Face Processing in Prematurely Born Individuals—A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
3. Results
3.1. Literature Search
3.2. Risk of Bias
3.2.1. Case–Control or Cohort Studies (n = 13)
3.2.2. Cross-Sectional Studies (n = 14)
3.3. General Study Characteristics
3.4. Behavioural Studies
3.4.1. Facial Identity and Facial Expression Processing Performance
3.4.2. Visual Attention Orientation Towards Faces and Facial Features
3.4.3. Key Findings of Behavioural Studies
3.5. Neuroimaging Studies
3.5.1. Studies Using (f)MRI
3.5.2. Studies Using EEG
3.5.3. Study Using fNIRS
Number of Stars | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Study, Year | Selection * | Comparability × | Outcome ° | Score ˘ |
Wocadlo and Rieger [38] | 3 | 1 | 3 | 7/9 |
Potharst et al. [46] | 3 | 1 | 3 | 7/9 |
Witt et al. [36] | 2 | 1 | 3 | 6/9 |
Perez-Roche et al. [44] | 3 | 2 | 3 | 8/9 |
Gao et al. [37] | 3 | 0 | 3 | 6/9 |
Mathewson et al. [43] | 3 | 0 | 2 | 5/9 |
Marotta et al. [35] | 3 | 1 | 2 | 6/9 |
Healy et al. [52] | 3 | 1 | 3 | 7/9 |
Carbajal-Valenzuela et al. [19] | 3 | 0 | 3 | 6/9 |
Amani et al. [56] | 3 | 0 | 2 | 5/9 |
Mossad et al. [60] | 2 | 0 | 3 | 5/9 |
Dean et al. [49] | 3 | 1 | 2 | 6/9 |
O’Reilly et al. [4] | 3 | 1 | 3 | 7/9 |
Total | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Study, Year | Yes * | No * | Unclear * | N. A. × | Overall Appraisal ° |
Williamson and Jakobson [39] | 7 | 1 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Pereira et al. [47] | 4 | 4 | 0 | 0 | Seek further info |
Twilhaar et al. [41] | 5 | 3 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Marleau et al. [40] | 8 | 0 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Pavlova et al. [42] | 5 | 3 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Bate et al. [45] | 6 | 2 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Telford et al. [48] | 6 | 2 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Yamamoto et al. [51] | 5 | 2 | 1 | 0 | Include |
Papini et al. [54] | 8 | 0 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Frie et al. [57] | 5 | 3 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Urbain et al. [59] | 8 | 0 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Sato et al. [61] | 6 | 2 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Grannis et al. [53] | 8 | 0 | 0 | 0 | Include |
Berdasco-Muñoz et al. [50] | 6 | 2 | 0 | 0 | Include |
3.5.4. Studies Using MEG
3.5.5. Key Findings of Neuroimaging Studies
4. Discussion
4.1. Preterm Individuals’ Face Processing Abilities Throughout Development
4.2. Structural, Functional, and Behavioural Face Processing Alterations in Preterm Individuals
4.3. Birth Weight and Not Gestational Age Might Be a Better Predictor for Atypical Face Processing
4.4. Challenges, Limitations, and Opportunities in Preterm Face Processing Research
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Overview of Used Search Terms per Database
Database | Term 1: PT-born Children | Term 2: Face Processing |
---|---|---|
PubMed | “infant, premature” [mesh] OR “prematurity” [tiab] OR “premature infant*“ [tiab] OR “pre-mature infant” [tiab] OR “pre-term baby” [tiab] OR “pre-term child” [tiab] OR “pre-term newborn” [tiab] OR “pre-term neonate” [tiab] OR “pre-term infant” [tiab] OR “pre-term newborn” [tiab] OR “preterm infant*” [tiab] OR “preterm baby” [tiab] OR “preterm child” [tiab] OR “neonatal prematurity” [tiab] OR “extremely premature infant*” [tiab] OR “extremely preterm infant*” [tiab] or “preterm” [tiab] OR “premature birth” [mesh] OR “premature birth*” [tiab] OR “preterm birth*” [tiab] OR “infant, low birth weight” [mesh] OR “low birth weight*” [tiab] OR “low-birth-weight” [tiab] OR “Small for Gestational Age” [tiab] OR “Very low birth weight” [tiab] OR “Extremely low birth weight” [tiab] OR “birth weight” [tiab] OR “neonatal underweight” [tiab] OR “premature” [tiab] OR “premature baby” [tiab] OR “premature child” [tiab] OR “premature infant” [tiab] OR “premature neonate” [tiab] OR “premature newborn” [tiab] OR “SGA neonate” [tiab] OR “small for age infant” [tiab] | “facial recognition” [mesh] OR “face recognition” [tiab] OR “face perception” [tiab] OR “face processing” [tiab] OR “facial identity recognition” [tiab] OR “facial emotion recognition” [tiab] OR “facial expression recognition” [tiab] OR “face emotion perception” [tiab] OR “face emotion recognition” [tiab] OR “emotion recognition” [tiab] OR “emotional regulation” [tiab] |
Embase | ‘prematurity’/exp OR ‘prematurity’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘extremely premature infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘pre-mature infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘pre-term baby’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘pre-term child’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘pre-term infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘pre-term newborn’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘pre-term neonate’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘premature’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘premature baby’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘premature birth’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘Premature child’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘premature infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘premature neonate’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘premature newborn’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘preterm baby’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘preterm child’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘preterm infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘preterm neonate’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘preterm newborn’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘low birth weight’/exp OR ‘low birth weight’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘birth weight, low’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘infant, low birth weight’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘LBW infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘LBW neonate’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘LBW newborn’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘low birth weight infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘low birth weight’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘neonatal underweight’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘small for date infant’/exp OR ‘small for date infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘infant, small for date’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘infant, small for gestational age’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘SGA infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘SGA neonate’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘SGA newborn’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘small for age infant’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘small for date baby’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘small for gestational age’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘small for gestational age infant’:ti,ab,kw | ‘facial recognition’/exp OR ‘face recognition’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘face perception’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘face processing’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘facial identity recognition’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘facial emotion recognition’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘face emotion recognition’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘facial expression recognition’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘face processing’/exp OR ‘face processing’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘emotion recognition’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘face emotion perception’:ti,ab,kw OR ‘emotional regulation’:ti,ab,kw |
Web of Science (Core collection) | TS = (“prematurity” OR “premature infant*” OR “pre-mature infant” OR “pre-term baby” OR “pre-term child” OR “pre-term newborn” OR “pre-term neonate” OR “preterm infant*” OR “neonatal prematurity” OR “extremely premature infant*” OR “extremely preterm infant*” or “preterm” OR “premature birth” [mesh] OR “premature birth*” OR “preterm birth*” OR “low birth weight*” OR “low-birth-weight” OR “Small for Gestational Age” OR “Very low birth weight” OR “Extremely low birth weight” OR “birth weight” OR “premature” OR “preterm”) | TS = (“face recognition” OR “face perception” OR “face processing” OR “facial identity recognition” OR “facial emotion recognition” OR “facial expression recognition” OR “face emotion perception” OR “face emotion recognition” OR “emotion recognition” OR “emotional regulation”) |
Appendix B. Chronologically Ordered Summary Overview of the Main Characteristics and Findings of Each Study in the Final Selection of Articles (n = 24)
Publication | Study Population | Method | Results | Conclusion or Interpretation | Limitations
Mentioned in Article |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wocadlo and Rieger [38] | 112 VPT children (age 8 y) born < 30 weeks with VLBW. Exclusion criteria: congenital abnormality, neurosensory impairment (NSI), visual or hearing impairment, IQ < 76. | Social Skills Rating System, Receptive Faces subtest (to evaluate the decoding of emotion). | 38 children performed below the average of the total study population, 74 above average.Children with below-average social skills showed significantly more errors in the receptive faces subtest, especially with negative emotions. | Errors in decoding facial emotion appears to be one factor that may lead to poorer acquisition of social skills, especially assertion (initiation of social interaction such as making friends) and responsibility (behaviours reflecting the ability to communicate such as intruding oneself, refusing requests politely, etc.) | No FT control group; missing data of the family questionnaires and the teacher report forms by which the number of below-average may be underestimated. |
Potharst et al. [46] | 102 VPT children (age 5 y) born < 30 weeks and/or <1000 g without a genetic syndrome or a disability. 95 FT controls (age-matched) born ≥ 37 weeks and > 2500 g. | Face recognition task and facial expressions task of the Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks battery. | The PT group made significantly more errors in face and emotion recognition after adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics. | Differences were found between the two groups for face and emotion recognition, suggesting this is not limited to children with periventricular leukomalacia (PVL). | Non-blinded assessment; substantial number of missing values; no individual FT control for every PT child. |
Healy et al. [52] | 73 VPT adolescents (age 14–16 y) born < 33 weeks with VLBW. 49 FT controls (age 14–16 y) born 38–42 weeks, >2500 g, with no history of neurologic conditions, intracranial infection, or head injury. | MRI after identifying the socially immature (SI) cases (VPT n = 37; FT n = 9) using the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) with social problem scores at or over the 90th percentile of scores of FT controls. | The SI PT subgroup shows significantly increased grey matter volume in bilateral FG, which was also associated with greater volume of the left orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) more strongly in the SI group. | A larger volume of the FG and the OFC may be precursors of, or consequences of, subclinical psychiatric problems such as socialization difficulties. | Small sample size; low statistical power; use of the narrow-band social problem scale; noncausal association. |
Witt et al. [36] | 41 VPT toddlers (age 2 y) born < 29 weeks with ELBW without major brain lesions or mental delay. 47 FT controls (matched on chronological age) born > 37 weeks. | Affect recognition: free labelling (“how does mummy feel?”) and TOM (“pick an expression that matches the emotional context”). | PT children were less accurate in naming facial expressions, but there was no difference when asked to choose the facial expression that best matched the given context. | Contextual information might help to understand emotional scenes. However, these results still suggest the presence of various emotional difficulties in PT children. | Small sample size; TOM test is potentially not sensitive enough. |
Williamson and Jakobson [39] | 34 VPT children (age 8–11 y) born < 33 weeks with VLBW. 36 FT children (age 8–11 y) born within 2 weeks of due date, at an appropriate size for their GA and without medical complications. | Child and Adolescent Social Perception Measure (after viewing videotaped audio-filtered scenes, subjects are asked to describe what happened and what the characters felt based upon face, body, and situational cues such as smiling, hand gestures, an angry mother, etc.). | FT children correctly identified significantly more face, body, and situational cues than PT children. PT children had difficulty interpreting emotions. These findings raise the possibility that PT children have social difficulties because they rely too heavily on situational cues. | Children born prematurely at VLBW experience difficulties in decoding the emotions of individuals, which arises primarily from problems processing nonverbal cues from moving faces and bodies. | Small sample with potential selection bias; over-representation of children with higher socio-economic status (SES); the specific selection of research instruments in this study to estimate verbal IQ and “autistic-like” traits instead of a complete comprehensive, diagnostic work-up. |
Papini et al. [54] | 36 VPT adults (age 37–42 y) born < 33 weeks with VLBW without any history of neurological conditions including meningitis, head injury and cerebral infections. 38 FT controls (age-matched) born at 38–42 weeks and >2500 g without birth complications and neurological conditions. | Resting-state fMRI, which was correlated with a facial emotion recognition task (ERT) in which participants receive a total score per emotion reflecting the number of correct recognitions of that emotion. | VPT adults had intact but slower facial emotion recognition ability. In FT controls, total scores for recognizing the angry emotion were significantly associated with resting-state functional connectivity between left superficial amygdala and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), whereas in PT adults this association was not significant. Yet, the association was not significantly different between the groups. | Functional connectivity between PCC and left superficial amygdala was significantly associated with the ability to recognize angry faces in FT but not in VPT adults. This points towards a potential substrate for selective ERT impairments. Increased amygdala–STS connectivity found in VPT adults could represent a compensatory strategy in a suboptimal emotional network. | MRI image distortion and signal dropout at amygdala; subtle impairments in emotion recognition in relation to amygdala connectivity could not be examined; difference in SES between groups. |
Frie et al. [57] | 27 EPT infants (mean corrected age (CA) of 8 months) born < 28 weeks with ELBW without PVL, other major brain abnormalities or retinopathy of prematurity. 26 FT controls (mean age of 8 months) born at 35–42 weeks with NBW. | Functional NIRS (fNIRS) while presenting faces of the mother versus a stranger. PT only: conventional MRI (T1 and T2). | FT infants showed higher hemodynamic response to their mother’s faces, whereas PT infants had no preference. MRI in the PT group showed negative correlation between oxygenated haemoglobin (HbO2) and regional grey matter volumes in the FG and amygdala when presented with unknown faces but no correlation when presented with the mothers’ faces. | EPT infants exhibit differences in functional cortical activation compared with term-born infants. The lower volume could indicate atrophy of the FG and amygdala, suggesting these areas might need an additional amount of HbO2. The negative correlation might represent a compensatory mechanism for structural alterations. | fNIRS could only cover the right hemisphere and only one optode was placed over the temporal lobe; artefacts were treated manually. |
Telford et al. [48] | 50 VPT infants (8 months CA) born < 33 weeks with VLBW. 50 FT controls (8 months old) born ≥ 37 weeks with NBW. Exclusion criteria: major congenital malformations, chromosomal abnormalities, congenital infections, major overt parenchymal lesions, post-haemorrhagic ventricular dilatation. | Eye tracking using Tobii x60 while performing 3 preferential looking tasks: (i) face scanning (eyes vs. mouth), (ii) face pop-out (pictures of a face among other images), (iii) social preferential looking (one picture displaying people versus one without people). | Compared to PT infants, FT infants had greater preference for looking at eyes than mouth; had longer looking time to the face (versus bird, tree, …); preferred looking at pictures featuring children. | Social orienting in late infancy differs between PT and FT individuals. The development of social cognition may be altered by PT birth. | Analysis of the pop-out task involves five simultaneous tests without Bonferroni correction, which raises the possibility that a shorter looking time in PT infants is a false positive. However, this is unlikely because of the large effect size and the consistency with the results of the other tasks. Bonferroni was not applied as test statistics are known to be correlated in this task. |
Pereira et al. [47] | 28 PT infants (48 h old) born at 33–36 weeks with LBW. 31 FT controls (48 h old) born at 37–41 weeks with NBW. Exclusion criteria: hemodynamically unstable newborns, intraventricular haemorrhage, Apgar score at 5 min < 5, abnormal fundoscopic examination. | Visual preference for faces using whiteboards shaped as a head and neck (one normal and one abnormal). | PT infants showed a lower occurrence of orientation movements and did not show a preference, whereas FT infants preferred the natural faces and made more orientations movements. | PT infants do not show a visual preference for natural faces, possibly because of an incomplete development. | The response to the natural face stimulus could not be filmed; small sample size. |
Carbajal-Valenzuela et al. [19] | 20 PT infants (8 months CA old) born < 35 weeks with LBW without risk for perinatal brain injury (PBI) or abnormal neurological examination. 15 FT controls (8 months old) born > 37 weeks and >2.5 kg, normal ophthalmological and neurological examination. | Continuous EEG at 4 and 8 months during an emotional face processing task. | At 8 months, FT infants had higher absolute power in occipital regions in almost all frequency bands for the positive face condition compared to the neutral face and non-face conditions, while PT infants had higher absolute power in frontal regions at high frequencies (10 Hz and higher) during all conditions. | The higher occipital power in the FTs reflects an allocation of sensorial and perceptual resources to process emotional faces, whereas the higher frontal power in the PTs could indicate an attention allocation that lacks the resources to improve face processing. | The use of scalp EEG for the localisation of abnormalities; small sample size; the anatomical difference between PT and FT was not considered. |
Perez-Roche et al. [44] | 31 PT children (age 5–15 y) born < 37 weeks with LBW, of which 19 were born < 32 weeks. 41 FT controls (age 5–15 y) born ≥ 37 weeks with NBW. Exclusion criteria: neurological impairments or diseases, congenital malformation, or ophthalmologic diseases. | Facial memory subtest of Test of Memory and Learning (TOMAL): recognizing and identifying photographs of faces. | The PT group had poorer immediate face recognition, but similar face memory. After adjusting for BW, prematurity was not associated with TOMAL outcomes, face recognition nor face memory. | Children born SGA are at increased risk of face recognition impairment and delayed face memory, both in early and late childhood, independently of their GA at birth. | The results lack a complete cognitive assessment. |
Gao et al. [37] | 64 ELBW adults (age 30–35 y) born at 23–34 weeks. Exclusion criteria: NSI, severe child-onset psychiatric problems, non-right-handed. 82 FT controls (mean age 32.5 y, standard deviation (SD) 1.4) born with NBW. | Facial expression emotion task: linking emotion icons to photographs. | There was no difference concerning threshold to detect emotions, but for angry faces, the ELBW group had a higher misidentification rate and misidentified more of them as fear. | ELBW adults were more likely to report another negatively valued facial expression (i.e., anger) while showing fear, but this study cannot explain the underlying mechanism. | Tests only occurred at a single time point in adulthood; no sad expressions were examined; the current sample was born in the 1980s, while neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) has improved in the meantime. |
Urbain et al. [59] | 19 VPT children (age 7–13 y) born < 32 weeks. 22 FT controls (age-matched). Exclusion criteria: any serious neurological sequelae, uncorrected hearing, or visual impairments, psychiatric or learning disorder, or standard contraindications for MRI. | MEG during emotional go/no-go tasks. Participants were instructed to press a button after the “go” stimuli and withhold after the “no-go” stimuli, identified by a coloured frame around happy or sad faces. They were told to ignore the faces, which allowed researchers to investigate the implicit effect of emotional context on inhibition processing. MRI (T1) | MEG: VPT showed reduced brain activity across a right lateralized parieto–frontal–temporal network. MRI: VPT showed reduced cortical thickness and cortical volume in the postcentral gyrus, the left praecuneus, bilaterally in the middle and inferior lateral and orbitofrontal regions, and bilaterally in the middle temporal and parahippocampal gyri. | Reduced cortical volume, thickness, and functional activity encompassing the right angular gyrus and prefrontal areas might be a biological marker of less efficient emotion regulation processes/performance in school-aged children born VPT. | The authors did not mention any limitations. |
Twilhaar et al. [41] | 61 VPT adolescents (age 13 y) born < 32 weeks with VLBW. 61 FT controls (age-matched) born ≥ 37 weeks and free of developmental, behavioural, or learning disorders. | Facial emotion recognition: a modified version of the morphed facial ERT. Cognitive control: (i) a spatial span task (reproducing a spatial sequence of dots), (ii) the sustained attention to response task (SART—go/no tasks using digits), (iii) the anti-saccade task (making eye movements in the opposite direction of a stimulus after fixating on a central cross). | VPT adolescents showed poorer emotion recognition abilities, especially for facial expressions of sadness. The VPT group also had an impaired visuospatial working memory span, more commission errors on the SART, and a greater proportion of erroneous saccades. | This study suggests that disturbances at later stages of social information processing (e.g., cognitive control) contribute to an important extent to the social problems of PT adolescents, whereas emotion recognition deficits do not seem to be one of the fundamental mechanisms behind social adjustment in PT adolescents. | A considerable proportion of the sample was excluded from the analysis of the facial emotion recognition task because they did not have correct trials in the lowest intensity levels of the emotions; small sample size to investigate more complex model including other factors such as family characteristics. |
Berdasco-Muñoz et al. [50] | 22 PT infants at age 8 months born < 37 weeks. 24 FT infants at age 6 months and 24 FT infants at age 8 months. Exclusion criteria: hearing or major health problems (e.g., brain injury, long retinopathy, or surgical intervention). | Eye tracking using Tobii x60 while viewing video clips of an actor reciting a children’s story in French and English to evaluate whether they looked more at eyes vs. mouth. | All infants tended to look globally more towards the eyes than the mouth. While PTs did not show a difference in the eyes-mouth index between native and non-native languages, FTs at both ages tended to globally look more towards the eyes than the mouth for the native language compared to the non-native language. | Global preference for the eyes over the mouth for all groups. Similar scanning patterns for the native and non-native language in PTs, while FTs looked more at the eyes than the mouth of a talking face for the native language compared to the non-native one. | The authors did not mention any limitations. |
Mathewson et al. [43] | 62 ELBW adults (age 22–26 y) born at 23–36 weeks (of which 22 were classified as SGA and 7 had NSI). 82 FT controls (age-matched) with NBW (of which one had NSI). | Visual discrimination task testing feature spacing using human faces, monkey faces, and houses. After seeing a test image, participants were asked to select the image with the same feature spacing. | ELBW adults made fewer correct responses than NBW controls. NBWs discriminated different feature spacing in human faces and monkey faces more accurately (and in houses as well to a lesser extent) compared to ELBWs. | ELBW adults were less proficient in using feature spacing to discriminate between human faces and between monkey faces. Together with marginally poorer discrimination of houses in the ELBW group, this study suggests that PT could affect a general perceptual mechanism. | Attrition of the cohort at this age; the time span between exposure (preterm birth) of interest and the measurement of adult outcomes; ELBW possessed several a priori disadvantages. |
Dean et al. [49] | 81 VPT infants born < 33 weeks with VLBW at age 7–9 months CA and 45 children at 5 y. 66 FT controls born ≥ 37 weeks with NBW at age 7–9 months and 35 controls at 5 y. Exclusion criteria: major congenital malformation, congenital infection, parenchymal brain injury (defined as cystic periventricular leukomalacia, haemorrhagic parenchymal infarction, and post-haemorrhagic ventricular dilatation). | Eye tracking using Tobii x60 while performing 3 preferential looking tasks (cf. Telford et al. [48]): (i) face scanning (eyes vs. mouth), (ii) face pop-out (pictures of a face among other images), and (iii) social preferential looking (one picture displaying people versus one without people). | PT infants spent less time looking at socially informative areas of interest in all three tasks (i.e., eyes, face and social scene), cf. Telford et al. [48]. No significant differences between proportional looking scores on any task were found at 5 years. Significant increases in proportional looking scores for the face and social preferential looking tasks were found in PT individuals between 7–9 months and 5 years. | PT infants have reduced social attentional preference in association at 7–9 months compared with term-born controls, but catch up and show equivalent social attentional preference by 5 years of age. | Maternal mental health and maternal cognition were not investigated; insufficient power to investigate impact of specific comorbidities of PT birth; the pop-out task was developed for use in infants. |
Mossad et al. [60] | 40 VPT children (age 6–8 y) born at ≤ 32 weeks with VLBW. 38 FT children (age 6 y) and 43 FT children (age 8 y) controls born ≥ 37 weeks with NBW. | MEG while participants focussed on a fixation cross and indicated by button press where a jumbled unidentifiable image was situated, while another image of an emotion was presented. MRI (T1) to localise network hubs. | At eight years, the PT children rated angry faces less negatively and showed reduced brain connectivity in theta, gamma, and alpha band networks across happy, angry, and fearful emotions. However, at six years, there was an increased connectivity in response to angry faces. | The core emotional face processing regions in FT children are part of a theta band network, with a right lateralisation for negative emotions. PT children showed reduced connectivity in that theta band between several regions. The change from increased to reduced connectivity in response to angry faces points to a dynamic period of brain network development. | The authors did not mention any limitations. |
Marotta et al. [35] | 53 LPT children (age 5–11 y) born at 34–36 weeks without neonatal compromise (i.e., anoxia or respiratory distress), some with LBW. 53 FT controls (age-matched) born ≥ 37 weeks with NBW. | ERT and Understanding of Social Situations test. | The two groups differed significantly with respect to emotion recognition (PTs score lower), but not with understanding of social situations. | LPT children need more contextual cues to interpret the social situation. | Subject to recall bias; no parents’ reports available; lack of info on risk factors for social and emotional difficulties; moderate size effect. |
Amani et al. [56] | 34 PT adults (age 30–35 y) born with ELBW and a mean GA of 27.13 (SD 2.42). 47 controls (age-matched) with NBW. | Resting-state frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA—greater relative right FAA is associated with a predisposition to experience and express avoidance and withdrawal behaviours) using EEG at 22–26 y. Threat-related attention biases were measured at 30–35 y using a dot probe task, where participants had to locate a dot on a screen while images with different facial emotions were projected. | Greater relative right frontal alpha EEG activity at rest at 22–26 y predicted avoidance of angry faces at 30–35 y in the ELBW participants, but not in the NBW participants. No significant associations were found for the attention bias to happy faces. | Early stress because of being prematurely born with ELBW may contribute to physiological changes that manifest in greater relative right FAA at rest, and this may be associated with stronger threat detection and anxious affectivity. | Attrition; EEG measures only the electrical activity at the scalp; the adult survivors of this study were born in the early era of NICU. |
O’Reilly et al. [4] | 129 EPT adults (age 19 y) born < 26 weeks with ELBW. 65 FT adults (age 19 y). | Facial emotion recognition with the Frankfurt Test and Training of Facial Affect Recognition 2nd Edition. | EPT adults had significantly lower scores in total (across all emotions) and for disgusted faces in the emotion recognition task compared to FT adults. | EPT adults show significant impairment in overall emotion recognition compared with FT controls. | In the larger framework of the longitudinal study, over 50% of participants was lost to follow-up until this age of 19 y. |
Yamamoto et al. [51] | 20 LBW infants (age 9–10 months CA) born at 23.4–36.9 weeks. 20 FT NBW controls (age 9–10 months), born at 37.0–41.6 weeks. Exclusion criteria: neurological abnormalities. | Eye tracking using Tobii T60 and photographs of faces, to evaluate whether they prefer known faces (after a familiarization phase) or unfamiliar faces. | Gaze time and eye gaze rate were lower in LBW infants during familiarization. LBW infants also shifted their attention less frequently. This shows that both attentional processes and face perception differ in several aspects between the LBW group and controls. | These findings may indicate that the LBW group does not look to the eyes for maintenance of attention but look to the eyes for orientation of attention. | Small sample size; development could not be investigated over time. |
Marleau et al. [40] | 30 EPT children (age 4–5 y) born at 22–28 weeks with ELBW. 30 FT children (age 4–5 y) born ≥ 37 weeks with NBW. Exclusion criteria: chromosomal abnormality, major congenital malformation, diagnosed intellectual disability, ASD, global developmental delay, any genetic, psychiatric, or metabolic condition, intraventricular grade IV haemorrhage, cystic PVL, Sarnat grade 2–3 hypoxic ischemic encephalopathy, deafness, visual impairment, acquired brain injury. | TOM: Wellman and Liu’s assessment battery consisting of questions and tasks to assess the individual’s TOM competencies. Affect recognition task: participants have to evaluate whether two facial expressions are similar or different. | The PT group performed poorly on TOM. A similar pattern was observed for affect recognition test with poorer scores in the PT group after controlling for confounds (parental education level, age at assessment, IQ, attentional and executive functioning). | PT birth is associated with difficulties in early basic social cognitive skills, namely TOM and facial emotion recognition. | Sample size and homogeneity; SES. |
Sato et al. [61] | 28 VPT children (age 5 y) born with VLBW and a mean GA of 27.9 (SD 2.0). 25 FT controls (age 5 y) born > 37 weeks with NBW. Exclusion criteria: neurological or neurodevelopmental disorder, chromosomal or major congenital abnormality, uncorrected hearing or visual impairment, or colour blindness. | MEG during an implicit emotional faces task where happy or angry face stimuli, a car stimulus and fixation crosses as inter-stimulus intervals were presented, but participants only had to react when a car stimulus appeared. Facial affect recognition subtest of the Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment and the Behaviour Assessment for Children to evaluate social and emotional functioning. MRI (T1) to localise network hubs. | In VLBW compared to FT children, reduced theta connectivity during angry face processing was found in a network anchored in left orbitofrontal and parietal regions, including hubs in the middle temporal lobe, calcarine and caudate gyri, and both left and right FG. | Reduced theta-band phase synchrony during face processing in VLBW, caused by a hypo-connected network for processing angry faces, indicates an atypical recruitment of emotional face processing networks. Hypo-connectivity between the calcarine and the FG suggest a vulnerability of the latter functionally. | The author did not mention any limitations. |
Bate et al. [45] | 103 Caucasian adults (age 18–33 y) born 25–42 weeks with varying BW without any history of neurological, intellectual, developmental, or psychiatric disorder. | Cambridge Face Memory Test and Cambridge Face Perception Test. | Both face recognition measures correlated with BW and birth weight-for-gestations centile scores, but not with GA alone. | The critical factor influencing face recognition ability is not prematurity per se but BW, with little to gain by correcting this measure for GA. | Despite explicit exclusion of individuals with visual problems, infants at low BW or gestation may have experienced less interaction with faces in their first weeks of life. |
Pavlova et al. [42] | 14 VPT adolescents (age 10–16 y) born at 26–32 weeks with LBW and signs of PVL and neurologically exhibited signs of cerebral palsy. 16 TD controls (age 11–17 y). | Face-n-Food task(a set of images in the predetermined order from the least to most resembling a face). | PT adolescents had a later face impression and gave overall much fewer face responses when compared with TD controls. | The face sensitivity is substantially lower in PT adolescents than the TD controls and appears to stem from deficits in face tuning (i.e., the sensitivity to a coarse face schema). | It left possible sex differences beyond attention. |
Grannis et al. [53] | 22 EPT adolescents (age 11–16 y) born with ELBW with normal or corrected vision and without neuro-developmental disorders. 24 FT controls born ≥ 37 weeks (age 11–16 y). | sMRI, diffusion MRI, and fMRI during a face processing task. | EPT adolescents showed greater neural activation in right fusiform area in response to faces. FT showed greater grey matter density within the right FG. Functional connectivity between the FG and the prefrontal cortex was non-significantly greater in FT. | Multimodal analyses revealed differences in structure, function, and connectivity that accurately distinguish EPT from FT youth. The findings suggest a compensatory role of the fusiform where less dense grey matter is countered by increased local blood-oxygen-level-dependant (BOLD) signal. | Small sample size; cross-sectional design; the use of neutral expression faces compared to a fixation cross. |
References
- Preterm Birth. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth#cms (accessed on 1 June 2022).
- Cutland, C.L.; Lackritz, E.M.; Mallett-Moore, T.; Bardají, A.; Chandrasekaran, R.; Lahariya, C.; Nisar, M.I.; Tapia, M.D.; Pathirana, J.; Kochhar, S.; et al. Brighton Collaboration Low Birth Weight Working Group. Low birth weight: Case definition guidelines for data collection, analysis, and presentation of maternal immunization safety data. Vaccine 2017, 35 Pt A. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, S.; Marlow, N. Early and long-term outcome of infants born extremely preterm. Arch. Dis. Child. 2017, 102, 97–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- O’Reilly, H.; Ni, Y.; Johnson, S.; Wolke, D.; Marlow, N. Extremely preterm birth and autistic traits in young adulthood: The EPICure study. Mol. Autism 2021, 12, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van de Weijer-Bergsma, E.; Wijnroks, L.; Jongmans, M.J. Attention development in infants and preschool children born preterm: A review. Infant Behav. Dev. 2008, 31, 333–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Crump, C.; Sundquist, J.; Sundquist, K. Preterm or Early Term Birth and Risk of Autism. Pediatrics 2021, 148, e2020032300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitzallen, G.C.; Taylor, H.G.; Bora, S. What Do We Know About the Preterm Behavioral Phenotype? A Narrative Review. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, S.; Marlow, N. Preterm birth and childhood psychiatric disorders. Pediatr. Res. 2011, 69 Pt 2, 11R–18R. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montagna, A.; Nosarti, C. Socio-Emotional Development Following Very Preterm Birth: Pathways to Psychopathology. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fenoglio, A.; Georgieff, M.K.; Elison, J.T. Social brain circuitry and social cognition in infants born preterm. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2017, 9, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haxby, J.V.; Hoffman, E.A.; Gobbini, M.I. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cognit. Sci. 2000, 4, 223–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rogers, C.E.; Lean, R.E.; Wheelock, M.D.; Smyser, C.D. Aberrant structural and functional connectivity and neurodevelopmental impairment in preterm children. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2018, 10, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mossad, S.I.; Vandewouw, M.M.; Smith, M.L.; Taylor, M.J. The preterm social brain: Altered functional networks for Theory of Mind in very preterm children. Brain Commun. 2021, 3, fcaa237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, Q.; Zhu, G.P.; Yi, L.; Cui, X.X.; Wang, H.; Wei, R.Y.; Hu, B.L. A Review of Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Studies of Motor and Cognitive Function in Preterm Infants. Neurosci. Bull. 2020, 36, 321–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gobbini, M.I.; Haxby, J.V. Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 32–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, M.H.; Griffin, R.; Csibra, G.; Halit, H.; Farroni, T.; de Haan, M.; Tucker, L.A.; Baron–Cohen, S.; Richards, J. The emergence of the social brain network: Evidence from typical and atypical development. Dev. Psychopathol. 2005, 17, 599–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haist, F.; Anzures, G. Functional development of the brain’s face-processing system. Wires Cognit. Sci. 2017, 8, 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Joseph, J.E.; DiBartolo, M.D.; Bhatt, R.S. Developmental changes in analytic and holistic processes in face perception. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carbajal-Valenzuela, C.C.; Santiago-Rodríguez, E.; Quirarte, G.L.; Harmony, T. Development of Emotional Face Processing in Premature and Full-Term Infants. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 2017, 48, 88–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samaey, C.; Van der Donck, S.; Lecei, A.; Vettori, S.; Qiao, Z.; van Winkel, R.; Boets, B. Between faces: Childhood adversity is associated with reduced threat-safety discrimination during facial expression processing in adolescence. BMC Med. 2024, 22, 382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nomi, J.S.; Uddin, L.Q. Face processing in autism spectrum disorders: From brain regions to brain networks. Neuropsychologia 2015, 71, 201–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campatelli, G.; Federico, R.R.; Apicella, F.; Sicca, F.; Muratori, F. Face Processing in Children with ASD: Literature Review. Res. Autism Spectr. Dis. 2013, 7, 444–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samaey, C.; Van der Donck, S.; van Winkel, R.; Boets, B. Facial Expression Processing Across the Autism-Psychosis Spectra: A Review of Neural Findings and Associations with Adverse Childhood Events. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 592937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van der Donck, S.; Dzhelyova, M.; Vettori, S.; Mahdi, S.S.; Claes, P.; Steyaert, J.; Boets, B. Rapid neural categorization of angry and fearful faces is specifically impaired in boys with autism spectrum disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2020, 61, 1019–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vettori, S.; Dzhelyova, M.; Van der Donck, S.; Jacques, C.; Steyaert, J.; Rossion, B.; Boets, B. Frequency-Tagging Electroencephalography of Superimposed Social and Non-Social Visual Stimulation Streams Reveals Reduced Saliency of Faces in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weigelt, S.; Koldewyn, K.; Kanwisher, N. Face identity recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 1060–1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weigelt, S.; Koldewyn, K.; Kanwisher, N. Face recognition deficits in autism spectrum disorders are both domain specific and process specific. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e74541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dean, B.; Ginnell, L.; Boardman, J.P.; Fletcher-Watson, S. Social cognition following preterm birth: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021, 124, 151–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burstein, O.; Zevin, Z.; Geva, R. Preterm Birth and the Development of Visual Attention During the First 2 Years of Life: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Netw. Open 2021, 4, e213687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tricco, A.C.; Lillie, E.; Zarin, W.; O’Brien, K.K.; Colquhoun, H.; Levac, D.; Moher, D.; Peters, M.D.J.; Horsley, T.; Weeks, L.; et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann. Intern. Med. 2018, 169, 467–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, L.L.; Wang, Y.Y.; Yang, Z.H.; Huang, D.; Weng, H.; Zeng, X.T. Methodological quality (risk of bias) assessment tools for primary and secondary medical studies: What are they and which is better? Mil Med. Res. 2020, 7, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ottawa Hospital Research Institute. Available online: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp (accessed on 2 July 2022).
- Critical Appraisal Tools. Available online: https://jbi.global/critical-appraisal-tools (accessed on 1 July 2022).
- Lin, Y.; Ding, H.; Zhang, Y. Gender Differences in Identifying Facial, Prosodic, and Semantic Emotions Show Category- and Channel-Specific Effects Mediated by Encoder’s Gender. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2021, 64, 2941–2955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marotta, A.; Rea, M.; Ferri, R.; Casagrande, M. Investigating socio-emotional cognition in late preterm children: A case-control study. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 17, 365–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Witt, A.; Theurel, A.; Tolsa, C.B.; Lejeune, F.; Fernandes, L.; de Jonge, L.V.; Monnier, M.; Bickle Graz, M.; Barisnikov, K.; Gentaz, E.; et al. Emotional and effortful control abilities in 42-month-old very preterm and full-term children. Early Hum. Dev. 2014, 90, 565–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gao, X.; Lahat, A.; Maurer, D.; Savoy, C.; Van Lieshout, R.J.; Boyle, M.H.; Saigal, S.; Schmidt, L.A. Sensitivity to facial expressions among extremely low birth weight survivors in their 30s. Dev. Psychobiol. 2017, 59, 1051–1057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wocadlo, C.; Rieger, I. Social skills and nonverbal decoding of emotions in very preterm children at early school age. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 3, 48–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williamson, K.E.; Jakobson, L.S. Social perception in children born at very low birthweight and its relationship with social/behavioral outcomes. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 990–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marleau, I.; Vona, M.; Gagner, C.; Luu, T.M.; Beauchamp, M.H. Social cognition, adaptive functioning, and behavior problems in preschoolers born extremely preterm. Child Neuropsychol. 2021, 27, 96–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Twilhaar, E.S.; de Kieviet, J.F.; Bergwerff, C.E.; Finken, M.J.J.; van Elburg, R.M.; Oosterlaan, J. Social Adjustment in Adolescents Born Very Preterm: Evidence for a Cognitive Basis of Social Problems. J. Pediatr. 2019, 213, 66–73.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pavlova, M.A.; Galli, J.; Zanetti, F.; Pagani, F.; Micheletti, S.; Rossi, A.; Sokolov, A.N.; Fallgatter, A.J.; Fazzi, E.M. Social cognition in individuals born preterm. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 14448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mathewson, K.J.; Maurer, D.; Mondloch, C.J.; Saigal, S.; Van Lieshout, R.J.; Schmidt, L.A. Visual configural processing in adults born at extremely low birth weight. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perez-Roche, T.; Altemir, I.; Giménez, G.; Prieto, E.; González, I.; López Pisón, J.; Pueyo, V. Face recognition impairment in small for gestational age and preterm children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2017, 62, 166–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bate, S.; Mestry, N.; Atkinson, M.; Bennetts, R.J.; Hills, P.J. Birthweight predicts individual differences in adult face recognition ability. Br. J. Psychol. 2021, 112, 628–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Potharst, E.S.; Van Wassenaer-Leemhuis, A.G.; Houtzager, B.A.; Livesey, D.; Kok, J.H.; Last, B.F.; Oosterlaan, J. Perinatal risk factors for neurocognitive impairments in preschool children born very preterm. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2013, 55, 178–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pereira, S.A.; Pereira Junior, A.; Costa, M.F.; Monteiro, M.V.; Almeida, V.A.; Fonseca Filho, G.G.; Arrais, N.; Simion, F. A comparison between preterm and full-term infants’ preference for faces. J. Pediatr. 2017, 93, 35–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Telford, E.J.; Fletcher-Watson, S.; Gillespie-Smith, K.; Pataky, R.; Sparrow, S.; Murray, I.C.; O’Hare, A.; Boardman, J.P. Preterm birth is associated with atypical social orienting in infancy detected using eye tracking. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2016, 57, 861–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dean, B.; Ginnell, L.; Ledsham, V.; Tsanas, A.; Telford, E.; Sparrow, S.; Fletcher-Watson, S.; Boardman, J.P. Eye-tracking for longitudinal assessment of social cognition in children born preterm. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2021, 62, 470–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berdasco-Muñoz, E.; Nazzi, T.; Yeung, H.H. Visual scanning of a talking face in preterm and full-term infants. Dev. Psychol. 2019, 55, 1353–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yamamoto, M.; Konishi, Y.; Kato, I.; Koyano, K.; Nakamura, S.; Nishida, T.; Kusaka, T. Do low birth weight infants not see eyes? Face recognition in infancy. Brain Dev. 2021, 43, 186–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Healy, E.; Reichenberg, A.; Nam, K.W.; Allin, M.P.G.; Walshe, M.; Rifkin, L.; Murray, S.R.M.; Nosarti, C. Preterm birth and adolescent social functioning-alterations in emotion-processing brain areas. J. Pediatr. 2013, 163, 1596–1604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grannis, C.; Hung, A.; French, R.C.; Mattson, W.I.; Fu, X.; Hoskinson, K.R.; Gerry Taylor, H.; Nelson, E.E. Multimodal classification of extremely preterm and term adolescents using the fusiform gyrus: A machine learning approach. NeuroImage Clin. 2022, 35, 103078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papini, C.; White, T.P.; Montagna, A.; Brittain, P.J.; Froudist-Walsh, S.; Kroll, J.; Karolis, V.; Simonelli, A.; Williams, S.C.; Murray, R.M.; et al. Altered resting-state functional connectivity in emotion-processing brain regions in adults who were born very preterm. Psychol. Med. 2016, 46, 3025–3039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kiiski, H.; Bennett, M.; Rueda-Delgado, L.M.; Farina, F.R.; Knight, R.; Boyle, R.; Roddy, D.; Grogan, K.; Bramham, J.; Kelly, C.; et al. EEG spectral power, but not theta/beta ratio, is a neuromarker for adult ADHD. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2020, 51, 2095–2109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amani, B.; Schmidt, L.A.; Mullen, J.N.; Lieshout, R.J.V. Right frontal brain activity at rest in the early 20s predicts threat-related bias ten years later among extremely low birth weight survivors. Neurosci. Lett. 2020, 730, 135012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frie, J.; Padilla, N.; Ådén, U.; Lagercrantz, H.; Bartocci, M. Extremely Preterm-Born Infants Demonstrate Different Facial Recognition Processes at 6-10 Months of Corrected Age. J. Pediatr. 2016, 172, 96–102.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carlsson, J.; Lagercrantz, H.; Olson, L.; Printz, G.; Bartocci, M. Activation of the right fronto-temporal cortex during maternal facial recognition in young infants. Acta Paediatr. 2008, 97, 1221–1225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Urbain, C.; Sato, J.; Hammill, C.; Duerden, E.G.; Taylor, M.J. Converging function, structure, and behavioural features of emotion regulation in very preterm children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019, 40, 3385–3397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mossad, S.I.; Muscat, C.; Pang, E.W.; Taylor, M. Emerging atypical connectivity networks for processing angry and fearful faces in very preterm born children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020, 41, 3794–3806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sato, J.; Safar, K.; Vandewouw, M.M.; Bando, N.; O’Connor, D.L.; Unger, S.L.; Taylor, M.J. Altered functional connectivity during face processing in children born with very low birth weight. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 2021, 16, 1182–1190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dupierrix, E.; de Boisferon, A.H.; Méary, D.; Lee, K.; Quinn, P.C.; Di Giorgio, E.; Simion, F.; Tomonaga, M.; Pascalis, O. Preference for human eyes in human infants. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 123, 138–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGurk, H.; MacDonald, J. Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 1976, 264, 746–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, W.; Klin, A. Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature 2013, 504, 427–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scott, H.K.; Cogburn, M. Piaget. In StatPearls; StatPearls Publishing: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Carver, L.J.; Dawson, G.; Panagiotides, H.; Meltzoff, A.N.; McPartland, J.; Gray, J.; Munson, J. Age-related differences in neural correlates of face recognition during the toddler and preschool years. Dev. Psychobiol. 2003, 42, 148–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Freire, A.; Lee, K. Face recognition in 4- to 7-year-olds: Processing of configural, featural, and paraphernalia information. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2001, 80, 347–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McKone, E.; Boyer, B.L. Sensitivity of 4-year-olds to featural and second-order relational changes in face distinctiveness. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2006, 94, 134–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, V.; Da Silva, N.; Rutherford, M.D. Toddlers Categorically Perceive Emotional Facial Expressions Along a Happy-Sad Continuum. Perception 2020, 49, 822–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghuman, A.S.; Brunet, N.M.; Li, Y.; Konecky, R.O.; Pyles, J.A.; Walls, S.A.; Destefino, V.; Wang, W.; Richardson, R.M. Dynamic encoding of face information in the human fusiform gyrus. Nat. Commun. 2014, 5, 5672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, Y.; Li, Q.; Cao, X. Development of Holistic Face Processing From Childhood and Adolescence to Young Adulthood in Chinese Individuals. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bank, S.; Rhodes, G.; Read, A.; Jeffery, L. Face and body recognition show similar improvement during childhood. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015, 137, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen Kadosh, K.; Cohen Kadosh, R.; Dick, F.; Johnson, M.H. Developmental changes in effective connectivity in the emerging core face network. Cereb. Cortex 2011, 21, 1389–1394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, M.J.; Batty, M.; Itier, R.J. The faces of development: A review of early face processing over childhood. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 1426–1442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aylward, E.H.; Park, J.E.; Field, K.M.; Parsons, A.C.; Richards, T.L.; Cramer, S.C.; Meltzoff, A.N. Brain activation during face perception: Evidence of a developmental change. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 308–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gathers, A.D.; Bhatt, R.; Corbly, C.R.; Farley, A.B.; Joseph, J.E. Developmental shifts in cortical loci for face and object recognition. Neuroreport 2004, 15, 1549–1553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haist, F.; Adamo, M.; Han Wazny, J.; Lee, K.; Stiles, J. The functional architecture for face-processing expertise: FMRI evidence of the developmental trajectory of the core and the extended face systems. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 2893–2908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fuhrmann, D.; Knoll, L.J.; Sakhardande, A.L.; Speekenbrink, M.; Kadosh, K.C.; Blakemore, S.J. Perception and recognition of faces in adolescence. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 33497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hendriks, M.H.A.; Dillen, C.; Vettori, S.; Vercammen, L.; Daniels, N.; Steyaert, J.; Op de Beeck, H.; Boets, B. Neural processing of facial identity and expression in adults with and without autism: A multi-method approach. NeuroImage Clin. 2021, 29, 102520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chau, C.M.Y.; Ranger, M.; Sulistyoningrum, D.; Devlin, A.M.; Oberlander, T.F.; Grunau, R.E. Neonatal pain and COMT Val158Met genotype in relation to serotonin transporter (SLC6A4) promoter methylation in very preterm children at school age. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newborn Size • INTERGROWTH-21st. Available online: https://intergrowth21.tghn.org/newborn-size-birth/ (accessed on 6 July 2022).
- Niedenthal, P.M. Embodying emotion. Science 2007, 316, 1002–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Efthimiou, T.N.; Baker, J.; Clarke, A.; Elsenaar, A.; Mehu, M.; Korb, S. Zygomaticus activation through facial neuromuscular electrical stimulation (fNMES) induces happiness perception in ambiguous facial expressions and affects neural correlates of face processing. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 2024, 19, nsae013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Romani, M.; Vigliante, M.; Faedda, N.; Rossetti, S.; Pezzuti, L.; Guidetti, V.; Cardona, F. Face memory and face recognition in children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 89, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García-Casal, J.A.; Goñi-Imizcoz, M.; Perea-Bartolomé, M.V.; Soto-Pérez, F.; Smith, S.J.; Calvo-Simal, S.; Franco-Martín, M. The Efficacy of Emotion Recognition Rehabilitation for People with Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017, 57, 937–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Tang, T.; Pledts, K.; Moerkerke, M.; Van der Donck, S.; Bollen, B.; Steyaert, J.; Alaerts, K.; Ortibus, E.; Naulaers, G.; Boets, B. Face Processing in Prematurely Born Individuals—A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 1168. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14121168
Tang T, Pledts K, Moerkerke M, Van der Donck S, Bollen B, Steyaert J, Alaerts K, Ortibus E, Naulaers G, Boets B. Face Processing in Prematurely Born Individuals—A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(12):1168. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14121168
Chicago/Turabian StyleTang, Tiffany, Kasper Pledts, Matthijs Moerkerke, Stephanie Van der Donck, Bieke Bollen, Jean Steyaert, Kaat Alaerts, Els Ortibus, Gunnar Naulaers, and Bart Boets. 2024. "Face Processing in Prematurely Born Individuals—A Systematic Review" Brain Sciences 14, no. 12: 1168. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14121168
APA StyleTang, T., Pledts, K., Moerkerke, M., Van der Donck, S., Bollen, B., Steyaert, J., Alaerts, K., Ortibus, E., Naulaers, G., & Boets, B. (2024). Face Processing in Prematurely Born Individuals—A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences, 14(12), 1168. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14121168