Metadiscourse, Cohesion, and Engagement in L2 Written Discourse
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Literature Review
2.1. Organizational Quality in L2 Texts
2.2. Textual Organizational Devices in L2 Texts
2.3. Development of L2 Textual Organzational Skills
- What kinds of textual organizational features exist in low-score, middle-score, and high-score L2 Chinese descriptive essays, respectively, and how are the organizational features different across the groups?
- What are the interrelations among various textual organizational features in L2 Chinese descriptive essays?
- How do textual organizational features relate to linguistic features in L2 Chinese descriptive essays?
3. Methods
3.1. Participants and Dataset
3.2. Measures of Textual Organizational Features
我 | 星期五 | 晚上 | 跟 | 朋友们 | 吃 | 很 | 好吃 | 的 | 饭。 |
Wo | xingqiwu | wanshang | gen | pengyoumen | chi | hen | haochi | de | fan. |
I | Friday | evening | with | friends | eat | very | delicious | Auxiliary | food |
I eat very delicious food with friends on Friday evenings. |
我 | 对 | 我的 | 大学 | 有 | 很 | 深 | 的 | 了解。 |
Wo | dui | wode | daxue | you | hen | shen | de | liaojie. |
I | to | my | university | have | very | deep | Auxiliary knowledge | |
I have very deep knowledge of my university. |
3.3. Analysis
4. Results
4.1. RQ1: Texual Organizational Features in the Essays
4.2. Interrelations among Textual Organizational Features
4.3. Interrelations between Textual Organizational and Linguistic Features
5. Discussion
5.1. RQ1: Textual Organizational Features in the Essays
5.2. Interrelations among the Textual Organizational Features
5.3. Interrelations between Textual Organizational Features and Linguistic Features
6. Implications
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Post-Hoc Analysis Results for Organizational Measures
(I) Groups | (J) Groups | Mean Difference (I–J) | Std. Error | p | 95% CI | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower Bound | Upper Bound | ||||||
Continuative/ additive marker | low | mid | 0.0002 | 0.0620 | 1.000 | −0.1529 | 0.1525 |
high | 0.0250 | 0.0600 | 1.000 | −0.1228 | 0.1727 | ||
mid | low | 0.0002 | 0.0620 | 1.000 | −0.1525 | 0.1529 | |
high | 0.0252 | 0.0591 | 1.000 | −0.1205 | 0.1709 | ||
high | low | −0.0250 | 0.0600 | 1.000 | −0.1727 | 0.1228 | |
mid | −0.0252 | 0.0591 | 1.000 | −0.1709 | 0.1205 | ||
Comparison/ contrast marker | low | mid | −0.0602 | 0.0730 | 1.000 | −0.2401 | 0.1196 |
high | −0.0179 | 0.0706 | 1.000 | −0.1919 | 0.1562 | ||
mid | low | 0.0602 | 0.0730 | 1.000 | −0.1196 | 0.2401 | |
high | 0.0424 | 0.0697 | 1.000 | −0.1293 | 0.2141 | ||
high | low | 0.0179 | 0.0706 | 1.000 | −0.1562 | 0.1919 | |
mid | −0.0424 | 0.0697 | 1.000 | −0.2141 | 0.1293 | ||
Causative marker | low | mid | 0.0125 | 0.0665 | 1.000 | −0.1514 | 0.1763 |
high | −0.0709 | 0.0643 | 0.825 | −0.2294 | 0.0877 | ||
mid | low | −0.0125 | 0.0665 | 1.000 | −0.1763 | 0.1514 | |
high | −0.0833 | 0.0635 | 0.582 | −0.2397 | 0.0730 | ||
high | low | 0.0709 | 0.0643 | 0.825 | −0.0877 | 0.2294 | |
mid | 0.0833 | 0.0635 | 0.582 | −0.0730 | 0.2397 | ||
Conditional/ hypothetical marker | low | mid | −0.0440 | 0.0257 | 0.278 | −0.1073 | 0.0194 |
high | −0.0613 * | 0.0249 | 0.050 | −0.1227 | −0.00001 | ||
mid | low | 0.0440 | 0.0257 | 0.278 | −0.0194 | 0.1073 | |
high | −0.0174 | 0.0245 | 1.000 | −0.0778 | 0.0431 | ||
high | low | 0.0613 * | 0.0249 | 0.050 | 0.00001 | 0.1227 | |
mid | 0.0174 | 0.0245 | 1.000 | −0.0431 | 0.0778 | ||
Misuse marker | low | mid | 0.1683 * | 0.0641 | 0.033 | 0.0104 | 0.3262 |
high | 0.1812 * | 0.0620 | 0.015 | 0.0285 | 0.3340 | ||
mid | low | −0.1683 * | 0.0641 | 0.033 | −0.3262 | −0.0104 | |
high | 0.0130 | 0.0612 | 1.000 | −0.1377 | 0.1637 | ||
high | low | −0.1812 * | 0.0620 | 0.015 | −0.3340 | −0.0285 | |
mid | −0.0130 | 0.0612 | 1.000 | −0.1637 | 0.1377 | ||
Preposition | low | mid | 0.0009 | 0.0052 | 1.000 | −0.0120 | 0.0137 |
high | 0.0002 | 0.0050 | 1.000 | −0.0122 | 0.0126 | ||
mid | low | −0.0009 | 0.0052 | 1.000 | −0.0137 | 0.0120 | |
high | −0.0007 | 0.0050 | 1.000 | −0.0129 | 0.0116 | ||
high | low | −0.0002 | 0.0050 | 1.000 | −0.0126 | 0.0122 | |
mid | 0.0007 | 0.0050 | 1.000 | −0.0116 | 0.0129 | ||
Frame marker | low | mid | −0.1816 * | 0.0531 | 0.003 | −0.3123 | −0.0508 |
high | −0.1614 * | 0.0514 | 0.008 | −0.2880 | −0.0348 | ||
mid | low | 0.1816 * | 0.0531 | 0.003 | 0.0508 | 0.3123 | |
high | 0.0202 | 0.0507 | 1.000 | −0.1047 | 0.1450 | ||
high | low | 0.1614 * | 0.0514 | 0.008 | 0.0348 | 0.2880 | |
mid | −0.0202 | 0.0507 | 1.000 | −0.1450 | 0.1047 | ||
Third-person pronoun | low | mid | −0.0007 | 0.0040 | 1.000 | −0.0107 | 0.0093 |
high | −0.0072 | 0.0039 | 0.210 | −0.0169 | 0.0024 | ||
mid | low | 0.0007 | 0.0040 | 1.000 | −0.0093 | 0.0107 | |
high | −0.0065 | 0.0039 | 0.289 | −0.0160 | 0.0030 | ||
high | low | 0.0072 | 0.0039 | 0.210 | −0.0024 | 0.0169 | |
mid | 0.0065 | 0.0039 | 0.289 | −0.0030 | 0.0160 | ||
Third-person pronoun/noun ratio | low | mid | −0.0028 | 0.0154 | 1.000 | −0.0408 | 0.0353 |
high | −0.0230 | 0.0149 | 0.387 | −0.0598 | 0.0138 | ||
mid | low | 0.0028 | 0.0154 | 1.000 | −0.0353 | 0.0408 | |
high | −0.0202 | 0.0147 | 0.524 | −0.0566 | 0.0161 | ||
high | low | 0.0230 | 0.0149 | 0.387 | −0.0138 | 0.0598 | |
mid | 0.0202 | 0.0147 | 0.524 | −0.0161 | 0.0566 | ||
Self-mention marker | low | mid | 0.0649 | 0.0831 | 1.000 | −0.1400 | 0.2697 |
high | 0.0723 | 0.0804 | 1.000 | −0.1259 | 0.2706 | ||
mid | low | −0.065 | 0.0831 | 1.000 | −0.2697 | 0.1400 | |
high | 0.0075 | 0.0793 | 1.000 | −0.1880 | 0.2030 | ||
high | low | −0.0723 | 0.0804 | 1.000 | −0.2706 | 0.1259 | |
mid | −0.0075 | 0.0793 | 1.000 | −0.2030 | 0.1880 | ||
Engagement marker | low | mid | −0.1701 * | 0.0568 | 0.012 | −0.3100 | −0.0302 |
high | −0.1341 | 0.0549 | 0.053 | −0.2695 | 0.0013 | ||
mid | low | 0.1701 * | 0.0568 | 0.012 | 0.0302 | 0.3100 | |
high | 0.0360 | 0.0542 | 1.000 | −0.0975 | 0.1695 | ||
high | low | 0.1341 | 0.0549 | 0.053 | −0.0013 | 0.2695 | |
mid | −0.0360 | 0.0542 | 1.000 | −0.1695 | 0.0975 |
Appendix B. Post-Hoc Analysis Results for Linguistic Measures
Low (n = 19) M (SD) | Middle (n = 20) M (SD) | High (n = 23) M (SD) | |
---|---|---|---|
Ratio of correct clauses | 0.5209 (0.2397) | 0.5510 (0.1196) | 0.6979 (0.0852) |
Lexical diversity | 5.02 (0.90) | 6.41 (0.89) | 7.06 (0.79) |
Clause length | 5.53 (0.89) | 6.51 (0.86) | 7.73 (0.83) |
Source | Dependent Variable | Df | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Groups | Ratio of correct clauses | 2 | 7.779 * | 0.001 |
Lexical diversity | 2 | 30.13 ** | <0.001 | |
Clause length | 2 | 34.685 ** | <0.001 | |
Error | Ratio of correct clauses | 59 | ||
Lexical diversity | 59 | |||
Clause length | 59 |
(I) Groups | (J) Groups | Mean Difference (I–J) | Std. Error | p | 95% CI | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower Bound | Upper Bound | ||||||
Ratio of correct clauses | low | mid | −0.0301 | 0.0505 | 1.000 | −0.1546 | 0.0943 |
high | −0.1770 * | 0.0489 | 0.002 | −0.2974 | −0.0566 | ||
mid | low | 0.0301 | 0.0505 | 1.000 | −0.0943 | 0.1546 | |
high | −0.1469 * | 0.0482 | 0.010 | −0.2657 | −0.0281 | ||
high | low | 0.1770 * | 0.0489 | 0.002 | 0.0566 | 0.2974 | |
mid | 0.1469 * | 0.0482 | 0.010 | 0.0281 | 0.2657 | ||
Lexical diversity | low | mid | −1.3891 ** | 0.2747 | <0.001 | −2.0661 | −0.7121 |
high | −2.0405 ** | 0.2659 | <0.001 | −2.6956 | −1.3853 | ||
mid | low | 1.3891 ** | 0.2747 | <0.001 | 0.7121 | 2.0661 | |
high | −0.6513 * | 0.2622 | 0.048 | −1.2974 | −0.0052 | ||
high | low | 2.0405 ** | 0.2659 | <0.001 | 1.3853 | 2.6956 | |
mid | 0.6513 * | 0.2622 | 0.048 | 0.0052 | 1.2974 | ||
Clause length | low | mid | −0.9734 * | 0.2744 | 0.002 | −1.6496 | −0.2972 |
high | −2.1962 ** | 0.2656 | <0.001 | −2.8506 | −1.5419 | ||
mid | low | 0.9734 * | 0.2744 | 0.002 | 0.2972 | 1.6496 | |
high | −1.2228 ** | 0.2619 | <0.001 | −1.8682 | −0.5775 | ||
high | low | 2.1962 ** | 0.2656 | <0.001 | 1.5419 | 2.8506 | |
mid | 1.2228 ** | 0.2619 | <0.001 | 0.5775 | 1.8682 |
Appendix C. Rating Scale for the Essays
Scores | Proficiency Level | Score Criteria |
---|---|---|
1 | Lower-Beginning | Limited content is presented. The meaning is difficult to understand. Limited formulaic language, such as familiar words or phrases, may be used. No discernible writing structure can be identified. |
2 | Higher-Beginning | Undeveloped content is presented. The meaning is generally comprehensible, but gaps in comprehension occurs. Formulaic language, such as familiar words or phrases, may be used. A very basic and undeveloped writing structure is available. |
3 | Lower-Intermediate | Simple and unsophisticated content is presented. A basic writing structure is available, but it lacks effective cohesion and coherence. The writing style resembles oral discourse and the writing communicates limited information to the audience. |
4 | Higher-Intermediate | Some variety of ideas is presented, but is often unsophisticated. A basic writing structure is available with some coherence and cohesion. The writing style resembles oral discourse and the writing communicates some basic information to the audience. |
5 | Lower-Advanced | A good variety of ideas is presented with some elaboration. An organized writing structure is presented with good coherence and cohesion. An introduction, elaboration, and conclusion on the topic are often presented. The writing communicates clear information to the audience. |
6 | Higher-Advanced | A good variety of well-developed ideas is presented. A clear and organized writing structure is evident with effective coherence and cohesion. An effective introduction, elaboration, and conclusion on the topic are presented. The writing communicates very clear information to the audience. |
References
- ACTFL. 2012. ACTFL Proficiency Guidelines. Available online: http://www.actfl.org/sites/default/files/pdfs/public/ACTFLProficiencyGuidelines2012_FINAL.pdf (accessed on 2 January 2020).
- Bardovi-Harlig, Kathleen. 1990. Pragmatic word order in English composition. In Coherence in Writing: Research and Pedagogical Perspectives. Edited by Ulla Connor and Ann M. Johns. Alexandria: TESOL, pp. 43–65. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, Yu-Hua, and Paul Baker. 2016. Investigating criterial discourse features across second language development: Lexical bundles in rated learner essays, CEFR B1, B2 and C1. Applied Linguistics 37: 849–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chiang, Steve. 2003. The importance of cohesive conditions to perceptions of writing quality at the early stages of foreign language learning. System 31: 471–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Connor, Ulla. 1990. Linguistic/rhetorical measures for international persuasive student writing. Research in the Teaching of English 24: 67–87. [Google Scholar]
- Crossley, Scott A., and Danielle S. McNamara. 2012. Predicting second language writing proficiency: The roles of cohesion and linguistic sophistication. Journal of Research in Reading 35: 115–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crossley, Scott A., Rod Roscoe, and Danielle S. McNamara. 2011. Predicting human scores of essay quality using computational indices of linguistic and textual features. In Artificial Intelligence in Education: AIED 2011. Edited by Gautam Biswas, Susan Bull, Judy Kay and Antonija Mitrovic. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 438–40. [Google Scholar]
- Crossley, Scott A., Kristopher Kyle, and Danielle S. McNamara. 2016a. The development and use of cohesive devices in L2 writing and their relations to judgments of essay quality. Journal of Second Language Writing 32: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crossley, Scott A., Kristopher Kyle, and Danielle S. McNamara. 2016b. The tool for the automatic analysis of text cohesion (TAACO): Automatic assessment of local, global, and text cohesion. Behavior Research Methods 48: 1227–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Evola, Jill, Ellen Mamer, and Becky Lentz. 1980. Discrete point versus global scoring for cohesive devices. In Research in Language Testing. Edited by John W. Oller and Kyle Perkins. Rowley: Newbury House, pp. 177–81. [Google Scholar]
- Ferris, Dana R. 1994. Lexical and syntactic features of ESL writing by students at different levels of L2 proficiency. TESOL Quarterly 28: 414–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graesser, Arthur C., Danielle S. McNamara, Max M. Louwerse, and Zhiqiang Cai. 2004. Coh-Metrix: Analysis of text on cohesion and language. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 36: 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Guo, Liang, Scott A. Crossley, and Danielle S. McNamara. 2013. Predicting human judgments of essay quality in both integrated and independent second language writing samples: A comparison study. Assessing Writing 18: 218–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halliday, Michael Alexander Kirkwood, and Ruqaiya Hasan. 1976. Cohesion in English. London: Longman. [Google Scholar]
- Harman, Ruth. 2013. Literary intertextuality in genre-based pedagogies: Building lexical cohesion in fifth-grade L2 writing. Journal of Second Language Writing 22: 125–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hu, Guangwei, and Feng Cao. 2011. Hedging and boosting in abstracts of applied linguistics articles: A comparative study on English- and Chinese-medium journals. Journal of Pragmatics 43: 2795–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hyland, Ken. 2005. Metadiscourse: Exploring Interaction in Writing. London: Continuum. [Google Scholar]
- Jafarpur, Abdoljavad. 1991. Cohesiveness as a basis for evaluating compositions. System 19: 459–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kennedy, Chris, and Dilys Thorp. 2007. A corpus-based investigation of linguistic responses to an IELTS academic writing task. In IELTS Collected Papers: Research in Speaking and Writing Assessment. Edited by Lynda Taylor and Peter Falvey. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 316–77. [Google Scholar]
- Kintsch, Walter. 1998. Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kormos, Judit. 2011. Task complexity and linguistic and discourse features of narrative writing performance. Journal of Second Language Writing 20: 148–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kyle, Kristopher, and Scott Crossley. 2017. Tool for the automatic analysis of cohesion. Available online: https://www.linguisticanalysistools.org/uploads/1/3/9/3/13935189/_taaco_1.5_user_manual_7-16-17.pdf (accessed on 5 January 2020).
- Lee, Joseph J., and Lydia Deakin. 2016. Interactions in L1 and L2 undergraduate student writing: Interactional metadiscourse in successful and less-successful argumentative essays. Journal of Second Language Writing 33: 21–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Shouji. 2014. The gap in the use of lexical cohesive devices in writing between native Chinese speakers and second language users. Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association 49: 25–47. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Ting, and Sue Wharton. 2012. Metadiscourse repertoire of L1 Mandarin undergraduates writing in English: A cross-contextual, cross-disciplinary study. Journal of English for Academic Purposes 11: 345–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Meihua, and George Braine. 2005. Cohesive features in argumentative writing produced by Chinese undergraduates. System 33: 623–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Louwerse, Max. 2002. An analytic and cognitive parameterization of coherence relations. Cognitive Linguistics 12: 291–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mazgutova, Diana, and Judit Kormos. 2015. Syntactic and lexical development in an intensive English for academic purposes programme. Journal of Second Language Writing 29: 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McCutchen, Deborah. 1996. A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review 8: 299–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNamara, Danielle S., Eileen Kintsch, Nancy Butler Songer, and Walter Kintsch. 1996. Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction 14: 1–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNamara, Danielle S., Scott A. Crossley, and Philip M. McCarthy. 2010. Linguistic features of writing quality. Written Communication 27: 57–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McNamara, Danielle S., Scott A. Crossley, and Rod Roscoe. 2013. Natural language processing in an intelligent writing strategy tutoring system. Behavior Research Methods 45: 499–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Neuner, Jerome L. 1987. Cohesive ties and chains in good and poor freshman essays. Research in the Teaching of English 21: 92–105. [Google Scholar]
- Polio, Charlene G. 1997. Measures of linguistic accuracy in second language writing research. Language Learning 47: 101–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polio, Charlene, and Mark C. Shea. 2014. An investigation into current measures of linguistic accuracy in second language writing research. Journal of Second Language Writing 26: 10–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reid, Joy. 1992. A computer text analysis of four cohesion devices in English discourse by native and nonnative writers. Journal of Second Language Writing 1: 79–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanders, Ted J.M., Wilbert P.M. Spooren, and Leo G.M. Noordman. 1992. Toward a taxonomy of coherence relations. Discourse Processes 15: 1–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, Raoul N., and William J. Frawley. 1983. Conjunctive cohesion in four English genres. Text 3: 347–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Dijk, Teun Adrianus, and Walter Kintsch. 1983. Strategies of Discourse Comprehension. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Yang, Chunsheng. 2013. Textual conjunctives and topic-fronting devices in CFL learners’ written summaries. Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association 48: 71–89. [Google Scholar]
- Yang, Wenxing, and Ying Sun. 2012. The use of cohesive devices in argumentative writing by Chinese EFL learners at different proficiency levels. Linguistics and Education 23: 31–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, Cecilia Guanfang. 2013. Measuring authorial voice strength in L2 argumentative writing: The development and validation of an analytic rubric. Language Testing 30: 201–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zwaan, Rolf A., and Gabriel A. Radvansky. 1998. Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin 123: 162–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Low-Score | Middle-Score | High-Score | |
---|---|---|---|
Score range | 1.0–2.5 | 3.0–4.0 | 4.5–6.0 |
Number of essays | 19 | 20 | 23 |
Mean essay length (characters) | 152 | 230 | 298 |
Total number of characters | 2895 | 4603 | 6864 |
Indices | Analysis Methods |
---|---|
Interactive Metadiscourse Markers | |
Local cohesion between/within clauses/sentences | |
Transitional markers:
| Proportion of the number of transitional markers in each category against the total number of interactive metadiscourse markers (i.e., total number of transitional and frame markers) in an essay |
Percentage of prepositions: relations among clausal constituents | Proportion of the number of prepositions to the total number of words in an essay |
Global cohesion across idea units | |
Frame markers: signal discourse acts, sequences, and stages (e.g., first, finally) | Proportion of the number of frame markers to the total number of interactive metadiscourse markers in an essay |
Text cohesion | |
Givenness: proportion of given to new information | Third-person pronoun/noun ratio: number of third-person pronouns divided by the number of nouns in an essay Third-person pronoun density: proportion of the number of third-person pronouns to the total number of words in an essay |
Interactional Metadiscourse Markers | |
Self-mention: referencing to the author (e.g., I); Engagement: addressing and involving the reader (e.g., you) | Proportion of the number of interactional markers in each category to the total number of interactional metadiscourse markers (i.e., total number of self-mention and engagement markers) in an essay |
Linguistic Indices | |
Linguistic accuracy: ratio of correct clauses | Number of error-free clauses divided by the total number of clauses in an essay |
Lexical complexity: lexical diversity | Number of word types divided by the square root of the total number of word tokens in an essay |
Syntactic complexity: clause length | Total number of words divided by the total number of clauses in an essay |
Low (n = 19) M (SD) | Middle (n = 20) M (SD) | High (n = 23) M (SD) | |
---|---|---|---|
Interactive Metadiscourse Markers | |||
Local cohesion between/within clauses/sentences: | |||
Continuative/additive marker | 17.97% (0.2318) | 17.99% (0.2086) | 15.47% (0.1371) |
Comparison/contrast marker | 16.18% (0.2648) | 22.20% (0.2398) | 17.96% (0.1795) |
Causative marker | 21.67% (0.2383) | 20.42% (0.1849) | 28.76% (0.1988) |
Conditional/hypothetical marker | 1.50% (0.0451) | 5.90% (0.0872) | 7.64% (0.0951) |
Misuse marker | 26.36% (0.3262) | 9.53% (0.1213) | 8.23% (0.0868) |
Preposition | 3.29% (0.0209) | 3.20% (0.0160) | 3.27% (0.0115) |
Global cohesion across idea units: | |||
Frame marker | 5.80% (0.1076) | 23.95% (0.2217) | 21.93% (0.1473) |
Text cohesion: | |||
Third-person pronoun | 1.49% (0.0118) | 1.56% (0.0140) | 2.21% (0.0120) |
Third-person pronoun/noun ratio | 5.54% (0.0466) | 5.82% (0.0525) | 7.84% (0.0454) |
Interactional Metadiscourse Markers | |||
Self-mention marker | 83.49% (0.3194) | 77.00% (0.2181) | 76.26% (0.2367) |
Engagement marker | 5.98% (0.1260) | 23.00% (0.2181) | 19.40% (0.1737) |
Value | F | Df | Error df | p | Partial eta Squared | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pillai’s trace | 0.567 | 1.799 * | 22 | 100 | 0.027 | 0.284 |
Wilks’ Λ | 0.494 | 1.882 * | 22 | 98 | 0.019 | 0.297 |
Hotelling’s trace | 0.900 | 1.963 * | 22 | 96 | 0.013 | 0.310 |
Roy’s largest root | 0.730 | 3.316 * | 11 | 50 | 0.002 | 0.422 |
Source | Dependent Variable | Df | F | p | Partial eta Squared |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Interactive Metadiscourse Markers | |||||
Local cohesion between/withinclauses/sentences: | |||||
Groups | Continuative/additive marker | 2 | 0.122 | 0.886 | 0.004 |
Comparison/contrast marker | 2 | 0.364 | 0.696 | 0.012 | |
Causative marker | 2 | 1.021 | 0.367 | 0.033 | |
Conditional/hypothetical marker | 2 | 3.150 * | 0.050 | 0.096 | |
Misuse marker | 2 | 5.079 * | 0.009 | 0.147 | |
Preposition | 2 | 0.015 | 0.985 | 0.001 | |
Global cohesion across idea units: | |||||
Frame marker | 2 | 7.079 * | 0.002 | 0.194 | |
Text cohesion: | |||||
Third-person pronoun | 2 | 2.151 | 0.125 | 0.068 | |
Third-person pronoun/noun ratio | 2 | 1.468 | 0.239 | 0.047 | |
Interactional Metadiscourse Markers | |||||
Self-mention marker | 2 | 0.468 | 0.628 | 0.016 | |
Engagement marker | 2 | 4.995 * | 0.010 | 0.145 | |
Error | Continuative/additive marker | 59 | |||
Comparison/contrast marker | 59 | ||||
Causative marker | 59 | ||||
Conditional/hypothetical marker | 59 | ||||
Misuse marker | 59 | ||||
Preposition | 59 | ||||
Frame marker | 59 | ||||
Third-person pronoun | 59 | ||||
Third-person pronoun/noun ratio | 59 | ||||
Self-mention marker | 59 | ||||
Engagement marker | 59 |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Continuative/additive marker | — | ||||||||
2. Comparison/contrast marker | −0.402 * | — | |||||||
3. Conditional/hypothetical marker | −0.094 | −0.038 | — | ||||||
4. Frame marker | −0.034 | −0.143 | −0.005 | — | |||||
5. Misuse marker | −0.093 | −0.207 | −0.189 | −0.336 * | — | ||||
6. Preposition | −0.285 * | 0.151 | −0.067 | −0.032 | 0.211 | — | |||
7. Third-person pron. | −0.039 | −0.186 | 0.028 | 0.272 * | 0.046 | 0.054 | — | ||
8. Third-person pron./ noun ratio | 0.012 | −0.184 | 0.129 | 0.257 * | 0.071 | 0.050 | 0.962 ** | — | |
9. Self-mention marker | 0.091 | 0.100 | −0.318 * | 0.021 | 0.008 | 0.077 | 0.123 | 0.118 | — |
10. Engagement marker | −0.062 | −0.112 | 0.598 ** | 0.165 | −0.092 | 0.001 | 0.055 | 0.087 | −0.566 ** |
Ratio of Correct Clauses | Lexical Diversity | Clause Length | |
---|---|---|---|
Continuative/additive marker | 0.026 | 0.018 | −0.045 |
Comparison/contrast marker | 0.098 | 0.084 | −0.047 |
Causative marker | 0.244 | 0.162 | 0.219 |
Conditional/hypothetical marker | 0.007 | 0.263 * | 0.194 |
Misuse marker | −0.269 * | −0.296 * | −0.188 |
Preposition | −0.088 | 0.009 | 0.204 |
Frame marker | 0.150 | 0.182 | 0.266 * |
Third-person pronoun | 0.052 | 0.171 | 0.214 |
Third-person pronoun/noun ratio | 0.025 | 0.116 | 0.143 |
Self-mention marker | 0.201 | −0.157 | −0.053 |
Engagement marker | 0.016 | 0.296 * | 0.337 * |
© 2020 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Liao, J. Metadiscourse, Cohesion, and Engagement in L2 Written Discourse. Languages 2020, 5, 25. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020025
Liao J. Metadiscourse, Cohesion, and Engagement in L2 Written Discourse. Languages. 2020; 5(2):25. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020025
Chicago/Turabian StyleLiao, Jianling. 2020. "Metadiscourse, Cohesion, and Engagement in L2 Written Discourse" Languages 5, no. 2: 25. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020025
APA StyleLiao, J. (2020). Metadiscourse, Cohesion, and Engagement in L2 Written Discourse. Languages, 5(2), 25. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020025