A Study on the Design of Embedded Visual Image Teaching Aids to Assist Young Children’s Cognitive and Fine Motor Development
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- What are the perceived differences in effectiveness and functionality between traditional visual aids and the aids developed in this study, as reported by education and healthcare professionals?
- How does the use of the developed teaching aids influence the cognitive development and fine motor learning of children, based on assessments conducted before and after their use?
2. Literature Review
2.1. The Concept and Theoretical Foundation of Visual Cognition in Young Children
- Emergence: Our eyes seek to identify an object’s outline, and by comparing it with past memories, we can quickly recognize the object. This principle represents the holistic nature of human vision.
- Reification: When our vision is stimulated, and we interpret spatial information from the external environment, our brains tend to “fill in” missing details, creating a complete message for understanding observed objects.
- Multi-stability (Organization): If an object has more than one possible interpretation, our brains can only process one interpretation at a time, although we can switch between different interpretations.
- Invariance: This principle highlights the constancy of our vision. Regardless of how an object changes (e.g., deforms, rotates, enlarges, or shrinks), we can still recognize it through its outline or features.
2.2. Establishing the Foundation for Visual Learning in Early Childhood from a Dynamic Systems Perspective
3. Research Methodology
3.1. Design Concepts of Visual Graphic Teaching Aids
3.2. Design Framework for Visual Graphic Teaching Aids
- Visual attention game board: This board uses patterns on visual cards to stimulate the interaction between vision, brain cognition, and spatial perception, guiding children to develop an understanding of object concepts.
- Visual memory game board: Children practice matching images through memory-based operations. This activity helps them confirm the location of objects based on the icons displayed on the cards, reinforcing their memory skills.
- Visual discrimination game board: Through the manipulation of picture cards, children learn to identify spatial relationships, distinguish differences in themes and object details, and engage in activities such as identification, pairing, and classification. These exercises help connect visual stimuli with memory, supporting cognitive development in young children.
- Visual imagination construction games: Using finger puppets and situational picture cards, children practice storytelling and develop their oral expression skills, fostering creativity and imagination.
- Visual pursuit game: This game encourages children to use visual tracking by working with graphics and symbols in various colors. They can classify, identify, or trace paths on the game board, enhancing their visual processing abilities.
- Fine motor operation practice: Children engage in exercises that involve grasping, holding, twisting, turning, pulling, pressing, and other fine motor skills. These activities are integrated with visual–motor development exercises, allowing children to practice coordinated actions using the teaching aids.
3.3. Research Design
3.4. Participants
3.5. Research Instruments
- Questionnaire on the Functions and Characteristics of Teaching Aids
- 2.
- Structured Observational Assessment
4. Results
4.1. Comparative Analysis of the Effectiveness of Traditional Teaching Aids and Visual Aids
4.2. Differences in Educational Care Providers’ Views on Features and Functions of Teaching
4.3. Structural Observation of Children’s Visual Graphics Learning Assessment Results
5. Discussion and Conclusions
5.1. Educational Service Personnel’s Views on Visual Graphic Aids and Traditional Teaching Aids in This Study
5.2. Children’s Performance in Structural Assessment of Operating Visual Image Teaching Aids
5.3. Learning the Process of Using Visual Image Teaching Aids
5.4. Recommendations
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Baddeley, Alan D., and Graham Hitch. 1974. Working Memory. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Gordon H. Bower. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, vol. 8, pp. 47–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumgartner, Elisabeth, Christiane B. Wiebel, and Karl R. Gegenfurtner. 2015. A Comparison of Haptic Material Perception in Blind and Sighted Individuals. Vision Research 115: 238–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bruner, Jerome S., Rose R. Olver, Patricia M. Greenfield, and Center for Cognitive Studies, Harvard University. 1966. Studies in Cognitive Growth: A Collaboration at the Center for Cognitive Studies; New York: Wiley. Available online: https://greenfieldlab.psych.ucla.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/168/2019/01/02-gb1966.pdf (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Chen, Chun-Ching, and Jiayang Ma. 2022. Use Grounded Theory to Explore Micro-Interaction Design Techniques. Journal of Design 27: 1–23. Available online: https://www.airitilibrary.com/Article/Detail/16068327-202206-202207010015-202207010015-1-23 (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Chou, Shuhui. 2017. Early Childhood Education for the 21st Century-Inquiry-Oriented Themed Courses. Taipei: Psychological Publishing Co., Ltd., pp. 113–69. [Google Scholar]
- Cleland, Frances E., and David L. Gallahue. 1993. Young Children’s Divergent Movement Ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills 77: 535–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale: L. Erlbaum Associates, pp. 35–134. [Google Scholar]
- Feest, Uljana. 2021. Gestalt Psychology, Frontloading Phenomenology, and Psychophysics. Synthese 198: 2153–73. Available online: https://philpapers.org/archive/FEEGPF.pdf (accessed on 12 April 2024). [CrossRef]
- Gallahue, David L., and John C. Ozmun. 2006. Understanding Motor Development: Infants, Children, Adolescents, Adults, 6th ed. Boston: McGraw Hill, pp. 2–63. [Google Scholar]
- Ghatala, Elizabeth S., and Joel R. Levin. 1973. Developmental Differences in Frequency Judgments of Words and Pictures. In ERIC, Resources in Education (RIE). pp. 1–21. Available online: https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/developmental-differences-frequency-judgments/docview/64122334/se-2 (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Gibson, James J. 2014. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception: Classic Edition, 1st ed. London: Psychology Press, p. 22. [Google Scholar]
- Heft, Harry. 2020. Ecological psychology and enaction theory: Divergent groundings. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hommel, Bernhard. 2019. Theory of Event Coding (TEC) V2.0: Representing and Controlling Perception and Action. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 81: 2139–54. Available online: https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13414-019-01779-4 (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Huang, Hsin-fu, Hao-yu Chuang, and Sheng-Yuan Huang. 2020. Tactile Information Processing Model of the Young Visually Im paired and Life Product Design Study. Design Research 23: 61–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kephart, Newell C. 1971. The Slow Learner in the Classroom. Second Edition. In ERIC, Resources in Education (RIE). pp. 1–438. Available online: https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/slow-learner-classroom-second-edition/docview/64253003/se-2 (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Ladd, Gary W., Sondra H. Birch, and Eric S. Buhs. 1999. Children’s Social and Scholastic Lives in Kindergarten: Related Spheres of Influence? Child Development 70: 1373–400. Available online: https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/childrens-social-scholastic-lives-kindergarten/docview/62403670/se-2 (accessed on 12 April 2024). [CrossRef]
- Leikin, Roza, and Bharath Sriraman. 2022. Empirical research on creativity in mathematics (education): From the wastelands of psychology to the current state of the art. ZDM-International Journal on Mathematics Education 54: 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levitin, Daniel J. 2002. Foundations of Cognitive Psychology: Core Readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, p. 399. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, Hsien-Yung. 2018. The Origins of Theories Related to Children’s Motor Development and Physical Ability Development. Chinese Journal of Physical Education 32: 165–73. Available online: https://www.hyread.com.tw/hyreadnew/search_detail_new.jsp?dtd_id=3&sysid=00506117 (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Makransky, Guido, and Gustav B. Petersen. 2021. The Cognitive Affective Model of Immersive Learning (CAMIL): A Theoretical Research-Based Model of Learning in Immersive Virtual Reality. Educational Psychology Review 33: 937–58. Available online: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10648-020-09586-2 (accessed on 12 April 2024). [CrossRef]
- Montello, Daniel R., Mary Hegarty, Anthony E. Richardson, and David Waller. 2004. Spatial Memory of Real Environments, Virtual Environments, and Maps. In Human Spatial Memory: Remembering Where. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 251–85. [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor, Rollanda E., and Joseph R. Jenkins. 1999. Prediction of Reading Disabilities in Kindergarten and First Grade. Scientific Studies of Reading 3: 159–97. Available online: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED405563.pdf (accessed on 12 April 2024). [CrossRef]
- Ruble, Diane N., Carol Lynn Martin, and Sheri A. Berenbaum. 2006. Gender Development. In Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, 6th ed. Edited by Nancy Eisenberg, William Damon and Richard M. Lerner. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: pp. 858–932. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2006-08776-014 (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Sahin, Feride, and Salih Ates. 2023. The Examination of the Relationship between Scientific Literacy and some Cognitively Based Individual Differences in Terms of Gender. Electronic Journal for Research in Science & Mathematics Education 27: 120–39. Available online: https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/examination-relationship-between-scientific/docview/2890010527/se-2 (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Schneekloth, Lynda H. 1989. Play Environments for Visually Impaired Children. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness 83: 196–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegel, Alexander W., and Sheldon H. White. 1975. The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. Advances in Child Development and Behavior 10: 9–55. Available online: https://typeset.io/papers/the-development-of-spatial-representations-of-large-scale-2vac6fj4ml (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1984. Toward a triarchic theory of human intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Science 7: 269–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sutton, Griffin P., Kimberly A. Barchard, Danielle T. Bello, Nicholas S. Thaler, Erik Ringdahl, Joan Mayfield, and Daniel N. Allen. 2011. Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration performance in children with traumatic brain injury and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Assessment 23: 805–9. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/21875221/ (accessed on 12 April 2024). [CrossRef]
- Takaya, Keiichi. 2015. Bruner’s Theory of Cognitive Development. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences 2: 880–85. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/304183714_Bruner’s_Theory_of_Cognitive_Development (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Thelen, Esther, and Linda B. Smith. 1995. A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Journal of Cognitive Neuro Science 7: 512–14. Available online: https://www.academia.edu/730236/A_dynamic_systems_approach_to_the_development_of_cognition_and_action (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Trevlas, Efthimios, Ourania Matsouka, and Evridiki Zachopoulou. 2003. Relationship between Playfulness and Motor Creativity in Preschool Children. Early Child Development and Care 173: 535–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tröster, Heinrich, and Michael Brambrin. 1970. The play behavior and play materials of blind and sighted infants and preschool ers. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness 88: 421–32. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232557142_The_play_behavior_and_play_materials_of_blind_and_sighted_infants_and_preschoolers (accessed on 12 April 2024).
- Verver, Suzanne H., Mathijs P. J. Vervloed, and Bert Steenbergen. 2020. Facilitating Play and Social Interaction between Children with Visual Impairments and Sighted Peers by Means of Augmented Toys. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities 32: 93–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagemans, Johan, James H. Elder, Michael Kubovy, Stephen E. Palmer, Mary A. Peterson, Manish Singh, and Rüdiger von der Heydt. 2021. A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization. Psychological Bulletin 138: 1172–217. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22845751/ (accessed on 12 April 2024). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Warren, Mary. 1993. A Hierarchical Model for Evaluation and Treatment of Visual Perceptual Dysfunction in Adult Acquired Brain Injury, Part 2. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 47: 55–66. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8418677/ (accessed on 12 April 2024). [CrossRef]
- Warren, Mary. 2011. Evaluation and Treatment of Visual Deficits. In Occupational Therapy: Practice Skills for Physical Dysfunction. Edited by Lorraine Pedretti. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., pp. 386–421. [Google Scholar]
- Zimbardo, Philip G., and Richard J. Gerring. 2009. Psychology and Life. London: Pearson Education, pp. 361–3. [Google Scholar]
Development Installment | Learning Content |
---|---|
Enactive representation | Toddlers explore through movement to learn about their surroundings and encode and store the perceived movement messages in their memories, laying the foundation for learning. |
Iconic representation | Young children learn about the world around them through exploratory experiences and learning that generates perceptual experiences and mental images, and cognitive learning through representational constructions. |
Symbolic representation | Young children learn through symbols, words, and language. Language can be used to encode information received and to help construct and think about cognitive concepts. |
Assessment Tools | Assessment Age | Assessment Ability | Assessment Time | Assessment Method |
---|---|---|---|---|
The Berry-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration (VMI) | 2–18 years old | Draw 27 geometric figures in order of difficulty with pen and paper | 10–15 min | Individual testing |
Movement Assessment Battery for Children for Children, Second (Movement ABC) | 3–16 years old | Hand operation Ball skills Balance ability | 20–30 min | Individual testing |
Test of Visual Motor Skills (TVMS) | 2–13 years old | Graphic copying | 20–30 min | Individual testing |
Peabody Developmental Motor Scales (PDMS) | 0–6 years old | Reflection Shift Grasp visual–motor coordination | 20–30 min | Individual testing |
Gestalt Theory | Design Principles | Teaching Focus |
---|---|---|
Similarity A combination of image elements with similar characteristics, either as a group or as a whole. | Children are provided with guidance to facilitate their recognition of features through identifying similarities among parts. They are also encouraged to identify the features and similarities between shapes or symbols, promoting their awareness of graphic patterns and norms. | |
Proximity Graphic elements and closely spaced or related blocks are treated as a group or as a whole. | Children are directed to observe and examine the characteristics and positions of images, fostering their ability to recognize similar images as part of the same space or whole through engaging in classification and categorization activities. | |
Continuity An image is presented with a continuous effect of movement or character, or in a regular pattern that leads the eye to make a connection. | Children are provided with guidance to observe and analyze the representations of continuity within visual sequences of images. This practice enables them to develop their ability to recognize relationships between images, collect relevant information, and further organize the logical structure of the information. These skills serve as a foundation for judgment and critical thinking. | |
Closure An image that characterizes elements that are closed or an image that causes the eye to perceive the composition of separate elements as a whole. | Children are guided to focus on features that belong to each other or that are closed between images to help recognize complex shapes and can practice recognizing changes in the shape of objects for manipulative learning. | |
Figure-ground The theme of the image is separated from the surrounding area, and the foreground is more prominent, so that different perceptions are created by different focuses of attention. | Children are guided to focus their attention on the target to distinguish individual and separate elements of the image through the foreground and background of the image and to integrate the boundaries between the two and their separate forms to facilitate the organization and recognition of the image. | |
Common Fate Patterns in which patterns and elements of an image are dynamically oriented so that the visual sense perceives them as a whole. | Children are guided to explore the directionality of visual images in relation to their position and to observe the position of the images, and to transform manipulation and movement in the same direction and dynamic. | |
Symmetry Stability and symmetry in the presentation of an image so that the eye naturally perceives it as a whole. | Children are guided to observe the graphic symmetry of visual images so that the operating parts and blocks can show symmetry and stability and can operate according to the sequence and steps of the parts provided in the teaching aids. | |
Common Region Principle Images are shown in a common region that presents the image’s regional boundaries, but is treated as an individual group. | Children are guided to observe images in terms of colors or lines and are able to practice recognizing colors in marked areas, treating common blocks as a group, and using hand–eye coordination to make arrangements and adjustments. |
Title | Content | Factor I | Factor II |
---|---|---|---|
10 | Teaching toys can help toddlers learn thinking and problem-solving skills. | 0.73 | |
8 | Teaching toys can satisfy children’s curiosity to explore and learn. | 0.66 | |
12 | Teaching toys are educational, practical, reusable and economical. | 0.64 | |
1 | Teaching toys designed with visual graphics and symbols with creative designs. | 0.87 | |
11 | Teaching toys can provide visual image cognitive learning. | 0.86 | |
5 | Teaching toys are designed with diversified and rich ways of play to enhance children’s learning enjoyment. | 0.76 | |
6 | Teaching toys can provide different visual and tactile stimulation and learning for toddlers. | 0.69 | |
4 | Teaching toys can enhance the fine motor development of toddlers. | 0.63 | |
7 | Teaching toys can develop children’s self-care skills. | 0.57 | |
Earmark | 6.44 | 1.23 | |
Variation (%) | 53.68 | 10.21 | |
Cumulative Interpretable Variables (%) | 53.68 | 63.89 |
Question | Group | Traditional Teaching Aids | Visual Graphic Aids | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Content | Mean | Standard Deviation | Mean | Standard Deviation | |
9 | Connecting teaching toys with children’s life experiences to arouse children’s interest in learning. | 1.90 | 1.90 | 4.86 | 0.35 |
2 | The overall design of teaching toys ranges from easy to difficult, and the gradual progression can guide children to explore learning opportunities. | 2.53 | 0.81 | 4.90 | 0.35 |
3 | Teaching toys provide hands-on learning opportunities for young children. | 2.69 | 0.84 | 4.91 | 0.28 |
10 | Teaching toys can help toddlers learn thinking and problem solving skills. | 1.76 | 1.07 | 4.87 | 0.34 |
8 | Teaching toys can satisfy children’s curiosity to explore and learn. | 1.90 | 1.08 | 4.86 | 0.35 |
12 | Teaching toys are educational, practical, reusable and economical. | 1.77 | 1.07 | 4.91 | 0.28 |
Question | Group | Traditional Aids | Visual Graphic Aids | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Content | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
1 | Creative Designs for Teaching Toys. | 2.76 | 0.71 | 4.94 | 0.23 |
4 | Teaching toys can enhance the development of fine motor skills of young children. | 2.24 | 0.94 | 4.91 | 0.28 |
5 | The design of educational toys is diversified, which enhances the enjoyment of learning in young children. | 1.91 | 0.96 | 4.89 | 0.32 |
6 | Teaching toys can provide toddlers with different visual and tactile stimulation. | 2.01 | 0.91 | 4.84 | 0.37 |
7 | Teaching toys can cultivate children’s self-care ability. | 1.96 | 1.08 | 4.87 | 0.34 |
11 | Teaching toys can provide visual image cognitive learning. | 1.86 | 1.11 | 4.93 | 0.31 |
Background Variables | Age | Count | Mean | SD | F |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | (1) 21–30 | 21 | 4.90 | 0.18 | 0.10 |
(2) 31–40 | 22 | 4.88 | 0.19 | ||
(3) 41–50 | 27 | 4.88 | 0.29 | ||
Education level | (1) No university degree | 23 | 4.88 | 0.19 | 0.05 |
(2) University | 47 | 4.89 | 0.25 | ||
Type of facility | (1) Kindergarten | 63 | 4.88 | 0.24 | 1.21 |
(2) Infant care centers | 7 | 4.98 | 0.63 | ||
Years of experience | (1) Less than 5 | 35 | 4.93 | 0.15 | 2.49 |
(2) 5 years and above | 35 | 4.84 | 0.28 | ||
Age of children | (1) 2–3 | 9 | 4.96 | 0.11 | 1.32 |
(2) 3–4 | 24 | 4.90 | 0.27 | ||
(3) 4–5 | 23 | 4.91 | 0.17 | ||
(4) 5–6 | 14 | 4.89 | 0.28 |
Background Variables | Age | Count | Mean | SD | F |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | (1) 21–30 | 21 | 4.94 | 0.13 | 0.80 |
(2) 31–40 | 22 | 4.91 | 0.18 | ||
(3) 41–50 | 26 | 4.86 | 0.29 | ||
Education level | (1) No university degree | 23 | 4.94 | 0.11 | 1.42 |
(2) University and above | 47 | 4.88 | 0.25 | ||
Type of facility | (1) Kindergarten | 63 | 4.89 | 0.22 | 0.15 |
(2) Infant care centers | 7 | 4.93 | 0.19 | ||
Years of experience | (1) Less than 5 | 35 | 4.95 | 0.12 | 4.63 * |
(2) 5 years and above | 35 | 4.84 | 0.28 | ||
Age of children | (1) 2–3 | 9 | 4.98 | 0.56 | 1.26 |
(2) 3–4 | 24 | 4.90 | 0.25 | ||
(3) 4–5 | 23 | 4.91 | 0.14 | ||
(4) 5–6 | 14 | 4.81 | 0.31 |
Measurement | Pre-Test | Post-Test | t | Cohen’s d | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||
Patten recognition | 3.10 | 0.28 | 4.48 | 0.47 | 22.70 * | 0.43 |
Patten understanding | 2.89 | 0.27 | 4.26 | 0.34 | 24.04 * | 0.40 |
Spatial thinking | 3.00 | 0.17 | 4.43 | 0.47 | 26.78 * | 0.49 |
Fine motor moments | 3.11 | 0.27 | 4.62 | 0.44 | 24.92 * | 0.49 |
Measurement | F-Value | p-Value |
---|---|---|
Patten recognition | 1.13 | .289 |
Patten understanding | 0.43 | .514 |
Spatial thinking | 0.21 | .651 |
Fine motor moments | 0.36 | .549 |
Measurement Items | Source of Variation | SS | df | MS | F-Value | p-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Patten recognition | pre-test | 2.261 | 1 | 2.261 | 12.163 ** | .001 |
Gender | 1 | 0.000 | .988 | |||
Patten understanding | pre-test | 0.175 | 1 | 0.175 | 1.501 | .226 |
Gender | 0.025 | 1 | 0.025 | 0.217 | .643 | |
Spatial thinking | pre-test | 0.093 | 1 | 0.093 | 0.409 | .525 |
Gender | 0.237 | 1 | 0.237 | 1.045 | .311 | |
Fine motor moments | pre-test | 0.140 | 1 | 0.140 | 0.713 | .402 |
Gender | 0.035 | 1 | 0.035 | 0.176 | .676 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lo, H.-C.; Wang, T.-H. A Study on the Design of Embedded Visual Image Teaching Aids to Assist Young Children’s Cognitive and Fine Motor Development. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 102. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100102
Lo H-C, Wang T-H. A Study on the Design of Embedded Visual Image Teaching Aids to Assist Young Children’s Cognitive and Fine Motor Development. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(10):102. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100102
Chicago/Turabian StyleLo, Hua-Chen, and Tzu-Hua Wang. 2024. "A Study on the Design of Embedded Visual Image Teaching Aids to Assist Young Children’s Cognitive and Fine Motor Development" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 10: 102. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100102
APA StyleLo, H. -C., & Wang, T. -H. (2024). A Study on the Design of Embedded Visual Image Teaching Aids to Assist Young Children’s Cognitive and Fine Motor Development. Journal of Intelligence, 12(10), 102. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100102