Immersion platform provides an output file used in the analysis. We chose to measure neurologic Immersion for this study because of the relationship between the quality of social connections and mood in the elderly [31].
A second neurologic measure from the Immersion platform, psychological safety (PS), measures parasympathetic tone, which reflects stress levels that impact mood [71]. In addition, we created a variable called peak Immersion, defined as
t = 0 T   ( v i t > M i ) d t / I m i
where vit is the hourly average neurophysiologic Immersion for each participant in day i at time t to the end of the day at time T, Mi is the median of the average hourly time series of Immersion for day i plus the standard deviation of the hourly data for day i for each participant, and this is divided by the sum of total Immersion Imi for each person for each day i. That is, peak Immersion (PI) cumulates the highest Immersion moments for an individual during the day capturing high-value social experiences relative to the Immersion from total social experiences.
Self-Report Measures. Mood was assessed by averaging four questions on a 1–5 scale (cheerful, stressed, lonely, energy) with stressed and lonely reverse coded using an abbreviated version of the Profile of Mood States (POMS) [72,73]. Mood was defined as “Low” if it was lower than the median value 4 and was labeled “High” for values greater than or equal to 4. Due to the moderate period of data collection, we did not seek to identify clinical depression, a state in which people remain for a period of time. Rather, we sought to identify variations in mood troughs that may be a prodrome for clinical depression. Mood has only moderate interpersonal and intrapersonal variation (Intrapersonal CV: 10.80%, Interpersonal CV: 16.26%). Energy was used as a second dependent variable because social activities are typically energizing. Energy has more variation than Mood (Intrapersonal CV: 23.64%, Interpersonal CV: 31.45%). “Low” energy was defined as a value of the median of 3 or lower and “High” was for values greater than the median. The only demographic data collected in this exploratory study was biological sex. A binary variable identifying if the participant was sick (Sick) was collected as a control.
Statistical Analysis. Multiple techniques were used to extract as many insights from the data as possible and to establish the robustness of our findings. While the data constitute a panel, both Mood and Energy show little time-series variation. Statistical tests (Results) indicate that each observation can be analyzed as an independent observation. The analysis begins with t-tests and correlations relating Mood and Energy to neurologic variables averaged for each day. We tested mean-corrected differences in neurologic variables for low and high Mood and Energy. An ordinary least squares (OLS) regression was estimated to predict participants’ Moods and Energy using Immersion, psychological safety, and peak Immersion as independent variables and including Sick as a control. Logistic (logit) regressions were also estimated to establish predictive accuracy. In addition, since neural variables are expected to be moderately correlated, principal components analysis (PCA) was used to seek to improve predictive accuracy. We used nine input features for the PCA: immersion, peak immersion, psychological safety with one- and two-day lags. These analyses were performed using the Stata 17 software package (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA).
Average hourly Immersion and psychological safety were used to build machine learning (ML) models to assess predictive accuracy compared to models using daily data. The ML models included regularized logistic regressions, random forests (RF), and support vector machines (SVM). Participants missing more than four hourly observations were dropped from the analyses for that day. In order to avoid further data loss, separate models were trained using Immersion and psychological safety. The tsfresh package in Python was used to extract features from the hourly time-series data. The tsfresh package extracts up to 794 time-series features and tests these for relationships to the dependent variables [74]. Features that had bivariate correlations with the dependent variables that had ps > 0.05 were discarded.
The unbalanced panel led us to use the synthetic minority oversampling technique (SMOTE) to balance the ratio of high and low values for mood and energy [75]. We then split the SMOTE data into a training (75% of the complete dataset) and testing (25%) set. The former set was used to tune hyperparameters using a 5-fold cross-validation grid search (the GridSearchCV function in the sklearn package). Table A1 in Appendix A shows the parameters tuned for the machine learning models.
We then estimated the models on the test set and the pre-SMOTE data. The area under the curve (AUC) was assessed for model fit, and true positive and true negative rates (Precision and Recall, respectively) and their average was used to measure accuracy. Possible overfitting was tested using 5-fold cross-validation on the entire data set. Accuracy and fit were examined for contemporaneous data and for a two-day lag prior to the self-report to determine the consistency of the neural measures.

3. Results

Descriptive Statistics. Mood and Energy had means on the high end of the reporting range and moderate spreads (Mood: M = 3.84 SD = 0.65, Figure 1; Energy: M = 3.04 SD = 0.97, Figure 2). The neurophysiologic variables fell into expected ranges (Immersion: M = 36.29, SD = 3.51, Range = [28.85, 48.91]; PI: M = 0.0076, SD = 0.0079, Range = [0, 0.054]; PS: M = 18.14, SD = 5.51, Range = [8.80, 40.70]).
Time Series Aspects. We tested if the time series of the two dependent variables contained information that should be included in our analyses. If participants’ time series were random walks, then the time series component of the analysis could be ignored. We used a Fisher-type unit-root examination using augmented Dickey–Fuller tests in which the null hypothesis is that the time series has a unit root [76]. The estimation indicated that both the one- and two-day lagged time component of the dependent variables did not affect the current value of the dependent variables (Table A2). As a result, each observation can be analyzed independently, ignoring the time dimension.
Mood. Among the three physiologic variables, only PI was statistically related to Mood (Immersion: r = 0.091, t (318) = 1.25, p = 0.105; PI: r = 0.16, t (318) = 2.66, p = 0.004; PS: r = −0.001, t (318) = −2.23, p = 0.990). Participants with high Moods had higher PI (Mhigh = 0.009, Mlow = 0.007; t (318) = −2.01, p = 0.02) and no difference in the other physiologic variables (Immersion: Mhigh = 36.14, Mlow = 36.35; t (318) = 0.4812, p = 0.68; PS: Mhigh = 1.77, Mlow = 1.82; t (318) = 0.721, p = 0.7644). Moods in males averaged 5.5% higher than that of females (Mmale = 4.0, Mfemale = 3.79; t (318) = −2.384, p = 0.008).
An ordinary least squares regression was used to test if Immersion, PS, and PI were related to participants’ Moods while controlling for sex and being sick. Thirteen values for PI exceeded 5 SD above the mean and these observations were removed. Due to the high variance of PI, it was standardized by subtracting the mean and dividing by the sample standard deviation using Stata’s standardize function. The regression was statistically significant (R2 = 0.173, F (5, 298) = 12.54, p =.000) and showed that Immersion and PI were positively associated with Mood, but PS was not (Immersion: ß = 0.403, p = 0.002; PI: ß = 0.155, p = 0.003; PS: ß = −0.091, p = 0.264). Variance inflation factors (VIFs) were within acceptable limits showing the estimation was well-specified.
A logistic regression was then estimated to examine the predictive accuracy of the physiologic variables. As above, the logistic regression showed that Immersion and PI were significant along with moderately large odds ratios (OR) (Immersion: OR = 3.29, p = 0.014; PI: OR = 1.533, p = 0.026; PS: OR = 1.136, p = 0.671; Table 1) with predictive accuracy of 67.76%.
We used the logistic regression estimates to examine when a participant was likely to have low Mood. For a woman who is not sick, there is a 75% likelihood she has low Mood when Immersion is 26.50 or lower, holding PS and PI at their medians. A man has a 75% chance of having low Mood when his Immersion is 12.36 or below for the median values of PS and PI. The threshold values of Immersion producing a 75% chance of low Mood are higher when PI and PS are lower (Table A3).
Principal Components. The first principal component (PC1) was loaded primarily on contemporaneous values and 1- and 2-day lags of PS and Immersion. The second principal component (PC2) was loaded on contemporaneous values and 1- and 2- day lags of PI. The third principal component (PC3) was loaded almost entirely on Immersion with a one-day lag (Table A4). Regression analysis was used to test the relationship between the first three principal components and the dependent variables. All three PCs were statistically significant (PC1: ß = 0.0599, p = 0.002; PC2: ß = 0.142, p = 0.000; PC3: ß = -0.095, p = 0.028). In addition, all three principal components were significant predictors of high and low Mood in a logistic regression (PC1: OR = 1.375, p = 0.000; PC2: OR = 1.963, p = 0.000; PC3: OR = 0.642, p = 0.026). The use of principal components improved the predictive accuracy of Mood to 74.81% (Table A5).
Energy. The second dependent variable, Energy, was centered on the mean (M = 3.038, SD = 0.966; Figure 2). Immersion, PI and PS were all related to Energy bilaterally (Immersion: r = 0.184, t (318) = 2.63, p = 0.009; PI: r = 0.233, t (318) = 4.84, p = 0.000; PS: r = 0.151, t (318) = 2.72, p = 0.0068). Immersion was 5.3% higher in those with high Energy compared to Immersion in low Energy participants (Mhigh = 3.76, Mlow = 3.57; t (318) = −4.138, p = 0.000). PI and PS were also higher in those with high Energy compared to low Energy participants (PI: Mhigh = 0.01, Mlow = 0.006; t (318) = −3.925, p = 0.000; PS: Mhigh = 1.99, Mlow = 1.74; t (318) = −3.7122, p = 0.000). There were no sex differences in participants’ Energy (Mmale = 3.08, Mfemale = 3.02; t (318) = -0.442, p = 0.659).
Regression estimates revealed significant associations for Immersion and PI with Energy (Immersion: ß = 0.498, p =.010; PI: ß = 0.268, p = 0.001; PS: ß = 0.122, p = 0.324). A logistic regression for high and low Energy found Immersion and PI were significant predictors (Immersion: OR = 3.11, p = 0.020; PI: OR = 1.655, p = 0.011; PS: OR = 1.66, p = 0.094; Table 2) and had predictive accuracy of 74.67%.
The logistic regression estimates were used to identify predictors for high and low Energy. For a woman who is not sick, there is a 75% or greater likelihood she has low Energy when Immersion is 35.80 or below, setting PI and PS to their medians. The corresponding threshold for men is an Immersion value of 40.17 (Table A6).
Next, we used ordinary least squares to test if PC1, PC2, and PC3 were related to participants’ Energy. PC1 and PC2 were both associated with Energy, though PC3 was not (PC1: ß = 0.143, p = 0.000; PC2: ß = 0.266, p = 0.000; PC3: ß = −0.116, p = 0.076). A logistic regression for high and low Energy found a significant association for PC1 and PC2 as in the OLS results (PC1: OR = 1.476, p = 0.000; PC 2: OR = 2.26, p = 0.000; PC3: OR = 0.889, p = 0.485) and produced a predictive accuracy of 74.81% (Table A7).
Machine Learning Models. We first estimated models using hourly data (2478 observations) to predict Mood. Extracting significant components produced 18 features from Immersion and five from PS. Then, we balanced the data with SMOTE and tuned the hyperparameters. The models were estimated on the training set with the following parametric restrictions for both Immersion and PS: logit (C = 100, penalty = “l2”), RF (max_features = ‘log2′, ‘min_samples_leaf’ = 2, ‘min_samples_split’ =2), and SVM (C = 100, kernel = ‘rbf’).
The ML estimations classifying Mood fit the data well in the test set (AUCs > 0.90). The RF algorithm produced the best fit for the test data set (AUC = 1.00), whereas the fit for logit (AUC = 0.71) and SVM (AUC = 0.82) declined. Using PS as the explanatory variable for the ML estimations had similarly high goodness-of-fit on the test set for RF (AUC = 0.90) and SVM (AUC = 0.94), but only moderate fit for regularized logit (AUC = 0.76). AUC fell for all three ML models when classifying the test observations (Logit: 0.59, RF: 0.78, SVM: 0.81). To account for possible overfitting, we used 5-fold cross-validation on the full SMOTE data. All models maintained high scores across the five folds indicating they were not overfit. Predictive accuracy for the ML models using Immersion was very high for the observed data; correct classification of Mood ranged from 99 to 100%. Using PS in the ML estimation was nearly as accurate, ranging from 98 to 100% (Table A8).
Repeating the ML estimations using Energy as the dependent variable, 27 features from the Immersion data and 11 from PS were extracted using logit (C = 100, penalty = “l2”), RF (max_features = ‘sqrt’, ‘min_samples_leaf’ = 2, ‘min_samples_split’ = [2, 5]), and SVM (C = 10, kernel = ‘rbf’). Immersion explained the Energy data moderately well, with RF performing the best on the test (AUC = 0.80) and observed set (AUC = 0.93) and SVM and logit performing adequately (SVM: 0.78; 0.86; Logit: 0.73; 0.78). Cross-validation shows that all the models perform moderately well but with high variation (SDs > 0.044).
Predictive accuracy for the Immersion ML models of Energy using the observed data was high, though below the classification accuracy of Mood, with the RF being most accurate (logit: 82%; RF: 95%; SVM: 90%). The RF and SVM models of Energy using PS as the independent variable had similar accuracy to the Immersion models, while the regularized logit was fairly inaccurate (logit: 65%; RF: 94%; SVM: 88%, Table A9).
Two Day Lag Machine Learning Models. We examined the predictive accuracy of Mood with neural data two days prior to the self-report to extend the principal component findings (2422 observations). Seventeen features were extracted from Immersion, and eight features were extracted from PS. Hyperparameters for Immersion were tuned using: logit (C = 1, penalty = “l2”), RF (max_features = [‘log2′;’sqrt’], ‘min_samples_leaf’ = 2, ‘min_samples_split’ = [5, 2]), and SVM (C = 10, kernel = ‘rbf’).
The two-day lagged goodness of fit mirrored the contemporaneous estimations. Fit was quite good for Mood using Immersion for the test and observation datasets for RF (AUC = 1.00; 0.96) and SVM (AUC = 0.99; 0.96). The logit performed well on the test set, but only moderately on the observed set (AUC = 0.94; 0.74). The fits for the estimations using PS were also good, though they declined somewhat from the test to observed data (RF: AUC = 0.91, 0.79; SVM: AUC = 0.96; 0.88). As with the Immersion estimates, the logit performed poorly (AUC = 0.67; 0.57; Table A10). The cross-validation showed that all models fit well except for logit (AUC = 0.78, SD = 0.055).
Next, we used Immersion and PS to predict Energy with two-day lagged data. This procedure extracted 30 features from Immersion and four from PS. Hyperparameters were: logit (C = [1, 10], penalty = “l2”), RF (max_features = [‘log2′, ’sqrt’], ‘min_samples_leaf’ = 2, ‘min_samples_split’ = 2), and SVM (C = 100, kernel = ‘rbf’). All Immersion and PS models did moderately well on the test dataset (AUCs ≥ 0.84). Indeed, the RF (AUC = 0.93) and SVM (AUC = 0.94) Immersion models fit better on the observed data set than on the training set. The PS models performed moderately well on the test data set, but only RF did so on the observed dataset (AUC = 0.83).
Predictive accuracy continued to be quite high using hourly data lagged two days. Immersion, in particular, strongly predicted Mood using the lagged data for all three ML estimations (logit: 92%, RF: 99%, SVM: 99%). The two-day lagged RF and SVM models were nearly identical in their accuracy compared to the contemporaneous data estimates. Predictive accuracy of Mood using two-day lagged PS was lower compared to the use of Immersion and was similar to the contemporaneous estimates. The RF and SVM models had high accuracy, while the logit was moderately accurate (logit: 76%; RF: 95%; SVM: 98%). The predictions for individuals’ Energy using Immersion were 95% and 96% accuracy (RF, SVM) for the observed data, while the logit model performed only moderately (79%). The two-day lagged predictions for Energy using PS, as with Mood, were less accurate, with the RF model being the best (logit: 57%, RF: 87%, SVM: 68%; Table A11).

4. Discussion

The key contribution of the present exploratory study is to demonstrate that high-frequency neurophysiologic measures accurately predict self-reported emotional states. Passive and continuous data collection, as used here, appears to be an effective way to monitor emotional wellness. Our analysis shows that declines in emotional states can be predicted two days in advance with high accuracy. Such data make it possible for family members or clinicians to check in on the elderly in order to halt a decline before a potential mental health crisis occurs. More generally, we have shown that neural measures can be used to monitor the quality of life in seniors and perhaps other vulnerable populations. Neural predictors of emotional states can also be used to identify the physiological processes inhibiting satisfaction with one’s life so that interventions are focused and effective [77].
The results are surprising because 1Hz neurophysiologic data were averaged into hourly and daily measures that we expected would return to a long-term equilibrium. However, this was not the case. Daily observations were statistically independent of each other, yet had predictive value as a group. Estimations using daily data predicted low Mood and low Energy with 68–75% accuracy using just three neurophysiologic variables in a standard logistic regression, controlling for illness and sex. The over-2000 hourly observations led us to estimate ML models for Mood and Energy. These models were extremely accurate, correctly classifying the dependent variables with 84–100% accuracy and were not overfit.
The most predictive measure, Immersion, appears to capture the neural value of social experiences, a key aspect of flourishing [78,79]. Neural measures such as Immersion are necessary because the ability to self-report the quality of relationships is difficult and is nearly impossible with the granularity of the neurophysiologic measures used here. Measurement of social connections is vital because they increase the positive affect [80,81] and improve life satisfaction [82,83,84].
Psychological safety on a daily basis did not influence Mood or Energy. However, this variable accurately predicted both dependent variables when measured hourly. Extensive research has related psychological safety and the absence of anxiety to improved mood [85]. Psychological safety regulates people’s emotional well-being by motivating a desire for social support to reduce anxiety [86,87,88]. When anxiety is reduced, the quality of social relationships improves, enhancing healthspans [89,90].
As expected, sickness reduces Mood and Energy. The desire and ability to socialize are reduced with illness, inhibiting the benefits of socializing [91,92,93]. Sickness negatively affects the quality of life in the elderly in part by inducing negative moods [94]. Chronic illness reduces the independence and mobility of senior citizens [95] affecting their ability to engage with others [91,96]. The physiologic variables in the predictive models of Mood and Energy were significant even when removing the effect of sickness.
The use of a commercial Neuroscience as a Service (NaaS) platform allowed us to exploit fully processed neurophysiologic measures that were captured at scale. This approach makes it easy for other researchers to replicate and extend our findings without having to buy expensive equipment or process high-frequency data. Nevertheless, this study has a number of limitations that should be addressed in subsequent research. The number of participants was small even while the number of observations was high. The sample population studied was fairly homogenous and came from a single retirement home. While we focused on predicting troughs in Mood and Energy in psychologically healthy adults, these states do not necessarily lead to clinical depression [97]. Future research should apply the methodology here to other vulnerable populations, including those with diagnosed mental illnesses, to assess its predictive accuracy in these populations. Lastly, we used two measures from the Immersion platform (Immersion, PS) and derived another measure (PI). Future research could derive additional measures from these data or, as we have done, rely on ML approaches to extract as much predictive value from the variables we used.

5. Conclusions

As people age, their moods tend to decline [98,99]. We have shown that troughs in Mood and Energy can be accurately predicted with off-the-shelf wearables and data from a commercial software platform. The present study did not seek to identify clinical depression, which would require longer data collection and more individuals. Rather, we hope that our findings will motivate researchers to identify the causal factors that threaten emotional wellness in older adults and other vulnerable populations. Future research should assess our techniques for those with diagnosed depression who are at risk of recurrence as well as professionals who face chronically high levels of stress, including first responders, clinicians, and members of the military. Accurate measurement is the first step toward improving emotional health so that people can live happier, healthier, and longer lives.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.J.Z.; formal analysis, S.H.M., M.K., R.S.A. and P.J.Z.; methodology, P.J.Z.; software, P.J.Z.; writing—original draft, R.S.A.; writing—review and editing, S.H.M., M.K. and P.J.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This project was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Claremont Graduate University (#1255) and follows the conditions in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Informed Consent Statement

All participants provided written informed consent prior to inclusion.

Data Availability Statement

The data are available on request from the senior author (P.J.Z.).

Conflicts of Interest

The senior author (P.J.Z.) is one of the founders of Immersion Neuroscience that was used in the data collection, but he had no influence on the analysis or interpretation of the findings.

Appendix A

Table A1. Tuned parameters for the machine learning (ML) models.
Table A1. Tuned parameters for the machine learning (ML) models.
MLParameterValues
LogitC
Regularization
1, 10, 100
L1,l2, elasticnet
RFMinimum sample split
Minimum samples per leaf
Maximum features per tree
2, 5, 10
2, 5, 10, 15
Square root, log2
SVMC
Kernal method
1, 10, 100
Linear, polynomial, radial, sigmoid
Table A2. Fisher-type unit-root test based on augmented Dickey–Fuller (ADF) tests for Mood and Energy.
Table A2. Fisher-type unit-root test based on augmented Dickey–Fuller (ADF) tests for Mood and Energy.
DistributionMoodEnergy
Statistic (p Value)Statistic (p Value)
Inverse Chi2 (36)92.69 (0.000)57.19 (0.014)
Inverse Normal−2.14 (0.016)−2.63 (0.004)
Inverse Logit t (89)−4.319 (0.000)−2.70 (0.004)
Mod. Inv. Chi26.682 (0.000)2.49 (0.006)
Table A3. There is a 75% chance of low Mood for the given values of Immersion setting PS and PI to their medians for participants who are not sick. The PI values are unstandardized.
Table A3. There is a 75% chance of low Mood for the given values of Immersion setting PS and PI to their medians for participants who are not sick. The PI values are unstandardized.
Mood ImmersionPSPI
Male12.361.760.005
Female26.501.760.005
Table A4. Principal components loadings.
Table A4. Principal components loadings.
VariablePrincipal Components Analysis (PCA)
PC 1PC 2PC 3
Immersion0.4020.011−0.125
Immersion (−1)0.398−0.0470.058
Immersion (−2)0.385−0.003−0.207
Peak0.0710.564−0.346
Peak (−1)0.0590.5590.801
Peak (−2)0.0640.591−0.389
Safety0.399−0.0630.090
Safety (−1)0.423−0.0690.112
Safety (−2)0.425−0.0970.061
% Variance Explained46.45%15.70%9.05%
Table A5. Principal components regression results for Mood with Sick and Male as controls.
Table A5. Principal components regression results for Mood with Sick and Male as controls.
VariableOLSVIFLogitOdd Ratio
PC 10.0599 **
(0.018)
1.10.319
(0.079)
1.375
PC 20.142
(0.037)
1.030.674
(0.171)
1.963
PC 3−0.095
(0.043)
1.03−0.443
(0.199)
0.642
Sick −0.917
(0.172)
1.01−5.04
(1.58)
0.006
Male
Intercept
0.297
(0.093)
3.84 ***
(0.043)
1.122.06
(0.430)
0.285
(0.162)
7.86
F-value11.59
p-value(0.000)
R-squared0.182
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table A6. There is a 75% chance of low Energy for the given values of Immersion setting PS and PI to their medians for participants who are not sick. The PI values are unstandardized.
Table A6. There is a 75% chance of low Energy for the given values of Immersion setting PS and PI to their medians for participants who are not sick. The PI values are unstandardized.
Energy ImmersionPSPI
Male40.171.760.005
Female35.801.760.005
Table A7. Principal components regression results for Energy with Sick and Male as controls.
Table A7. Principal components regression results for Energy with Sick and Male as controls.
VariableOLSVIFLogitOdd Ratio
PC 10.143 *
(0.028)
1.10.389
(0.081)
1.48
PC 20.266
(0.056)
1.030.816
(0.161)
2.26
PC 3−0.116
(0.065)
1.03−0.117
(0.167)
0.889
Sick −0.918
(0.258)
1.01−2.09
(1.15)
0.123
Male
Intercept
0.134
(0.140)
3.078 ***
(0.065)
1.12−0.775
(0.457)
−0.736
(0.173)
0.461
F-value2.420
p-value(0.081)
R-squared 0.316
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table A8. ML classification of Mood using Immersion and PS in regularized logit, random forest (RF), and support vector machine (SVM) estimations. CV is cross-validation.
Table A8. ML classification of Mood using Immersion and PS in regularized logit, random forest (RF), and support vector machine (SVM) estimations. CV is cross-validation.
Immersion AUCAccuracyPrecisionRecall
TestLogit0.930.930.910.95
RF1.001.001.001.00
SVM0.930.911.000.83
ObservedLogit0.710.910.990.91
RF1.001.001.001.00
SVM0.820.961.000.96
CVLogit0.940.880.910.84
0.0320.0240.040.014
RF1.000.970.990.96
0.0040.0180.0240.031
SVM1.000.960.990.92
0.0060.0230.0120.051
PS
TestLogit0.760.760.80.68
RF0.90.890.940.83
SVM0.940.940.970.9
ObservedLogit0.590.750.980.75
RF0.780.941.000.94
SVM0.810.951.000.95
CVLogit0.850.790.810.75
0.0640.0650.0720.099
RF0.960.910.960.86
0.0330.0370.0420.095
SVM0.950.920.990.85
0.0180.030.0130.065
Table A9. ML classification of Energy using Immersion and PS in regularized logit, random forest (RF), and support vector machine (SVM) estimations. CV is cross-validation.
Table A9. ML classification of Energy using Immersion and PS in regularized logit, random forest (RF), and support vector machine (SVM) estimations. CV is cross-validation.
Immersion AUCAccuracyPrecisionRecall
TestLogit0.730.740.670.74
RF0.800.800.730.81
SVM0.780.780.780.67
ObservedLogit0.780.820.90.85
RF0.930.950.970.96
SVM0.860.900.970.88
CVLogit0.810.780.830.7
0.0810.0840.0950.089
RF0.880.80.840.74
0.0880.0830.1180.101
SVM0.890.80.880.71
0.0760.0740.10.083
PS
TestLogit0.70.710.650.63
RF0.830.830.750.89
SVM0.830.820.860.67
ObservedLogit0.630.650.850.63
RF0.930.940.960.96
SVM0.840.880.970.86
CVLogit0.750.650.680.58
0.0440.0490.0520.073
RF0.870.790.810.81
0.050.0330.0950.106
SVM0.870.810.870.75
0.050.0280.060.079
Table A10. ML classification of Mood using Immersion and PS with a 2-day lag in regularized logit, random forest (RF), and support vector machine (SVM) estimations. CV is cross-validation.
Table A10. ML classification of Mood using Immersion and PS with a 2-day lag in regularized logit, random forest (RF), and support vector machine (SVM) estimations. CV is cross-validation.
Immersion AUCAccuracyPrecisionRecall
TestLogit0.940.940.970.89
RF1.001.0011.001.00
SVM0.990.991.000.97
ObservedLogit0.740.920.990.93
RF0.960.991.000.99
SVM0.960.991.000.99
CVLogit0.960.930.960.89
0.0370.0340.0280.081
RF0.990.981.000.95
0.0020.02900.058
SVM0.990.981.000.95
0.0020.02100.042
PS
TestLogit0.670.630.550.83
RF0.910.910.890.91
SVM0.960.951.000.89
ObservedLogit0.570.760.970.76
RF0.790.950.980.96
SVM0.880.981.000.98
CVLogit0.780.690.680.68
0.0550.0440.030.102
RF0.990.950.970.93
0.0160.0310.0560.023
SVM0.970.960.990.93
0.020.0080.0120.016
Table A11. ML classification of Energy using Immersion and PS with a 2 day lag in regularized logit, random forest (RF), and support vector machine (SVM) estimations. CV is cross-validation.
Table A11. ML classification of Energy using Immersion and PS with a 2 day lag in regularized logit, random forest (RF), and support vector machine (SVM) estimations. CV is cross-validation.
Immersion AUCAccuracyPrecisionRecall
TestLogit0.840.840.870.82
RF0.850.850.880.85
SVM0.890.890.930.85
ObservedLogit0.750.790.900.80
RF0.930.950.970.96
SVM0.940.960.980.96
CVLogit0.830.780.820.71
0.070.070.080.08
RF0.890.810.870.74
0.090.090.130.09
SVM0.890.850.910.78
0.090.0810.120.073
Safety
TestLogit0.780.710.940.48
RF0.740.740.760.76
SVM0.800.740.950.55
ObservedLogit0.620.570.860.48
RF0.830.870.950.86
SVM0.700.680.940.59
CVLogit0.710.650.720.49
0.070.060.100.12
RF0.750.710.710.71
0.070.070.080.06
SVM0.710.680.780.50
0.060.070.080.12

References

  1. Boima, V.; Tetteh, J.; Yorke, E.; Archampong, T.; Mensah, G.; Biritwum, R.; Yawson, A.E. Older adults with hypertension have increased risk of depression compared to their younger counterparts: Evidence from the World Health Organization study of Global Ageing and Adult Health Wave 2 in Ghana. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 277, 329–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Meeks, T.W.; Vahia, I.V.; Lavretsky, H.; Kulkarni, G.; Jeste, D.V. A tune in “a minor” can “b major”: A review of epidemiology, illness course, and public health implications of subthreshold depression in older adults. J. Affect. Disord. 2011, 129, 126–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Social Isolation and Loneliness in Older Adults Opportunities for the Health Care System; The National Academies Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  4. Nakagomi, A.; Shiba, K.; Kondo, K.; Kawachi, I. Can online communication prevent depression among older people? A longitudinal analysis. J. Appl. Gerontol. 2022, 41, 167–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Santini, Z.I.; Fiori, K.L.; Feeney, J.; Tyrovolas, S.; Haro, J.M.; Koyanagi, A. Social relationships, loneliness, and mental health among older men and women in ireland: A prospective community-based study. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 204, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Cacioppo, J.T.; Patrick, W. Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection; WW Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  7. Miller, K.J.; Gonçalves-Bradley, D.C.; Areerob, P.; Hennessy, D.; Mesagno, C.; Grace, F. Comparative effectiveness of three exercise types to treat clinical depression in older adults: A systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Ageing Res. Rev. 2020, 58, 100999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Garland, E.J.; Solomons, K. Early detection of depression in young and elderly people. Br. Columbia Med. J. 2002, 44, 469–472. [Google Scholar]
  9. Sau, A.; Bhakta, I. Predicting anxiety and depression in elderly patients using machine learning technology. Healthc. Technol. Lett. 2017, 4, 238–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bader, C.S.; Skurla, M.; Vahia, I.V. Technology in the assessment, treatment, and management of depression. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 2020, 28, 60–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Van den Berg, K.S.; Wiersema, C.; Hegeman, J.M.; van den Brink, R.H.S.; Rhebergen, D.; Marijnissen, R.M.; Oude, V.R.C. Clinical characteristics of late-life depression predicting mortality. Aging Ment. Health 2021, 25, 476–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Mroczek, D.K.; Stawski, R.S.; Turiano, N.A.; Chan, W.; Almeida, D.M.; Neupert, S.D.; Spiro, A. Emotional reactivity and mortality: Longitudinal findings from the VA normative aging study. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2015, 70, 398–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. National Institute of Mental Health. Depression. 2018. Available online: https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/depression (accessed on 3 August 2022).
  14. Lim, G.Y.; Tam, W.W.; Lu, Y.; Ho, C.S.; Zhang, M.W.; Ho, R.C. Prevalence of depression in the community from 30 countries between 1994 and 2014. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 2861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Kuehner, C. Why is depression more common among women than among men? Lancet Psychiatry 2017, 4, 146–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Byers, A.L.; Yaffe, K.; Covinsky, K.E.; Friedman, M.B.; Bruce, M.L. High occurrence of mood and anxiety disorders among older adults: The national comorbidity survey replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 489–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Explore Depression in the United States: 2021 Senior Report. Available online: https://www.americashealthrankings.org/explore/senior/measure/depression_sr/state/ALL (accessed on 3 August 2022).
  18. Rodda, J.; Walker, Z.; Carter, J. Depression in older adults. BMJ Br. Med. J. 2011, 343, 683–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Matos, Q.A.; von Gunten, A.; Martins, M.; Wellens, N.I.H.; Verloo, H. The forgotten psychopathology of depressed long-term care facility residents: A call for evidence-based practice. Dement Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. Extra 2021, 11, 38–44. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cheruvu, V.K.; Chiyaka, E.T. Prevalence of depressive symptoms among older adults who reported medical cost as a barrier to seeking health care: Findings from a nationally representative sample. BMC Geriatr. 2019, 19, 192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Bekhet, A.K.; Zauszniewski, J.A. Mental health of elders in retirement communities: Is loneliness a key factor? Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2012, 26, 214–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Valiengo, L.; Stella, F.; Forlenza, O.V. Mood disorders in the elderly: Prevalence, functional impact, and management challenges. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016, 12, 2105–2114. [Google Scholar]
  23. Maier, A.; Riedel-Heller, S.G.; Pabst, A.; Luppa, M. Risk factors and protective factors of depression in older people 65+. A systematic review. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0251326. [Google Scholar]
  24. Dockray, S.; Steptoe, A. Positive affect and psychobiological processes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 35, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Zautra, A.J.; Johnson, L.M.; Davis, M.C. Positive affect as a source of resilience for women in chronic pain. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 73, 212–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Song, S.; Graham-Engeland, J.E.; Mogle, J.; Martire, L.M. The effects of daily mood and couple interactions on the sleep quality of older adults with chronic pain. J. Behav. Med. 2015, 38, 944–955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Khazaee-pool, M.; Sadeghi, R.; Majlessi, F.; Rahimi Foroushani, A. Effects of physical exercise programme on happiness among older people. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 2015, 22, 47–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Okely, J.A.; Weiss, A.; Gale, C.R. The interaction between stress and positive affect in predicting mortality. J. Psychosom. Res. 2017, 100, 53–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Steptoe, A.; Dockray, S.; Wardle, J. Positive affect and psychobiological processes relevant to health. J. Personal. 2009, 77, 1747–1776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Uchino, B.N.; Rook, K.S. Emotions, relationships, health and illness into old age. Maturitas 2020, 139, 42–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Golden, J.; Conroy, R.M.; Bruce, I.; Denihan, A.; Greene, E.; Kirby, M.; Lawlor, B.A. Loneliness, social support networks, mood and wellbeing in community-dwelling elderly. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2009, 24, 694–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Menezes, I.C.; von Werne Baes, C.; Lacchini, R.; Juruena, M.F. Genetic biomarkers for differential diagnosis of major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder: A systematic and critical review. Behav. Brain Res. 2019, 357, 29–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Caracciolo, B.; Bäckman, L.; Monastero, R.; Winblad, B.; Fratiglioni, L. The symptom of low mood in the prodromal stage of mild cognitive impairment and dementia: A cohort study of a community dwelling elderly population. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2011, 82, 788–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Mauss, I.B.; Robinson, M.D. Measures of emotion: A review. Cogn. Emot. 2009, 23, 209–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Brown, L.J.E.; Astell, A.J. Assessing mood in older adults: A conceptual review of methods and approaches. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2012, 24, 1197–1206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Ahrens, T.; Deuschle, M.; Krumm, B.; van der Pompe, G.; den Boer, J.A.; Lederbogen, F. Pituitary-adrenal and sympathetic nervous system responses to stress in women remitted from recurrent major depression. Psychosom. Med. 2008, 70, 461–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Taggart, P.; Lambiase, P. Anger, emotion, and arrhythmias: From brain to heart. Front. Physiol. 2011, 2, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Saviola, F.; Pappaianni, E.; Monti, A.; Grecucci, A.; Jovicich, J.; De Pisapia, N. Trait and state anxiety are mapped differently in the human brain. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 11112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Shores, M.M.; Pascualy, M.; Lewis, N.L.; Flatness, D.; Veith, R.C. Short-term sertraline treatment suppresses sympathetic nervous system activity in healthy human subjects. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2001, 26, 433–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Giuliano, R.J.; Gatzke-Kopp, L.M.; Roos, L.E.; Skowron, E.A. Resting sympathetic arousal moderates the association between parasympathetic reactivity and working memory performance in adults reporting high levels of life stress. Psychophysiology 2017, 54, 1195–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kalin, N.H. The critical relationship between anxiety and depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 2020, 177, 365–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Schreuder, M.J.; Hartman, C.A.; George, S.V.; Menne-Lothmann, C.; Decoster, J.; Winkel, R.; Delespaul, P.; Hert, M.D.; Derom, C.; Thiery, E.; et al. Early warning signals in psychopathology: What do they tell? BMC Med. 2020, 8, 269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Wichers, M.; Groot, P.C. Critical slowing down as a personalized early warning signal for depression. Psychother. Psychosom. 2016, 85, 114–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bourland, S.L.; Stanley, M.A.; Snyder, A.G.; Novy, D.M.; Beck, J.G.; Averill, P.M.; Swann, A.C. Quality of life in older adults with generalized anxiety disorder. Aging Ment. Health 2000, 4, 315–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lewinsohn, P.M.; Libet, J. Pleasant events, activity schedules, and depressions. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1972, 79, 291–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Leonavičius, R.; Adomaitienė, V. Anxiety and social activities in multiple sclerosis patients. Open Med. 2013, 8, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Márquez-González, M.; Losada, A.; Rider, K.L.; López-Pérez, B. Spanish validation of the California Older Person’s Pleasant Events Schedule (COPPES). Clin. Gerontol. 2014, 37, 151–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Rider, K.L.; Gallagher-Thompson, D.; Thompson, L.W. California Older Person’s Pleasant Events Schedule: A tool to help older adults increase positive experiences. Clin. Gerontol. 2016, 39, 64–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Everard, K.M.; Lach, H.W.; Fisher, E.B.; Baum, M.C. Relationship of activity and social support to the functional health of older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2000, 55, S208–S212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Holtfreter, K.; Reisig, M.D.; Turanovic, J.J. Depression and infrequent participation in social activities among older adults: The moderating role of high-quality familial ties. Aging Ment. Health 2017, 21, 379–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Heejung, J.; Fengyan, T. Effects of social support and volunteering on depression among grandparents raising grandchildren. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 2016, 83, 491–507. [Google Scholar]
  52. Brenes, G.A. Anxiety, depression, and quality of life in primary care patients. Prim. Care Companion J. Clin. Psychiatry 2007, 9, 437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Elizabeth, J.; King, N.; Ollendick, T.H.; Gullone, E.; Tonge, B.; Watson, S.; Macdermott, S. Social anxiety disorder in children and youth: A research update on aetiological factors. Couns. Psychol. Q. 2006, 19, 151–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Mendlowicz, M.V.; Stein, M.B. Quality of life in individuals with anxiety disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 2000, 157, 669–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Hutson, M. AI in action: How algorithms can analyze the mood of the masses. Science 2017, 357, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Saha, K.; Chan, L.; De Barbaro, K.; Abowd, G.D.; De Choudhury, M. Inferring mood instability on social media by leveraging ecological momentary assessments. Proc. ACM Interact. Mob. Wearable Ubiquitous Technol. 2017, 1, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Gaikwad, G.; Joshi, D.J. Multiclass mood classification on Twitter using lexicon dictionary and machine learning algorithms. In Proceedings of the 2016 International Conference on Inventive Computation Technologies (ICICT), Coimbatore, India, 26–27 August 2016. [Google Scholar]
  58. Panwar, S.; Rad, P.; Choo, K.K.R.; Roopaei, M. Are you emotional or depressed? Learning about your emotional state from your music using machine learning. J. Supercomput. 2019, 75, 2986–3009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Chen, C.; Li, H.; Luo, W.; Xie, J.; Yao, J.; Wu, L.; Xia, Y. Predicting the effect of street environment on residents’ mood states in large urban areas using machine learning and street view images. Sci. Total Environ. 2022, 816, 151605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Zhou, X.; Jin, K.; Shang, Y.; Guo, G. Visually interpretable representation learning for depression recognition from facial images. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 2018, 11, 542–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Dey, A.; Dasgupta, K. Mood recognition in online sessions using machine learning in realtime. In Proceedings of the 2021 5th International Conference on Computer, Communication and Signal Processing (ICCCSP), Chennai, India, 24–25 May 2021. [Google Scholar]
  62. Israelashvili, J.; Sauter, D.; Fischer, A. How well can we assess our ability to understand others’ feelings? Beliefs about taking others’ perspectives and actual understanding of others’ emotions. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Barrett, L.F.; Adolphs, R.; Marsella, S.; Martinez, A.M.; Pollak, S.D. Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2019, 20, 1–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Thomas, D.L.; Diener, E. Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 59, 291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Zillmann, D. Attribution and misattribution of excitatory reactions. In New Directions in Attribution Research; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2018; pp. 335–368. [Google Scholar]
  66. Barraza, J.A.; Alexander, V.; Beavin, L.E.; Terris, E.T.; Zak, P.J. The heart of the story: Peripheral physiology during narrative exposure predicts charitable giving. Biol. Psychol. 2015, 105, 138–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Zak, P.J.; Barraza, J.A. Measuring immersion in experiences with biosensors. In Proceedings of the 11th International Joint Conference on Biomedical Engineering Systems and Technologies, Funchal, Portugal, 19–21 January 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Zak, P.J. Neurological Correlates Allow Us to Predict Human Behavior. Available online: https://www.the-scientist.com/features/neurological-correlates-allow-us-to-predict-human-behavior-67948 (accessed on 3 August 2022).
  69. Barraza, J.; Zak, P. Empathy toward strangers triggers oxytocin release and subsequent generosity. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1167, 182–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Ježová, D.; Jurčovičová, J.; Vigaš, M.; Murgaš, K.; Labrie, F. Increase in plasma ACTH after dopaminergic stimulation in rats. Psychopharmacology 1985, 85, 201–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Nowack, K.; Zak, P.J. Sustain High Performance with Psychological Safety. Am. Soc. Train. Dev. Available online: https://www.dropbox.com/s/03rghulo3pe78pu/Sustain_High_Performance_With_Psychological_Safety.pdf?dl=0 (accessed on 3 August 2022).
  72. McNair, D.M.; Lorr, M.; Droppleman, L. Profile of Mood States Questionnaire; Educational and Industrial Testing Service: San Diego, CA, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  73. Curran, S.L.; Andrykowski, M.A.; Studts, J.L. Short form of the profile of mood states (POMS-SF): Psychometric information. Psychol. Assess. 1995, 7, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Christ, M.; Braun, N.; Neuffer, J.; Kempa-Liehr, A.W. Time Series Feature Extraction on basis of Scalable Hypothesis tests (tsfresh—A Python package). Neurocomputing 2018, 307, 72–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Chawla, N.V.; Bowyer, K.W.; Hall, L.O.; Kegelmeyer, W.P. SMOTE: Synthetic minority over-sampling technique. J. Artif. Intell. Res. 2002, 16, 321–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Maddala, G.S.; Wu, S. A Comparative Study of Unit Root Tests with Panel Data and a New Simple Test. Oxf. Bull. Econ. Stat. 1999, 61, 631–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Morawetz, C.; Riedel, M.C.; Salo, T.; Berboth, S.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Laird, A.R.; Kohn, N. Multiple large-scale neural networks underlying emotion regulation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020, 116, 382–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Baños, R.M.; Etchemendy, E.; Castilla, D.; García-Palacios, A.; Quero, S.; Botella, C. Positive mood induction procedures for virtual environments designed for elderly people. Interact. Comput. 2012, 24, 131–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Uysal, R. Social competence and psychological vulnerability: The mediating role of flourishing. Psychol. Rep. 2015, 117, 554–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Martino, J.; Pegg, J.; Frates, E.P. The connection prescription: Using the power of social interactions and the deep desire for connectedness to empower health and wellness. Am. J. Lifestyle Med. 2017, 11, 466–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Sun, J.; Harris, K.; Vazire, S. Is well-being associated with the quantity and quality of social interactions? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2020, 119, 1478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Zak, P.J. Immersion: The Science of the Extraordinary and Source of Happiness; Lioncrest Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  83. Hsu, H.-C. Trajectories and covariates of life satisfaction among older adults in Taiwan. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2012, 55, 210–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Ferring, D.; Balducci, C.; Burholt, V.; Wenger, C.; Thissen, F.; Weber, G.; Hallberg, I. Life satisfaction of older people in six european countries: Findings from the european study on adult well-being. Eur. J. Ageing Soc. Behav. Health Perspect. 2004, 1, 15–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Shannon, B.W. The role of psychological safety in human development. Res. Hum. Dev. 2016, 13, 6–14. [Google Scholar]
  86. Zhou, H.; Chen, J. How Does Psychological Empowerment Prevent Emotional Exhaustion? Psychological Safety and Organizational Embeddedness as Mediators. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 546687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Remtulla, R.; Hagana, A.; Houbby, N.; Ruparell, K.; Aojula, N.; Menon, A.; Meyer, E. Exploring the barriers and facilitators of psychological safety in primary care teams: A qualitative study. BMC Health Serv. Res. 2021, 21, 269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Frazier, M.L.; Fainshmidt, S.; Klinger, R.L.; Pezeshkan, A.; Vracheva, V. Psychological safety: A meta-analytic review and extension. Pers. Psychol. 2017, 70, 113–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Bowling, A.; Gabriel, Z.; Dykes, J.; Dowding, L.M.; Evans, O.; Fleissig, A.; Sutton, S. Let’s ask them: A national survey of definitions of quality of life and its enhancement among people aged 65 and over. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 2003, 56, 269–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Hansson, L. Quality of life in depr(logit: 65%; RF: 94%; SVM: 88%), ession and anxiety. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2002, 14, 185–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Meek, K.P.; Bergeron, C.D.; Towne, S.D.; Ahn, S.N.; Ory, M.G.; Smith, M.L. Restricted social engagement among adults living with chronic conditions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Simon, G.E. Treating depression in patients with chronic disease. West. J. Med. 2001, 175, 292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Godil, A.; Mallick, M.S.A.; Adam, A.M.; Haq, A.; Khetpal, A.; Afzal, R.; Shahid, N. Prevalence and severity of depression in a Pakistani population with at least one major chronic disease. J. Clin. Diagn. Res. JCDR 2017, 11, OC05. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Wróblewska, I.; Bartyzel, M.; Chmielowiec, B.; Puścion, M.; Chmielewski, J.P. The impact of depression on the quality of life in elderly people. Med. Ogólna I Nauk. O Zdrowiu 2021, 27, 199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Yohannes, A.; Baldwin, R.; Connolly, M. Mood disorders in elderly patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Rev. Clin. Gerontol. 2000, 10, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Bucks, R.S.; Gidron, Y.; Harris, P.; Teeling, J.; Wesnes, K.A.; Perry, V.H. Selective effects of upper respiratory tract infection on cognition, mood and emotion processing: A prospective study. Brain Behav. Immun. 2008, 22, 399–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Nesse, R.M. Is depression an adaptation? Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2000, 57, 14–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Lukaschek, K.; Vanajan, A.; Johar, H.; Weiland, N.; Ladwig, K.-H. “In the mood for ageing”: Determinants of subjective well-being in older men and women of the population-based kora-age study. BMC Geriatr. 2017, 17, 126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Okamura, T.; Ura, C.; Miyamae, F.; Sugiyama, M.; Inagaki, H.; Ayako, E.; Hiroshi, M.; Motokawa, K.; Awata, S. Prevalence of depressed mood and loss of interest among community-dwelling older people: Large-scale questionnaire survey and visiting intervention. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2018, 18, 1567–1572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The distribution of participants’ moods.
Figure 1. The distribution of participants’ moods.
Brainsci 12 01240 g001
Figure 2. Distribution of participants’ energy.
Figure 2. Distribution of participants’ energy.
Brainsci 12 01240 g002
Table 1. OLS estimation showed that Immersion and peak Immersion have positive effects on Mood. When predicting high and low Mood using a logit estimation, only peak Immersion was significant. Sick and Male are controls. Standard errors are in parenthesis and * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001.
Table 1. OLS estimation showed that Immersion and peak Immersion have positive effects on Mood. When predicting high and low Mood using a logit estimation, only peak Immersion was significant. Sick and Male are controls. Standard errors are in parenthesis and * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001.
VariableOLSVIFLogitOdds Ratio
Immersion0.403 *
(0.126)
1.601.19
(0.484)
3.296
PS−0.091
(0.081)
1.580.127
(0.301)
1.136
PI19.63 *
(6.52)
1.030.428 *
(0.192)
1.153
Sick−0.922 ***
(0.152)
1.02−3.889
(1.14)
0.020
Male
Intercept
0.285
(0.087)
3.068 *
(0.900)
1.091.687
(0.370)
−0.535
(3.139)
5.403
F-value12.54 Likelihood ratio χ255.42
p-value0.000 p-value0.000
R-squared 0.174 Pseudo R-squared0.134
Table 2. Energy is positively associated with Immersion and peak Immersion in an OLS estimation. Sick and Male are controls. A logit estimation for high and low Energy confirms that Immersion and peak Immersion are significant predictors. Standard errors are in parentheses and * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001.
Table 2. Energy is positively associated with Immersion and peak Immersion in an OLS estimation. Sick and Male are controls. A logit estimation for high and low Energy confirms that Immersion and peak Immersion are significant predictors. Standard errors are in parentheses and * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001.
VariableOLSVIFLogitOdds Ratio
Immersion0.499 *
(0.192)
1.601.134 *
(0.488)
3.11 *
PS0.122
(0.123)
1.580.507
(0.302)
1.66
PI33.95 **
(9.95)
1.030.504 **
(0.199)
1.66
Sick −0.594 **
(0.232)
1.02−1.733
(1.06)
0.177
Male0.159
(0.132)
1.09−0.504
(0.394)
0.604
Intercept0.764 *
(0.614)
−5.888 **
(1.593)
F-value7.04 Likelihood ratio χ237.88
p-value(0.000) p-value0.000
R-squared 0.106 Pseudo R-squared0.107
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Merritt, S.H.; Krouse, M.; Alogaily, R.S.; Zak, P.J. Continuous Neurophysiologic Data Accurately Predict Mood and Energy in the Elderly. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1240. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12091240

AMA Style

Merritt SH, Krouse M, Alogaily RS, Zak PJ. Continuous Neurophysiologic Data Accurately Predict Mood and Energy in the Elderly. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(9):1240. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12091240

Chicago/Turabian Style

Merritt, Sean H., Michael Krouse, Rana S. Alogaily, and Paul J. Zak. 2022. "Continuous Neurophysiologic Data Accurately Predict Mood and Energy in the Elderly" Brain Sciences 12, no. 9: 1240. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12091240

APA Style

Merritt, S. H., Krouse, M., Alogaily, R. S., & Zak, P. J. (2022). Continuous Neurophysiologic Data Accurately Predict Mood and Energy in the Elderly. Brain Sciences, 12(9), 1240. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12091240

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop