Gender Congruency Effects in Spanish: Behavioral Evidence from Noun Phrase Production
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Gender Congruency Effect
1.2. The Grammatical Gender Feature in Spanish
1.3. The Current Study: Native Noun Phrase Production in Spanish
Hypotheses
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.3. Design and Procedure
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Target Picture | Gender | Distractor Type | Title 3 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
G+S+ | G+S− | G−S+ | G−S− | ||
gato (cat) | Masculine | conejo (rabbit) | sol (sun) | rata (rat) | miel (honey) |
caballo (horse) | Masculine | burro (donkey) | sello (stamp) | mula (mule) | flor (flower) |
cuervo (crow) | Masculine | papagayo (parrot) | monte (mountain) | paloma (dove) | toalla (towel) |
guitarra (guitar) | Feminine | trompeta (trumpet) | isla (island) | violin (violin) | zapato (shoe) |
mesa (table) | Feminine | silla (chair) | araña (spider) | armario (closet) | gorro (hat) |
estantería (shelving) | Feminine | mecedora (rocking chair) | bandera (flag) | sofá (sofa) | cabello (hair) |
cama (bed) | Feminine | alacena (cupboard) | garra (claw) | sillón (armchair) | libro (book) |
cerdo (pig) | Masculine | perro (dog) | hielo (ice) | vaca (cow) | madera (wood) |
tigre (tiger) | Masculine | león (lion) | avión (airplane) | serpiente (snake) | oreja (ear) |
brazo (arm) | Masculine | pie (foot) | pan (bread) | pierna (leg) | arena (sand) |
boca (mouth) | Feminine | nariz (nose) | abeja (bee) | ojo (eye) | árbol (tree) |
tenedor (fork) | Masculine | cuchillo (knife) | bolso (handbag) | cuchara (spoon) | pintura (painting) |
tomate (tomato) | Masculine | chile (chili) | diente (tooth) | patata (potato) | piedra (stone) |
abrigo (coat) | Masculine | jersey (sweater) | huevo (egg) | camiseta (shirt) | estrella (star) |
manzana (apple) | Feminine | pera (pear) | bota (boot) | plátano (banana) | pescado (fish) |
autobús (bus) | Masculine | tren (train) | mono (monkey) | bicicleta (bike) | llave (key) |
dedo (finger) | Masculine | codo (elbow) | ratón (mouse) | palma (palm) | nave (ship) |
zorro (fox) | Masculine | lobo (wolf) | papel (paper) | hiena (hyena) | lengua (tongue) |
flecha (arrow) | Feminine | pistola (gun) | roca (rock) | puñal (dagger) | oso (bear) |
maíz (corn) | Masculine | trigo (wheat) | gallo (rooster) | soja (soy) | carta (letter) |
References
- Levelt WJ, M.; Roelofs, A.; Meyer, A.S. A Theory of Lexical Access in Speech Production. Behav. Brain Sci. 1999, 22, 1–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schriefers, H.; Teruel, E. Grammatical Gender in Noun Phrase Production: The Gender Interference Effect in German. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2000, 26, 1368–1377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heim, S.; Friederici, A.D.; Schiller, N.O.; Rüschemeyer, S.-A.; Amunts, K. The Determiner Congruency Effect in Language Production Investigated with Functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 928–940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schiller, N.O.; Costa, A. Different Selection Principles of Freestanding and Bound Morphemes in Language Production. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2006, 32, 1201–1207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schiller, N.O. Psycholinguistic Approaches to the Investigation of Grammatical Gender. In The Expression of Gender; Corbett, G.G., Ed.; De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2013; pp. 161–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schiller, N.O.; Caramazza, A. Grammatical Feature Selection in Noun Phrase Production: Evidence from German and Dutch. J. Mem. Lang. 2003, 48, 169–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bürki, A.; Sadat, J.; Dubarry, A.-S.; Alario, F.-X. Sequential Processing during Noun Phrase Production. Cognition 2016, 146, 90–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- La Heij, W.; Mak, P.; Sander, J.; Willeboordse, E. The Gender-Congruency Effect in Picture-Word Tasks. Psychol. Res. 1998, 61, 209–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Starreveld, P.; La Heij, W. Phonological Facilitation of Grammatical Gender Retrieval. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2004, 19, 677–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schiller, N.O.; Caramazza, A. Grammatical Gender Selection and the Representation of Morphemes: The Production of Dutch Diminutives. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2006, 21, 945–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa, A.; Sebastian-Galles, N.; Miozzo, M.; Caramazza, A. The Gender Congruity Effect: Evidence from Spanish and Catalan. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1999, 14, 381–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miozzo, M.; Caramazza, A. The Selection of Determiners in Noun Phrase Production. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1999, 25, 907–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miozzo, M.; Costa, A.; Caramazza, A. The Absence of a Gender Congruency Effect in Romance Languages: A Matter of Stimulus Onset Asynchrony? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2002, 28, 388–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caramazza, A.; Miozzo, M.; Costa, A.; Schiller, N.; Alario, F.-X. A Crosslinguistic Investigation of Determiner Production. In Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001; pp. 209–226. [Google Scholar]
- Alario, F.-X.; Caramazza, A. The Production of Determiners: Evidence from French. Cognition 2002, 82, 179–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sá-Leite, A.R.; Haro, J.; Comesaña, M.; Fraga, I. Of Beavers and Tables: The Role of Animacy in the Processing of Grammatical Gender Within a Picture-Word Interference Task. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 661175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopp, H. Learning (Not) to Predict: Grammatical Gender Processing in Second Language Acquisition. Second. Lang. Res. 2016, 32, 277–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paolieri, D.; Lotto, L.; Leoncini, D.; Cubelli, R.; Job, R. Differential Effects of Grammatical Gender and Gender Inflection in Bare Noun Production: Gender and Morphology in Picture Naming. Br. J. Psychol. 2011, 102, 19–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cubelli, R.; Lotto, L.; Paolieri, D.; Girelli, M.; Job, R. Grammatical Gender Is Selected in Bare Noun Production: Evidence from the Picture–Word Interference Paradigm. J. Mem. Lang. 2005, 53, 42–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Grebmer zu Wolfsthurn, S.; Pablos, L.; Schiller, N.O. Noun-Phrase Production as a Window to Language Selection: An ERP Study. Neuropsychologia 2021, 162, 108055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, M.; Schiller, N.O. A Review on Grammatical Gender Agreement in Speech Production. Front. Psychol. 2019, 9, 2754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sá-Leite, A.R.; Luna, K.; Tomaz, Â.; Fraga, I.; Comesaña, M. The Mechanisms Underlying Grammatical Gender Selection in Language Production: A Meta-Analysis of the Gender Congruency Effect. Cognition 2022, 224, 105060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bürki, A.; Van den Hoven, E.; Schiller, N.O.; Dimitrov, N. Cross-linguistic differences in gender congruency effects: Evidence from meta-analyses. J. Mem. Lang. In Press.
- Rosinski, R.R.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Kukish, K.S. Automatic Semantic Processing in a Picture-Word Interference Task. Child Dev. 1975, 46, 247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glaser, W.R. Picture Naming. Cognition 1992, 42, 61–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schriefers, H. Syntactic Processes in the Production of Noun Phrases. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1993, 19, 841–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paolieri, D.; Lotto, L.; Morales, L.; Bajo, T.; Cubelli, R.; Job, R. Grammatical Gender Processing in Romance Languages: Evidence from Bare Noun Production in Italian and Spanish. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2010, 22, 335–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finocchiaro, C.; Alario, F.-X.; Schiller, N.; Costa, A.; Miozzo, M.; Caramazza, A. Gender Congruency Goes Europe: A Cross-Linguistic Study of the Gender Congruency Effect in Romance and Germanic Languages. Ital. J. Linguist./Riv. Di Linguist. 2011, 23, 161–198. [Google Scholar]
- O’Rourke, P. The Gender Congruency Effect in Bare Noun Production in Spanish; Coyote Papers: Working Papers in Linguistics; University of Arizona Linguistics Circle: Tucson, AZ, USA, 2007; Volume 15, pp. 66–89. [Google Scholar]
- White, L.; Valenzuela, E.; Kozlowska–Macgregor, M.; Leung, Y.-K.I. Gender and Number Agreement in Nonnative Spanish. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2004, 25, 105–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bull, W.E. Spanish for Teachers: Applied Linguistics; Krieger Publishing Company: Malabar, FL, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Eddington, D. Spanish Gender Assignment in an Analogical Framework. J. Quant. Linguist. 2002, 9, 49–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beatty-Martínez, A.L.; Dussias, P.E. Revisiting Masculine and Feminine Grammatical Gender in Spanish: Linguistic, Psycholinguistic, and Neurolinguistic Evidence. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sá-Leite, A.R.; Luna, K.; Fraga, I.; Comesaña, M. The Gender Congruency Effect across Languages in Bilinguals: A Meta-Analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2020, 27, 677–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Teschner, R.V.; Russell, W.M. The Gender Patterns of Spanish Nouns: An Inverse Dictionary-Based Analysis. Hisp. Linguist. 1984, 1, 115–132. [Google Scholar]
- Grüter, T.; Lew-Williams, C.; Fernald, A. Grammatical Gender in L2: A Production or a Real-Time Processing Problem? Second Lang. Res. 2012, 28, 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harris, J.W. The Exponence of Gender in Spanish. Linguist. Inq. 1991, 22, 27–62. [Google Scholar]
- Roelofs, A. Goal-Referenced Selection of Verbal Action: Modeling Attentional Control in the Stroop Task. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schriefers, H.; Meyer, A.S.; Levelt, W.J. Exploring the Time Course of Lexical Access in Language Production: Picture-Word Interference Studies. J. Mem. Lang. 1990, 29, 86–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glaser, W.R.; Düngelhoff, F.-J. The Time Course of Picture-Word Interference. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1984, 10, 640–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Severens, E.; Lommel, S.V.; Ratinckx, E.; Hartsuiker, R.J. Timed Picture Naming Norms for 590 Pictures in Dutch. Acta Psychol. 2005, 119, 159–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davies, M. Corpus del Español: Two Billion Words, 21 Countries. 2016. Available online: https://www.corpusdelespanol.org/x.asp (accessed on 13 August 2021).
- Schneider, W.; Eschman, A.; Zuccolotto, A. E-Prime User’s Guide; Psychology Software Tools Inc.: Sharpsburg, PA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Van Casteren, M.; Davis, M.H. Mix, a Program for Pseudorandomization. Behav. Res. Methods 2006, 38, 584–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boersma, P.; Weenink, D. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer (6.0.49) [Computer Software]. 2019. Available online: https://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/ (accessed on 10 April 2022).
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 17 September 2022).
- Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, D.J. Random Effects Structure for Testing Interactions in Linear Mixed-Effects Models. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Akaike, H. A New Look at the Statistical Model Identification. IEEE Trans. Autom. Control. 1974, 19, 716–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neath, A.A.; Cavanaugh, J.E. The Bayesian Information Criterion: Background, Derivation, and Applications. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Comput. Stat. 2012, 4, 199–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- La Heij, W. Components of Stroop-like Interference in Picture Naming. Mem. Cogn. 1988, 16, 400–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Belke, E.; Meyer, A.S.; Damian, M.F. Refractory Effects in Picture Naming as Assessed in a Semantic Blocking Paradigm. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 2005, 58, 667–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levelt, W.J.M. Accessing Words in Speech Production: Stages, Processes and Representations. Cognition 1992, 42, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Real Academia Española: Diccionario de la Lengua Española, 23rd ed.; Dictionary of the Spanish Language: Madrid, Spain, 2014.
- Damian, M.F.; Martin, R.C. Semantic and Phonological Codes Interact in Single Word Production. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1999, 25, 345–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berger, A.; Kiefer, M. Comparison of Different Response Time Outlier Exclusion Methods: A Simulation Study. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 2194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linde-Domingo, J.; Treder, M.S.; Kerrén, C.; Wimber, M. Evidence that Neural Information Flow is Reversed between Object Perception and Object Reconstruction from Memory. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Target Picture PIG [CERDO] | Condition | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
G+S+ | G+S− | G−S+ | G−S− | |
Grammatical gender el (m.) | ||||
Distractors Grammatical gender of distractors | dog perro el (m.) | ice hielo el (m.) | cow vaca la (f.) | wood madera la (f.) |
Condition | Naming Latencies (ms) | |
---|---|---|
Mean | SD | |
Gender-congruent/Semantically Related (G+S+) | 714 | 194 |
Gender-congruent/Semantically Unrelated (G+S−) | 673 | 149 |
Gender-incongruent/Semantically Related (G−S+) | 725 | 184 |
Gender-incongruent/Semantically Unrelated (G−S−) | 691 | 165 |
Formula: naming latency ~ gender congruency (congruent vs. incongruent) + semantic relatedness (related vs. unrelated) + (semantic relatedness | participant) + (1 | item) | ||||
Predictors | RTs | |||
Estimate | 95% CI | Statistic | Pr(>|z|) | |
(Intercept) | 714.882 | 683.193–746.570 | 44.239 | <0.001 *** |
Gender [Congruent] | −13.526 | −22.634–−4.419 | −2.912 | 0.004 ** |
Semantic [Related] | 38.409 | 17.320 – 59.498 | 3.571 | <0.001 *** |
Random Effects | ||||
σ2 | 0.04 | |||
τ00 Participant | 2460.61 | |||
τ00 Item | 578.38 | |||
τ11 participant. SemanticRelated | 872.19 | |||
ρ01 participant | −0.20 | |||
ICC | 1.00 | |||
N participant | 30 | |||
N Item | 20 | |||
Observations | 2288 | |||
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 | 0.115/1.000 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wu, R.; Schiller, N.O. Gender Congruency Effects in Spanish: Behavioral Evidence from Noun Phrase Production. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 696. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040696
Wu R, Schiller NO. Gender Congruency Effects in Spanish: Behavioral Evidence from Noun Phrase Production. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(4):696. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040696
Chicago/Turabian StyleWu, Ruixue, and Niels O. Schiller. 2023. "Gender Congruency Effects in Spanish: Behavioral Evidence from Noun Phrase Production" Brain Sciences 13, no. 4: 696. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040696
APA StyleWu, R., & Schiller, N. O. (2023). Gender Congruency Effects in Spanish: Behavioral Evidence from Noun Phrase Production. Brain Sciences, 13(4), 696. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040696