Executive Functioning in Different Types of Reading Disabilities
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Association Between Executive Functioning and Reading Comprehension Difficulties
1.2. Association Between Executive Functioning and Difficulties in Fluent and Accurate Reading
1.3. The Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Working Memory
2.2.2. Cognitive Flexibility
2.2.3. Inhibition
2.2.4. Planning
2.2.5. Rapid Naming
2.2.6. Processing Speed
2.3. Procedures
2.4. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Executive Functioning Dimensions | Typical Readers (n = 30) | Poor Readers (n = 29) | Poor Comprehenders (n = 8) | Dysfluent Readers (n = 24) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
Rapid Alternating Stimulus—Shapes/colors | 11.30 | 2.89 | 6.90 | 3.37 | 11.25 | 2.31 | 7.92 | 3.32 |
Rapid Automatized Naming—Digits | 9.43 | 2.24 | 7.21 | 3.22 | 11.00 | 2.27 | 6.33 | 3.20 |
Processing speed—Coding | 10.20 | 2.34 | 9.66 | 2.89 | 10.88 | 4.58 | 9.00 | 3.59 |
Processing speed—Symbol Search | 12.67 | 2.47 | 10.72 | 2.62 | 12.25 | 3.45 | 11.75 | 2.74 |
Planning—Maze | 11.83 | 3.35 | 10.21 | 2.02 | 10.88 | 2.30 | 11.13 | 2.72 |
Planning—Tower | 10.20 | 3.01 | 9.48 | 3.36 | 10.25 | 2.31 | 11.50 | 2.72 |
Working Memory | 10.00 | 1.64 | 8.24 | 2.32 | 8.75 | 2.05 | 8.83 | 2.18 |
Cognitive Flexibility | 9.53 | 3.39 | 6.48 | 3.04 | 7.88 | 3.64 | 6.79 | 3.67 |
Inhibition | 50.13 | 4.45 | 50.41 | 5.67 | 48.50 | 2.98 | 51.50 | 4.38 |
References
- Al-Shidhani, Thuraya Ahmed, and Vinita Arora. 2012. Understanding dyslexia in children through human development theories. Sultan Qaboos University Medical Journal 12: 286–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, Lauren S., Tiffany P. Hogan, Rachel L. Schechter, Pamela E. Hook, and Elizabeth C. Brooke. 2019. Can educational technology effectively differentiate instruction for reader profiles? Reading and Writing 32: 2327–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bexkens, Anika, Wery P. M. Van Den Wildenberg, and Jurgen Tijms. 2015. Rapid automatized naming in children with dyslexia: Is inhibitory control involved? Dyslexia 21: 212–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borella, Erika, Barbara Carretti, and Santiago Pelegrina. 2010. The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Learning Disabilities 43: 541–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckingham, Jennifer, Robyn Beaman, and Kevin Wheldall. 2014. Why poor children are more likely to become poor readers: The early years. Educational Review 66: 428–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burgoyne, Alexander P., Jason S. Tsukahara, Cody A. Mashburn, Richard Pak, and Randall W. Engle. 2023. Nature and measurement of attention control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 152: 2369–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Butterfuss, Reese, and Panayiota Kendeou. 2018. The role of executive functions in reading comprehension. Educational Psychology Review 30: 801–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cadime, Irene, Bruna Rodrigues, Sandra Santos, Fernanda Leopoldina Viana, Séli Chaves-Sousa, Maria do Céu Cosme, and Iolanda Ribeiro. 2017. The role of word recognition, oral reading fluency and listening comprehension in the simple view of reading: A study in an intermediate depth orthography. Reading and Writing 30: 591–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cadime, Irene, Séli Chaves-Sousa, Fernanda L. Viana, Sandra Santos, José Maia, and Iolanda Ribeiro. 2021. Growth, stability and predictors of word reading accuracy in European Portuguese: A longitudinal study from Grade 1 to Grade 4. Current Psychology 40: 5185–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cartwright, Kelly B. 2002. Cognitive development and reading: The relation of reading-specific multiple classification skill to reading comprehension in elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology 94: 56–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cartwright, Kelly B., Elizabeth A. Coppage, Amanda B. Lane, Terrain Singleton, Timothy R. Marshall, and Cassandra Bentivegna. 2017. Cognitive flexibility deficits in children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. Contemporary Educational Psychology 50: 33–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cartwright, Kelly B., Timothy R. Marshall, Cathy M. Huemer, and Joan B. Payne. 2019. Executive function in the classroom: Cognitive flexibility supports reading fluency for typical readers and teacher-identified low-achieving readers. Research in Developmental Disabilities 88: 42–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cartwright, Kelly B., Timothy R. Marshall, Kristina L. Dandy, and Marisa C. Isaac. 2010. The development of graphophonological-semantic cognitive flexibility and its contribution to reading comprehension in beginning readers. Journal of Cognition and Development 11: 61–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cecilia, Maria Rosita, Pierpaolo Vittorini, Vicenza Cofini, and Ferdinando Di Orio. 2014. The prevalence of reading difficulties among children in scholar age. Styles of Communication 6: 18–30. [Google Scholar]
- Cirino, Paul T., Jeremy Miciak, Yusra Ahmed, Marcia A. Barnes, W. Pat Taylor, and Elyssa H. Gerst. 2019. Executive function: Association with multiple reading skills. Reading and Writing 32: 1819–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Dams, Judi E., Moniek M.H. Schaars, Eliane Segers, and Elma Blom. 2023. Understanding variation in prospective poor decoders: A person-centred approach from kindergarten to Grade 2. Dyslexia 29: 312–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daucourt, Mia C., Christopher Schatschneider, Carol M. Connor, Stephanie Al Otaiba, and Sara A. Hart. 2018. Inhibition, updating working memory, and shifting predict reading disability symptoms in a hybrid model: Project KIDS. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, Adele. 2013. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64: 135–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dolean, Dacian Dorin, Arne Lervåg, Laura Visu-Petra, and Monica Melby-Lervåg. 2021. Language skills, and not executive functions, predict the development of reading comprehension of early readers: Evidence from an orthographically transparent language. Reading and Writing 34: 1491–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doyle, Caoilainn, Alan F. Smeaton, Richard A.P. Roche, and Lorraine Boran. 2018. Inhibition and updating, but not switching, predict developmental dyslexia and individual variation in reading ability. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duke, Nell K., and Kelly B. Cartwright. 2021. The science of reading progresses: Communicating advances beyond the Simple View of Reading. Reading Research Quarterly 56: 25–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elwér, Åsa, Janice M. Keenan, Richard K. Olson, Brian Byrne, and Stefan Samuelsson. 2013. Longitudinal stability and predictors of poor oral comprehenders and poor decoders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 115: 497–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engel de Abreu, Pascale M. J., Neander Abreu, Carolina C. Nikaedo, Marina L. Puglisi, Carlos J. Tourinho, Mônica C. Miranda, Debora M. Befi-Lopes, Orlando F. A. Bueno, and Romain Martin. 2014. Executive functioning and reading achievement in school: A study of Brazilian children assessed by their teachers as “poor readers”. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Esgalhado, Graça, and Henrique Pereira. 2012. Efeito do género e da escolaridade no teste stroop: Da infância à adultez jovem. International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology 2: 77–86. [Google Scholar]
- Esgalhado, Graça, Fátima Simões, and Henrique Pereira. 2010. Versão portuguesa do Teste Stroop de Cores e Palavras: Aferição para a infância e adolescência. Lisbon: Edições Placebo. [Google Scholar]
- Feng, Guoyan, Xiaohui Yan, Linling Shen, Kyle Perkins, Jiaqi Mao, Yu Wu, Liping Shi, and Fan Cao. 2023. Distinct neural correlates of poor decoding and poor comprehension in children with reading disability. Cerebral Cortex 33: 3239–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feng, Yanan, Qi Liu, Xinyan Xie, Qi Jiang, Kaiheng Zhu, Pei Xiao, Xiaoqian Wu, Pengxiang Zuo, and Ranran Song. 2022. The prevalence and associated risk factors of children with reading disabilities in a multiethnic city: A cross-sectional study. Frontiers in Pediatrics 10: 864175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Field, Andy. 2009. Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. London: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Follmer, Jake. 2018. Executive function and reading comprehension: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist 53: 42–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friedman, Naomi P., and Akira Miyake. 2004. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133: 101–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gernsbacher, Morton Ann. 1991. Cognitive processes and mechanisms in language comprehension: The structure building framework. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Gordon H. Bower. Cambridge: Academic Press, pp. 217–63. [Google Scholar]
- Gnaedinger, Emily K., Alycia M. Hund, and Matthew S. Hesson-McInnis. 2016. Reading-specific flexibility moderates the relation between reading strategy use and reading comprehension during the elementary years. Mind, Brain, and Education 10: 233–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hedge, Craig, Georgina Powell, and Petroc Sumner. 2018. The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods 50: 1166–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henry, Lucy A., and Caroline Bettenay. 2010. The assessment of executive functioning in children. Child and Adolescent Mental Health 15: 110–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Himi, Samsad Afrin, Markus Bühner, and Sven Hilbert. 2021. Advancing the understanding of the factor structure of executive functioning. Journal of Intelligence 9: 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoover, Wesley A., and Philip B. Gough. 1990. The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing 2: 127–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jerman, Olga, Chandra Reynolds, and H. Lee Swanson. 2012. Does growth in working memory span or executive processes predict growth in reading and math in children with reading disabilities? Learning Disability Quarterly 35: 144–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johann, Verena, Tanja Könen, and Julia Karbach. 2020. The unique contribution of working memory, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and intelligence to reading comprehension and reading speed. Child Neuropsychology 26: 324–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelso, Katrina, Anne Whitworth, and Suze Leitão. 2022. Profiles of oral and reading comprehension in poor comprehenders. Reading and Writing Quarterly 38: 526–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kintsch, Walter. 1988. The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review 95: 163–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kintsch, Walter, and Katherine Rawson. 2005. Comprehension. In The Science of Reading: A Handbook. Edited by Margaret J. Snowling and Charles Hulme. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing, pp. 209–26. [Google Scholar]
- Korkmaz, Selcuk, Dincer Goksuluk, and Gokmen Zararsiz. 2014. MVN: An R package for assessing multivariate normality. The R Jounal 6: 151–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laureys, Felien, Silke De Waelle, Maria T. Barendse, Matthieu Lenoir, and Frederik J. A. Deconinck. 2022. The factor structure of executive function in childhood and adolescence. Intelligence 90: 101600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leach, Jennifer Mirak, Hollis S. Scarborough, and Leslie Rescorla. 2003. Late-emerging reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology 95: 211–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Locascio, Gianna, E. Mark Mahone, Sarah H. Eason, and Laurie E. Cutting. 2010. Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficits. Journal of Learning Disabilities 43: 441–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Löffler, Christoph, Gidon T. Frischkorn, Dirk Hagemann, Kathrin Sadus, and Anna Lena Schubert. 2024. The common factor of executive functions measures nothing but speed of information uptake. Psychological Research 88: 1092–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López-Vallejo, Sofía, Carlos Burneo-Garcés, and Miguel Pérez-García. 2024. Development of working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Cross-sectional analysis by age intervals and gender in Ecuadorian preschoolers. PLoS ONE 19: e299394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moura, Octávio, Cristina P. Albuquerque, Maria Salomé Pinho, Manuela Vilar, Ana Filipa Lopes, Isabel Alberto, Marcelino Pereira, Maria João Seabra-Santos, and Mário R. Simões. 2018. Factor structure and measurement invariance of the Coimbra neuropsychological assessment battery (BANC). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 33: 66–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moura, Octávio, Mário R. Simões, and Marcelino Pereira. 2015. Executive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. Clinical Neuropsychologist 28: 20–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nation, Kate. 2004. To read but not to read: Identifying and understanding the nature of poor reading comprehension in children. In Students with Both Gifts and Learning Disabilities: Identification, Assessment, and Outcomes. Edited by Tina A. Newman and Robert J. Sternberg. Boston: Springer, pp. 119–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Noble, Kimberly G., Martha J. Farah, and Bruce D. McCandliss. 2006. Socioeconomic background modulates cognition–achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development 21: 349–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norton, Elizabeth S., and Maryanne Wolf. 2012. Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology 63: 427–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nouwens, Suzan, Margriet A. Groen, Tijs Kleemans, and Ludo Verhoeven. 2021. How executive functions contribute to reading comprehension. British Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 169–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oakhill, Jane, Joanne Hartt, and Deborah Samols. 2005. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Reading and Writing 18: 657–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ober, Teresa M., Patricia J. Brooks, Bruce D. Homer, and David Rindskopf. 2020. Executive functions and decoding in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic investigation. Educational Psychology Review 32: 735–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ober, Teresa M., Patricia J. Brooks, Jan L. Plass, and Bruce D. Homer. 2019. Distinguishing direct and indirect effects of executive functions on reading comprehension in adolescents. Reading Psychology 40: 551–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ozernov-Palchik, Ola, Elizabeth S. Norton, Georgios Sideridis, Sara D. Beach, Maryanne Wolf, John D. E. Gabrieli, and Nadine Gaab. 2017. Longitudinal stability of pre-reading skill profiles of kindergarten children: Implications for early screening and theories of reading. Developmental Science 20: 139–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papadopoulos, Timothy C., George C. Spanoudis, and George K. Georgiou. 2016. How is RAN related to reading fluency? A comprehensive examination of the prominent theoretical accounts. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Potocki, Anna, Monique Sanchez, Jean Ecalle, and Annie Magnan. 2017. Linguistic and cognitive profiles of 8- to 15-year-old children with specific reading comprehension difficulties: The role of executive functions. Journal of Learning Disabilities 50: 128–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Powell, Daisy, and Lynette Atkinson. 2021. Unraveling the links between rapid automatized naming (RAN), phonological awareness, and reading. Journal of Educational Psychology 113: 706–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qiu, Yani, Sarah Griffiths, Courtenay Norbury, and Jo S. H. Taylor. 2023. Inhibitory control predicts growth in irregular word reading: Evidence from a large-scale longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology 59: 2367–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodrigues, Bruna, Irene Cadime, and Iolanda Ribeiro. 2023. Cognitive and metacognitive strategy use in poor comprehenders: An exploratory study. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties 28: 139–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodrigues, Bruna, Irene Cadime, Tânia Freitas, Celda Choupina, Adriana Baptista, Fernanda Leopoldina Viana, and Iolanda Ribeiro. 2022. Assessing oral reading fluency within and across grade levels: Development of equated test forms. Behavior Research Methods 54: 3043–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rollins, Lauren Hart, Sara Sanders, Kristine Jolivette, and Ashley S. Virgin. 2022. Assessment of strategy instruction and self-regulation in reading comprehension: A review of the literature. Preventing School Failure 66: 301–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Santos, Sandra, Irene Cadime, Fernanda L. Viana, Séli Chaves-Sousa, Elena Gayo, José Maia, and Iolanda Ribeiro. 2017. Assessing reading comprehension with narrative and expository texts: Dimensionality and relationship with fluency, vocabulary and memory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 58: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Starr, Ariel, Elena R. Leib, Jessica W. Younger, Melina R. Uncapher, and Silvia A. Bunge. 2023. Relational thinking: An overlooked component of executive functioning. Developmental Science 26: 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tabachnick, Barbara G., and Linda S. Fidell. 2013. Using Multivariate Statistics, 6th ed. Upper Saddle River: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Tabullo, Ángel Javier, Juan Manuel Benegas, and Santiago Vernucci. 2023. The role of executive functions in the comprehension of expository and narrative texts by high school students. Suma Psicologica 30: 30–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taub, Gordon E., and Judit Szente. 2012. The impact of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness on the reading fluency of a minority student population. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 26: 359–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torppa, Minna, Asko Tolvanen, and Anna-Maija Poikkeus. 2007. Reading development subtypes and their early characteristics. Annals of Dyslexia 57: 3–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- U.S. Department of Education. 2021. Students with Disabilities; Washington, DC: Institute for Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics. Available online: https://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator/cgg (accessed on 11 February 2024).
- Vadasy, Patricia F., Elizabeth A. Sanders, and Kelly B. Cartwright. 2023. Cognitive flexibility in beginning decoding and encoding. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk 28: 412–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vale, Ana Paula, Ana Sucena, and Fernanda L. Viana. 2011. Prevalência da dislexia entre crianças do 1.o ciclo do ensino básico falantes do Português Europeu [Prevalence of dyslexia among children from first-to-fourth grades in the European-Portuguese orthography]. Revista Lusófona de Educação 18: 45–56. [Google Scholar]
- Van Reybroeck, Marie, and Margot De Rom. 2020. Children with dyslexia show an inhibition domain-specific deficit in reading. Reading and Writing 33: 907–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, Richard K., Fotena A. Zirps, Ashley A. Edwards, Sarah G. Wood, Rachel E. Joyner, Betsy J. Becker, Guangyun Liu, and Bethany Beal. 2020. The prevalence of dyslexia: A new approach to its estimation. Journal of Learning Disabilities 53: 354–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, David. 2003. Escala de Inteligência de Wechsler para Crianças—Terceira edição (WISC—III). Lisbon: Cegoc-Tea Edições. [Google Scholar]
- Wise, Justin C., Hye Kyeong Pae, Christopher B. Wolfe, Rose A. Sevcik, Robin D. Morris, Maureen Lovett, and Maryanne Wolf. 2008. Phonological awareness and rapid naming skills of children with reading disabilities and children with reading disabilities who are at risk for mathematics difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice 23: 125–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, Maryanne, and Patricia G. Bowers. 1999. The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 415–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Mengjiao, and Christopher A. Was. 2023. The relationship between executive functions and metacognition in college students. Journal of Intelligence 11: 220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zou, Zhenfei, Wei Zhao, and Miao Li. 2022. The deficit profile of executive function in Chinese children with different types of reading difficulties. Reading and Writing 35: 565–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
n | Additional Educational Support | Age | Sex | Low SES | Maternal Education | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n (%) | M (SD) | Male n (%) | Female n (%) | n (%) | Grade 1–9 n (%) | Grade 10–12 n (%) | Higher Education n (%) | ||
Dysfluent readers | 24 | 5 (20.8) | 8.30 (0.43) | 15 (62.5) | 9 (37.5) | 8 (33.3) | 14 (58.3) | 3 (12.5) | 7 (29.2) |
Poor comprehenders | 8 | 1 (12.5) | 8.33 (0.46) | 6 (75.0) | 2 (25.0) | 1 (12.5) | 1 (12.5) | 4 (50.0) | 3 (37.5) |
Poor readers | 29 | 7 (24.1) | 8.29 (0.56) | 12 (41.4) | 17 (58.6) | 13 (44.8) | 11 (37.9) | 12 (41.4) | 6 (20.7) |
Typical readers | 30 | 2 (6.7) | 8.53 (0.27) | 18 (60.0) | 12 (40.0) | 3 (10.0) | 5 (17.2) | 10 (34.5) | 14 (48.3) |
Executive Functioning Dimensions | M (SD) | 1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | Low SES |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Rapid Alternating Stimulus—Shapes/colors | 9.00 (3.66) | 1 | 0.559 *** | 0.223 * | 0.355 *** | 0.301 ** | 0.209 * | 0.315 ** | 0.424 *** | −0.020 | −0.297 ** |
2. Rapid Automatized Naming—Digits | 8.04 (3.21) | 1 | 0.295 ** | 0.356 *** | 0.125 | 0.013 | 0.084 | 0.206 | −0.274 ** | −0.255 * | |
3. Processing Speed—Coding | 9.77 (3.10) | 1 | 0.462 *** | 0.088 | 0.043 | 0.279 ** | 0.232 * | 0.008 | −0.122 | ||
4. Processing Speed—Symbol Search | 11.77 (2.75) | 1 | 0.320 ** | 0.115 | 0.416 *** | 0.396 *** | −0.080 | −0.128 | |||
5. Planning—Maze | 11.04 (2.77) | 1 | 0.194 | 0.123 | 0.057 | −0.194 | −0.073 | ||||
6. Planning—Tower | 10.32 (3.06) | 1 | 0.200 | −0.008 | 0.187 | 0.065 | |||||
7. Working Memory | 9.02 (2.15) | 1 | 0.352 *** | 0.073 | −0.098 | ||||||
8. Cognitive Flexibility | 7.69 (3.60) | 1 | 0.089 | −0.271 ** | |||||||
9. Inhibition | 50.44 (4.76) | 1 | −0.047 |
Executive Functioning Dimensions | F(3, 87) | p | η2p |
---|---|---|---|
Rapid Alternating Stimulus—Shapes/colors | 12.102 | <.001 | 0.294 |
Rapid Automatized Naming—Digits | 8.951 | <.001 | 0.236 |
Processing speed—Coding | 1.038 | .380 | 0.035 |
Processing speed—Symbol Search | 2.679 | .052 | 0.085 |
Planning—Maze | 1.762 | .160 | 0.057 |
Planning—Tower | 2.002 | .120 | 0.065 |
Working Memory | 3.766 | .014 | 0.115 |
Cognitive Fexibility | 4.773 | .004 | 0.141 |
Inhibition | 0.878 | .456 | 0.029 |
Function | Eigenvalue | % Variance | Canonical R2 | Wilk’s Lambda | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 0.615 | 71.6 | 0.38 | ||
2 | 0.188 | 21.9 | 0.16 | ||
3 | 0.055 | 6.4 | 0.05 | ||
1 through 3 | 0.494 | <.001 | |||
2 through 3 | 0.798 | .274 | |||
Only 3 | 0.948 | .723 |
Executive Functioning Dimensions | Discriminant Functions | ||
---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | |
Rapid Alternating Stimulus—Shapes/colors | 0.805 | 0.248 | −0.363 |
Rapid Automatized Naming—Digits | 0.658 | −0.414 | −0.431 |
Cognitive Flexibility | 0.494 | 0.204 | 0.350 |
Planning—Tower | −0.044 | 0.529 | −0.524 |
Working Memory | 0.391 | 0.396 | 0.335 |
Planning—Maze | 0.241 | 0.363 | 0.061 |
Processing Speed—Symbol Search | 0.333 | 0.342 | −0.189 |
Inhibition | −0.146 | 0.282 | 0.198 |
Processing Speed—Coding | 0.202 | −0.234 | −0.092 |
Group | Predicted Group Membership | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Typical Readers | Poor Readers | Poor Comprehenders | Dysfluent Readers | |
Original | ||||
Typical readers | 73.3 | 13.3 | 3.3 | 10.0 |
Poor readers | 6.9 | 65.5 | 3.4 | 24.1 |
Poor comprehenders | 62.5 | 37.5 | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Dysfluent readers | 20.8 | 25.0 | 0.0 | 54.2 |
Cross-validated | ||||
Typical readers | 66.7 | 16.7 | 3.3 | 13.3 |
Poor readers | 6.9 | 44.8 | 10.3 | 37.9 |
Poor comprehenders | 62.5 | 37.5 | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Dysfluent readers | 25.0 | 37.5 | 4.2 | 33.3 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Cadime, I.; Rodrigues, B.; Ribeiro, I.; Martín-Aragoneses, M.T. Executive Functioning in Different Types of Reading Disabilities. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100101
Cadime I, Rodrigues B, Ribeiro I, Martín-Aragoneses MT. Executive Functioning in Different Types of Reading Disabilities. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(10):101. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100101
Chicago/Turabian StyleCadime, Irene, Bruna Rodrigues, Iolanda Ribeiro, and María Teresa Martín-Aragoneses. 2024. "Executive Functioning in Different Types of Reading Disabilities" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 10: 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100101
APA StyleCadime, I., Rodrigues, B., Ribeiro, I., & Martín-Aragoneses, M. T. (2024). Executive Functioning in Different Types of Reading Disabilities. Journal of Intelligence, 12(10), 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100101