The rationale for data analyses is listed below. Two within-participants factors, i.e., syntactic category and word type, and one between-participants factor, i.e., group, were put into a three-way ANOVA. First, we analyzed the data from college students to find out the typical pattern and possible impact of syntactic categories in false memory. Later, we analyzed the data from the DS group and the TD controls. Third, the discrimination sensitivities between associates from lures and associates from unrelated words were calculated in the noun- and verb-eliciting false-memory tasks. Fourth, error patterns in the noun- and verb-eliciting false-memory tasks were analyzed.
3.2. Data Analyses of People with Down Syndrome and Typically Developing Controls
No three-way interactions were observed (F < 1). Two-way interaction between types of words and groups was significant [F(4,114) = 12.06, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.29]. One of the simple main effects was from the difference among groups in each type of word, suggesting group difference in lures [DS, 0.518, SE = 0.040; CA, 0.441, SE = 0.040; MA, 0.359, SE = 0.040; F(2,57) = 3.96, p = 0.025, ƞ2 = 0.122], associates [CA, 0.709, SE = 0.031; MA, 0.592, SE = 0.031; DS, 0.588, SE = 0.031; F(2,57) = 4.931, p = 0.011, ƞ2 = 0.147], and unrelated words [DS, 0.355, SE = 0.032; MA, 0.157, SE = 0.032; CA, 0.101, SE = 0.032; F(2,57) = 17.270, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.377]. The difference in lures was due to the difference between the DS and MA (p = 0.007). The difference in associations was due to the difference between DS and CA (p = 0.008) and the difference between MA and CA (p = 0.01). The difference in unrelated words was due to the difference between DS and CA (p < 0.001) and DS and MA (p < 0.001). Another simple main effect was the difference in the types of words used in each group. In the CA group, associates were responded to more accurately (0.709, SD = 0.157) than lures (0.440, SD = 0.153) and unrelated words (0.100, SD = 0.196) at p < 0.001 [F(2,38) = 91.743, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.828]. The latter two types of words differed significantly (p < 0.001). In the MA group, associates were responded to more accurately (0.591, SD = 0.123) than lures (0.359, SD = 0.131) and unrelated words (0.156, SD = 0.132) at p < 0.001 [F(2,38) = 77.022, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.802]. The latter two types of words differed significantly (p < 0.001). In the DS group, associates were responded to more accurately (0.588, SD = 0.132) than lures (0.517, SD = 0.232) and unrelated words (0.355, SD = 0.187) at p < 0.001 [F(2,38) = 22.058, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.537]. The latter two types of words differed significantly (p < 0.001). The difference between associates and lures was not significant.
Two-way interaction of syntactic categories and types of words reached significance [F(2,114) = 4.718, p = 0.011, ƞ2 = 0.076]. The simple main effect was from the difference among types of nouns [F(2,118) = 97.864, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.624] and verbs [F(2,118) = 131.211, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.690]. For nouns, associates (0.619, SE = 0.021) received higher responses than lures (0.441, SE = 0.027) or unrelated words (0.224, SE = 0.025) [all p < 0.001]. For verbs, associated words (0.641, SE = 0.022) received higher responses than lures (0.438, SE = 0.024) or unrelated words (0.184, SE = 0.024) [all p < 0.001]. No differences in the syntactic categories of the word types emerged.
The main effect of types of words reached significance [
F(2,114) = 179.553,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.759], suggesting associates received higher “yes” responses (0.630, SE = 0.018) than lures (0.439, SE = 0.023) and unrelated words (0.204, SE = 0.019) at
p < 0.001. Unrelated words received the least number of responses. The main effect of groups was significant [
F(2,57) = 5.114,
p = 0.009,
ƞ2 = 0.152], implying that the DS group pressed the highest frequency of “yes” responses (0.487, SE = 0.026) compared to the CA group (0.417, SE = 0.026) and the MA group (0.369, SE = 0.026). Only the difference between the DS and MA groups was significant (
p = 0.002).
Figure 3 shows the accuracy of word types with error bars across nouns and verbs of the three groups. In sum, people with DS had developmental delays in responding to associates and developmental deviance in processing lures and unrelated words.
A participant was not counted in the analyses of reaction times because the response time was too long. The three-way ANOVA revealed no interaction. The main effect of word types was significant [
F(2,82) = 4.500,
p < 0.014,
ƞ2 = 0.099], suggesting unrelated words were responded to the slowest (2233 ms, SE = 161) compared to the lures (2112 ms, SE = 158) [
p = 0.030] and the associates (2034 ms, SE = 147) [
p = 0.026]. The latter two values were not statistically significant. The main effect of groups was significant [
F(2,46) = 7.117,
p = 0.002,
ƞ2 = 0.236]. People with DS (2956 ms, SE = 268 ms) were slowest in responding to all trials compared to the two controls (CA, 1588 ms, SE = 277 ms,
p = 0.001; MA, 1857 ms, SE = 277 ms,
p = 0.007). No significant differences emerged between the CA and MA groups.
Figure 4 shows the reaction times with error bars for the types of nouns and verbs in the three groups.
3.4. Analyses of Error Patterns in Noun-Eliciting False-Memory Task
The error pattern was analyzed topic-wise across groups. The nominal topics and word types were the within-participants factors, and groups were the between-participants factor in a three-way analysis of variance. The results revealed a significant three-way interaction [F(36,1026) = 1.571, p = 0.018, ƞ2 = 0.052]. One of the simple main effects was from the two-way interaction of topics and groups to lures [F(18,513) = 2.273, p = 0.002, ƞ2 = 0.074]. The simple simple main effect was from the group effect on the topics of public square dancing [DS, 0.500, SD = 0.296; MA, 0.133, SD = 0.199; CA, 0.116, SD = 0.163; F(2,57) = 18.279, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.391], amusement park [DS, 0.516, SD = 0.314; MA, 0.266, SD = 0.231; CA, 0.283, SD = 0.311; F(2,57) = 4.694, p = 0.013, ƞ2= 0.141], and summer [DS, 0.433, SD = 0.326; MA, 0.250, SD = 0.238; CA, 0.216, SD = 0.291; F(2,57) = 3.286, p = 0.045, ƞ2 = 0.103]. Based on the topic-wise comparisons, people with DS made more errors than the two control groups with post hoc analyses with the least significant difference at p < 0.001 on the topic of public square dancing compared to both the CA group and MA group, at p = 0.013 (DS vs. CA) and p = 0.008 (DS vs. MA) on the topic of amusement park, and at p = 0.021 (DS vs. CA) and p = 0.049 (DS vs. MA) on the topic of summer. The remaining group effect was observed on the topics of breakfast store [DS, 0.650, SD = 0.366; MA, 0.300, SD = 0.303; CA, 0.500, SD = 0.315; F(2,57) = 5.674, p = 0.006, ƞ2= 0.166] and road [DS, 0.616, SD = 0.363; MA, 0.300, SD = 0.303; CA, 0.483, SD = 0.295; F(2,57) = 4.866, p = 0.011, ƞ2 = 0.146], suggesting higher errors by people with DS than the MA controls on the topics of breakfast store (p = 0.001) and road (p = 0.003). Another simple simple main effect was from the topic effect on groups [CA, F(9,171) = 9.735, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.339; MA, F(9,171) = 7.431, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.281; DS, F(9,171) = 2.145, p = 0.028, ƞ2 = 0.101]. All the groups erred highest on the topic of hospital (CA, 0.700, SE = 0.064; MA, 0.733, SE = 0.052; DS, 0.667, SE = 0.084); however, the patterns of lowest errors were different in people with DS from the two TD control groups. While the CA and MA groups erred least with public square dancing (CA, 0.117, SE = 0.036; MA, 0.133, SE = 0.045), the DS group erred least with barbershop (0.433, SE = 0.091) and with summer (0.433, SE = 0.073). The main effect of the group reached significance [F(2,57) = 5.558, p = 0.006, ƞ2 = 0.163], suggesting the DS group erred more (0.547, SE = 0.044) than the CA group (0.433, SE = 0.044) and the MA group (0.342, SE = 0.044). The difference between the DS and the MA groups was statistically significant (p = 0.002).
The interaction of topics and groups in processing associates and unrelated words failed to reach significance. The main effect of groups in processing associates was significant [F(2,57) = 3.631, p = 0.033, ƞ2 = 0.113], suggesting the CA group erred least (0.307, SE = 0.035) compared with the MA group (0.432, SE = 0.035, p = 0.013) and the DS group (0.405, SE = 0.035, p = 0.049). The lowest errors emerged with hospital (0.217, SE = 0.039); the highest was with summer (0.472, SE = 0.038). Moreover, the main effect of the group in processing the unrelated words was significant [F(2,57) = 16.733, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.370], suggesting the DS group erred most (0.387, SE = 0.035) compared with the CA group (0.120, SE = 0.035) and the MA group (0.167, SE = 0.035) at p < 0.001. In sum, people with DS made errors in associates like the MA controls (i.e., delay), but they made more errors in unrelated words the most (i.e., deviance). The error patterns of people with DS differed from those of the two control groups.
Another simple main effect was the two-way interaction between the type and group for each topic. The results showed the topics of the
Dragon Boat Festival,
public square dancing,
bathroom,
road, and
summer reached significance.
Table 2 presents the statistical results.
In the analysis of the topic of the
Dragon Boat Festival, the simple main effect was from group effect on associates [
F(2,57) = 3.485,
p = 0.037,
ƞ2 = 0.109]. The DS group erred more (0.533, SD = 0.199) than the MA group (0.500, SD = 0.253,
p = 0.017), which was greater than that of the CA group (0.333, SD = 0.305,
p = 0.045). No differences were observed between the groups for lures or unrelated words. The other simple main effect was from types of words on groups [CA,
F(2,38) = 7.858,
p = 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.293; MA,
F(2,38) = 8.703,
p = 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.314; DS,
F(2,38) = 10.916,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.365]. People with DS erred least with unrelated words (0.217, SE = 0.065) than with lures (0.517, SE = 0.074) and associates (0.533, SE = 0.045) at
p = 0.001. The patterns observed in the CA group were similar to those in people with DS (unrelated words, 0.117, SE = 0.050; associates, 0.333, SE = 0.068; lures, 0.517, SE = 0.085). The difference between unrelated words and lures was significant (
p = 0.001), as was the difference between unrelated words and associates (
p = 0.008). However, the MA group made more errors with associates (0.500, SE = 0.057) than lures (0.300, SE = 0.072) and unrelated words (0.150, SE = 0.051). The difference between associates and lures was
p = 0.019, and the difference between associates and unrelated words was
p < 0.001. The results revealed different patterns in the MA group compared to the CA and DS groups. A graph shows “yes” responses as the semantic distance between the topic of the
Dragon Boat Festival and the types of words in
Figure 5. The data of college students are included in the graph to uncover the whole picture of semantic relatedness.
In the analysis of the topic of
public square dancing, the interaction of types of words and groups reached significance [
F(4,114) = 6.755,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.192]. The simple main effect was from the group effect on lures [
F(2,57) = 5.674,
p = 0.006,
ƞ2 = 0.116] and unrelated words [
F(2,57) = 3.572,
p = 0.035,
ƞ2 = 0.111]. People with DS made more errors (0.650, SE = 0.074) with lures than did the CA (0.500, SE = 0.074) and MA groups (0.300, SE = 0.074). Only the difference of people with DS and the MA group was significant (
p = 0.001). People with DS (0.450, SE = 0.066) erred more than the two control groups (0.233, SE = 0.066) with unrelated words, suggesting deviant processing in people with DS. Another simple main effect was from the effect of word types on control groups [CA,
F(2,38) = 4.815,
p = 0.014,
ƞ2 = 0.202; MA,
F(2,38) = 19.046,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.501; DS,
F < 1]. In the CA and MA groups, associates received more errors than lures (CA,
p = 0.012; MA,
p < 0.001) and unrelated words (CA,
p = 0.049; MA,
p = 0.001) [CA, associates, 0.300, SD = 0.239; lures, 0.116, SD = 0.163; unrelated words, 0.183, SD = 0.253; MA, associates, 0.650, SD = 0.275; lures, 0.133, SD = 0.199; unrelated words, 0.216, SD = 0.311]. However, people with DS did not show this pattern. Instead, people with DS erred more with lures (0.500, SE = 0.066) than with associates (0.400, SE = 0.071) or unrelated words (0.383, SE = 0.070). The latter two types of words yielded no difference. This finding suggests that people with DS were deviant in making the highest “yes” responses to unrelated words and showed a distinct pattern of higher errors with lures than the control groups. A graph shows “yes” responses as the semantic distance between the topic of
public square dancing and the types of words in
Figure 5.
In the analysis of the topic of
bathroom, the interaction of types of words and groups reached significance [
F(4,114) = 2.731,
p = 0.032,
ƞ2 = 0.087]. The simple main effect was from the group effect on unrelated words [
F(2,57) = 7.320,
p = 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.204], suggesting people with DS made more errors than the CA group (
p < 0.001) and the MA group (
p = 0.017). Another simple main effect was from types of words in groups [CA,
F(2,38) = 23.413,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.552; MA,
F(2,38) = 3.973,
p = 0.027,
ƞ2 = 0.173]. However, this difference was not significant in the DS group. In the CA and MA groups, lures received more errors than associates or unrelated words (CA, lures, 0.600, SD = 0.255; associates, 0.333, SD = 0.305; unrelated words, 0.050, SD = 0.122; MA, lures, 0.416, SD = 0.322; associates, 0.400, SD = 0.298; unrelated words, 0.166, SD = 0.315). The CA group showed a difference between unrelated words and lures at
p < 0.001 and a difference between unrelated words and associates at
p = 0.001. The difference between lures and associates was
p = 0.012. The MA group showed a difference between unrelated words and lures at
p = 0.010 and a difference between unrelated words and associates at
p = 0.019. The MA group did not show a difference between lures and associates. A graph shows “yes” responses as the semantic distance between the topic of
bathroom and the types of words in
Figure 6.
In the analysis of the topic of
road, the interaction of types of words and groups was significant [
F(4,114) = 2.721,
p = 0.033,
ƞ2 = 0.087]. The simple main effect was from the group effect on lures [
F(2,57) = 4.866,
p = 0.011,
ƞ2 = 0.146] and unrelated words [
F(2,57) = 10.756,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.274]. People with DS made more errors with lures than those in the MA group (
p = 0.003). People with DS erred more with unrelated words than the MA and CA groups, both at
p < 0.001. Another simple main effect was from differences among types of words in the CA group [
F(2,38) = 8.412,
p = 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.307] and the DS group [
F(2,38) = 5.159,
p = 0.010,
ƞ2 = 0.214]. In the CA group, lures (0.483, SD = 0.295) received more errors than associates (0.216, SD = 0.223,
p = 0.014) and unrelated words (0.166, SD = 0.299,
p = 0.001). In the DS group, lures (0.616, SD = 0.363) received more errors than unrelated words (0.516, SD = 0.350) or associates (0.316, SD = 0.275), both at
p = 0.012. A graph shows “yes” responses as the semantic distance between the topic of
road and the types of words in
Figure 6.
In the analysis of the topic of
summer, the interaction of types of words and groups was significant [
F(4,114) = 5.919,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.172]. The simple main effect was from group effect on types of words [lures,
F(2,57) = 3.286,
p = 0.045,
ƞ2 = 0.103; associates,
F(2,57) = 3.643,
p = 0.032,
ƞ2 = 0.113; unrelated words,
F(2,57) = 17.401,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.379]. Regarding the error patterns of lures and unrelated words, the DS group made more errors than the CA and MA groups (lures, DS, 0.433, SE = 0.064; CA, 0.217, SE = 0.064; MA, 0.250, SE = 0.064; unrelated words, DS, 0.433, SE = 0.049; CA, 0.033, SE = 0.049; MA, 0.150, SE = 0.049). For lures, the difference between the DS group and the MA group was at
p = 0.049, and the difference between the DS group and the CA group was at
p = 0.021. In unrelated words, the difference between the DS group and two control groups was
p < 0.001. However, in the error pattern of associates, the MA group (0.617, SE = 0.066) erred most compared to the CA group (0.417, SE = 0.066,
p = 0.037) and DS group (0.383, SE = 0.066,
p = 0.015). Another simple main effect was from differences among types of words in the CA group [
F(2,38) = 12.938,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.405] and the MA group [
F(2,38) = 15.721,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.453]. Both the CA and MA groups erred more with associates (CA: 0.417, SE = 0.063; MA: 0.617, SE = 0.078) than with lures (CA: 0.217, SE = 0.065; MA: 0.250, SE = 0.053) or unrelated words (CA: 0.033, SE = 0.023; MA: 0.150, SE = 0.045). Together, these results suggest that people with DS show deviations in processing summer-related semantic words. A graph shows “yes” responses as the semantic distance between the topic of
summer and the types of words in
Figure 7.
No interaction emerged among the processing of barbershop and breakfast store, amusement park, Christmas, and hospital. People with DS erred most compared with the CA and MA controls. Significant differences among the groups emerged in processing barbershop, breakfast store, public square dancing, amusement park, bathroom, road, and summer. The topic-wise analyses yielded effects on word types in each group. In the CA group, all the effects of the types of words were significant (except for the topic of amusement park). In the MA group, almost all effects of the types of words were significant (except for breakfast store, amusement park, and road). In the DS group, almost all effects of the types of words were not significant (except for the Dragon Boat Festival, road, and hospital). These findings suggest that people with DS deviate from forming concepts related to nouns through lexical semantics.
3.5. Analyses of Error Patterns in Verb-Eliciting False-Memory Task
To determine which semantic concepts of verbs were fragile in people with DS compared to controls, the error patterns of topics in each group were analyzed. Verbal topics and types of words were taken as the within-participants factors and groups as the between-participants factor in a three-way analysis of variance. The results revealed a significant three-way interaction [F(36,1026) = 2.083, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.068]. One of the simple main effects was from the two-way interaction of topics and groups in processing lures [F(18,513) = 2.566, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.083]. The simple simple main effect was from the group effect on do housework [F(2,57) = 3.243, p = 0.046, ƞ2 = 0.102], transport during Spring Festival [F(2,57) = 3.917, p = 0.025, ƞ2 = 0.121], get married [F(2,57) = 3.966, p = 0.024, ƞ2 = 0.122], and shop online. In the processing of the former two topics and the last one, the DS group erred more than the two control groups [do housework, DS, 0.583, SD = 0.402; CA, 0.533, SD = 0.380; MA, 0.300, SD = 0.340; transport during Spring Festival, DS, 0.466, SD = 0.294; CA, 0.283, SD = 0.311; MA, 0.216, SD = 0.270; shop online, DS, 0.650, SD = 0.350; CA, 0.500, SD = 0.275; MA, 0.350, SD = 0.253]. Only the differences between the DS group and MA group were significant (do housework, p = 0.020; transport during the Spring Festival, p = 0.009; shop online, p = 0.002). When processing the concept of get married, the DS group (0.466, SD = 0.313) produced fewer “yes” responses compared with the two control groups (0.716, SD = 0.329). The differences between the DS group and the two controls were significant (p = 0.018).
Another simple simple main effect was from the effect of types of words on groups [CA, F(9,171) = 5.018, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.209; MA, F(9,171) = 5.373, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.220; DS, F(9,171) = 2.996, p = 0.002, ƞ2 = 0.136]. The error patterns differed between the DS and control groups. In the two control groups, both groups erred most with get married (0.716, SD = 0.329) and least with celebrate birthday (CA, 0.250, SD = 0.283; MA, 0.216, SD = 0.270) and transport during Spring Festival (CA, 0.283, SD = 0.311; MA, 0.216, SD = 0.270). However, people with DS erred most with shop online (0.650, SD = 0.350) and least with celebrate birthday (0.400, SD = 0.368) and play the piano (0.283, SD = 0.369).
Another simple main effect was from the effect of types of words on processing associates [F(18,513) = 2.003, p = 0.008, ƞ2 = 0.066]. The simple simple main effect was from the group effect on get married [F(2,57) = 10.121, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.262], celebrate New Year [F(2,57) = 5.894, p = 0.005, ƞ2 = 0.171], and play the piano [F(2,57) = 4.101, p = 0.022, ƞ2 = 0.126]. In the processing of the former two topics, people with DS erred most among groups [get married, DS, 0.550, SD = 0.329; MA, 0.283, SD = 0.291; CA, 0.150, SD = 0.228; celebrate New Year, DS, 0.366, SD = 0.284; MA, 0.250, SD = 0.303; CA, 0.083, SD = 0.183]. In the processing of play the piano, people with DS (0.416, SD = 0.322) made errors similar to those in the CA group (0.383, SD = 0.311), which were fewer than those in the MA group (0.633, SD = 0.262). Another simple simple main effect was from the effect of topics on groups [CA, F(9,171) = 4.400, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.188; MA, F(9.171) = 4.812, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.202]. No significant differences were observed between the DS and two control groups. Moreover, the error patterns differed between the CA and MA groups. In the CA group, the errors with transport during Spring Festival were the highest (0.400, SD = 0.352) and the errors with celebrate the New Year (0.083, SD = 0.183) and get married (0.150, SD = 0.228) were the least. In the MA group, the errors with play the piano were the highest (0.633, SD = 0.262) and the errors with celebrate New Year (0.250, SD = 0.303) and watch movies (0.250, SD = 0.262) were the least. The interaction between topics and groups in processing unrelated words was not significant. People with DS erred most among groups with the unrelated words [main effect of group, F(2,57) = 13.132, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.315; DS, 0.323, SE = 0.035; MA, 0.147, SE = 0.035; CA, 0.082, SE = 0.035].
In sum, people with DS were deviant in responding to lures of the verb-eliciting topics of
do housework,
transport during Spring Festival,
get married, and
shop online. People with DS showed deviant error patterns in processing lures that differed from the control groups. People with DS were also deviant in responding to associates of the verb-eliciting topics
get married,
celebrate New Year, and
play the piano. Unlike the two control groups, people with DS showed similar “yes” responses to all topics, without significant differences for any specific topic in the processing of associates. To clarify the nature of processing verb-eliciting topics in false-memory tasks across groups, topic-based statistical results are listed in
Table 3.
In the processing of do housework, the interaction of types and groups reached significance [F(4,114) = 2.778, p = 0.030, ƞ2 = 0.089]. The simple main effect was from group effect on types [lures, F(2,57) = 3.243, p = 0.046, ƞ2 = 0.102; unrelated words, F(2,57) = 5.778, p = 0.005, ƞ2 = 0.169]. In the processing of lures, people with DS (0.583, SD = 0.402) erred more than those with CA (0.533, SD = 0.380) or MA (0.300, SD = 0.340). The difference between the DS group and the MA group was at p = 0.020. In the processing of unrelated words, people with DS (0.250, SD = 0.283) erred more than those with CA (0.016, SD = 0.074) or MA (0.183, SD = 0.253). The difference between the DS group and the CA group was at p = 0.002; the difference between the CA and MA group was at p = 0.022.
Another simple main effect was from the effect of types of words on groups [CA,
F(2,38) = 13.103,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.408; MA,
F(2,38) = 4.217,
p = 0.022,
ƞ2 = 0.182; CA,
F(2,38) = 3.690,
p = 0.034,
ƞ2 = 0.163]. People with DS responded more to lures than to associates or unrelated words (CA, lures, 0.533, SE = 0.085; associates, 0.367, SE = 0.068; unrelated words, 0.017, SE = 0.017; DS, lures, 0.583, SE = 0.090; associates, 0.367, SE = 0.072; unrelated words, 0.250, SE = 0.063). The differences between lures and associates or lures and unrelated words in the CA group were both at
p < 0.001. The difference between lures and unrelated words in the DS group was at
p = 0.003. However, the MA group responded more strongly to associates (0.483, SD = 0.295) than to lures (0.300, SD = 0.340) or unrelated words (0.183, SD = 0.253). A graph showing “yes” responses as the semantic distance between the topic of
do housework and the types of words is shown in
Figure 8.
In the processing of
get married, the interaction of types and groups reached significance [
F(4,114) = 7.658,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.212]. The simple main effect was from group effect on types [lures,
F(2,57) = 3.966,
p = 0.024,
ƞ2 = 0.122; associates,
F(2,57) = 10.121,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.262; unrelated words,
F(2,57) = 8.471,
p = 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.229]. People with DS deviated from processing topics. When processing associates and unrelated words, most people with DS erred most significantly among groups (associates, DS: 0.550, SD = 0.329; MA: 0.283, SD = 0.291; CA: 0.150, SD = 0.228; unrelated words: DS, 0.383, SD = 0.291; MA, 0.150, SD = 0.228; CA, 0.100, SD = 0.156). In the processing of lures, people with DS had significantly fewer “yes” responses than those in the two control groups (DS, 0.466, SD = 0.313; MA, CA, 0.717, SD = 0.329). Another simple main effect was from the effect of types of words on groups [CA,
F(2,38) = 39.271,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.674; MA,
F(2,38) = 20.193,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.515]. No difference was observed in the DS group. Both the CA and MA groups erred more with lures than with associates and unrelated words. The graph showing “yes” responses as the semantic distance between the topic of
get married and types of words is depicted in
Figure 8.
In the processing of
play the piano, the interaction of types and groups reached significance [
F(4,114) = 2.563,
p = 0.042,
ƞ2 = 0.083]. The simple main effect was from the group effect on types of words [associates,
F(2,57) = 4.101,
p = 0.022,
ƞ2 = 0.126; unrelated words,
F(2,57) = 4.682,
p = 0.013,
ƞ2 = 0.141]. People with DS erred more with unrelated words than those in the MA and CA groups (DS, 0.333, SD = 0.324; MA, 0.183, SD = 0.253; CA, 0.083, SD = 0.183). Only the difference between the DS group and the CA group was statistically significant (
p = 0.004). People with DS did not show significant differences in the error patterns associated with the CA group (DS, 0.416, SD = 0.322; CA, 0.383, SD = 0.311), which were significantly lower than those in the MA group (0.633, SD = 0.262). No difference was observed in lure processing among the groups, although the MA group (0.350, SD = 0.295) responded more positively than the CA (0.283, SD = 0.311) and DS (0.283, SD = 0.329) groups. Another simple main effect was from the effect of types of words on groups [CA,
F(2,38) = 8.430,
p = 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.307; MA,
F(2,38) = 15.596,
p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.451]. Both control groups responded more accurately to associates than to lures or unrelated words. In the CA group, the difference between unrelated words and associates was at
p = 0.001; the difference between unrelated words and lures was at
p = 0.010. In the MA group, the difference between unrelated words and associates was at
p < 0.001; the difference between unrelated words and lures was at
p = 0.029. The difference between the associates and lures was at
p = 0.009. No differences in word types were observed in the DS group. The graph showing “yes” responses as the semantic distance between the topic of
play the piano and types of words is depicted in
Figure 9.
Taken together, people with DS deviated in processing do housework, get married, and play the piano. Moreover, people with DS erred most among groups in the processing of transport during Spring Festival, celebrate New Year, watch movies, and shop online [main effect of group, transport during Spring Festival, F(2,57) = 5.780, p = 0.005, ƞ2 = 0.169; celebrate New Year, F(2,57) = 6.559, p = 0.003, ƞ2 = 0.187; watch movies, F(2,57) = 11.807, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.293; shop online, F(2,57) = 11.522, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.288]. People with DS did not show group differences in the processing of celebrate birthday, raise flag, and get lost.
Another simple main effect was from the interaction of topics and types of words in groups [CA, F(18,380) = 4.303, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.169; MA, F(18,380) = 3.909, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.156]. No interaction emerged in people with DS. The main effect of types of words on the three groups reached significance [CA, F(2,380) = 100.056, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.345; MA, F(2,380) = 45.850, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.194; DS, F(2,380) = 12.568, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.062]. People in the DS and the CA group responded more accurately to lures (CA: 0.448, SE = 0.023; DS: 0.488, SE = 0.025) than to associates (CA: 0.275, SE = 0.020; DS: 0.418, SE = 0.021) or unrelated words (CA: 0.082, SE = 0.012; DS: 0.323, SE = 0.022). The MA group responded more accurately to associated words (0.385, SE = 0.021) than to lures (0.377, SE = 0.022) or unrelated words (0.147, SE = 0.016). In the CA and MA groups, the main effect of topics was significant [CA, F(9,190) = 2.155, p = 0.027, ƞ2 = 0.093; MA, F(9,190) = 2.279, p = 0.019, ƞ2 = 0.097]. Error patterns in the CA and MA groups were similar. Three topics received the highest errors for associates, and over half received the highest errors for lures. The topic of get lost was not significant. People with DS show deviant error patterns in verb-eliciting false-memory tasks.
Regression analyses of dependent variables with the standardized tests of WSIC were employed with working memory forward digit span length and score, working memory backward digit span length and score, and total memory length and score. The results revealed that total memory length negatively predicted the yes/response proportion of unrelated words in verbs for people with DS (r = −0.455, adjusted R2 = 0.163, F(1,19) = 4.706, p = 0.04). This finding suggests that people with DS might use a verbatim strategy to memorize the stimuli but not automatically integrate the words into semantic memory. No other factor emerged as a predictor of accuracy or reaction times for any group of nouns or verbs in this study.