Comparing Different Methods That Measure Bilingual Children’s Language Environment: A Closer Look at Audio Recordings and Questionnaires
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Parental Questionnaires
1.2. Audio Recordings
1.3. Comparing the Methods
1.4. The Present Study
- Which language input measure correlates best with the vocabulary scores of bilingual children: parental questionnaires, day-long audio recordings, or a combination of both?
- Is there a difference in measuring the quantitative language input in the minority language compared to the majority language?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Vocabulary
2.2.2. Language Environment Measures
Parental Questionnaires: Quantifying Bilingual Experience (Q-BEx)
Day-Long Audio Recording: Language Environment Analysis (LENA)
Combined Method: LENA and Q-BEx
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Results
3.2. Individual Methods
3.3. Comparing the Methods
3.4. Comparing Majority and Minority Languages
3.5. Exploratory Analysis
4. Discussion
4.1. Comparisons between Language Input Quantity Measures
4.2. Minority Language versus Majority Language Vocabulary
4.3. Expressive versus Receptive Vocabulary
4.4. Limitations and Further Research
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
References
- Anderson, Nina. J., Susan A. Graham, Heather Prime, Jennifer M. Jenkins, and Sheri Madigan. 2021. Linking quality and quantity of parental linguistic input to child language skills: A meta-analysis. Child Development 92: 484–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bail, Amelie, Giovanna Morini, and Rochelle S. Newman. 2015. Look at the gato! Code-switching in speech to toddlers. Journal of Child Language 42: 1073–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bang, Janet Y., George Kachergis, Adriana Weisleder, and Virginia A. Marchman. 2022. An Automated Classifier for Child-Directed Speech from LENA Recordings. Somerville: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergelson, Elika, Marisa Casillas, Melanie Soderstrom, Amanda Seidl, Anne S. Warlaumont, and Andrei Amatuni. 2019. What do North American babies hear? A large-scale cross-corpus analysis. Developmental Science 22: e12724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bergelson, Elika, Melanie Soderstrom, Iris-Corinna Schwarz, Caroline F. Rowland, Nairán Ramírez-Esparza, Lisa R. Hamrick, Ellen Marklund, Marina Kalashnikova, Ava Guez, Marisa Casillas, and et al. 2023. Everyday language input and production in 1001 children from six continents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 120: 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bijeljac-Babic, Ranka, Josette Serres, Barbara Höhle, and Thierry Nazzi. 2012. Effect of bilingualism on lexical stress pattern discrimination in French-learning infants. PLoS ONE 7: e30843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bruyneel, Eva, Ellen Demurie, Sofie Boterberg, Petra Warreyn, and Herbert Roeyers. 2021. Validation of the Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) system for Dutch. Journal of Child Language 48: 765–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Byers-Heinlein, Krista. 2013. Parental language mixing: Its measurement and the relation of mixed input to young bilingual children’s vocabulary size. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 32–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Byers-Heinlein, Krista, Esther Schott, Ana Maria Gonzalez-Barrero, Melanie Brouillard, Daphnée Dubé, Amel Jardak, Alexandra Laoun-Rubenstein, Meghan Mastroberardino, Elizabeth Morin-Lessard, Maria P. Tamayo, and et al. 2020. MAPLE: A multilingual approach to parent language estimates. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23: 951–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carbajal, Maria Julia, and Sharon Peperkamp. 2020. Dual language input and the impact of language separation on early lexical development. Infancy 25: 22–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carroll, Susanne E. 2017. Exposure and input in bilingual development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casillas, Marisa, Penelope Brown, and Stephen C. Levinson. 2020. Early language experience in a Tseltal Mayan village. Child Development 91: 1819–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattani, Allegra, Kirsten Abbot-Smith, Rafalla Farag, Andrea Krott, Frédérique Arreckx, Ian Dennis, and Caroline Floccia. 2014. How much exposure to English is necessary for a bilingual toddler to perform like a monolingual peer in language tests? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 49: 649–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Chuansheng, Shin-Ying Lee, and Harold W. Stevenson. 1995. Response style and cross-cultural comparisons of rating scales among East Asian and North American students. Psychological Science 6: 170–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cristia, Alejandrina, Federica Bulgarelli, and Elika Bergelson. 2020. Accuracy of the language environment analysis system segmentation and metrics: A systematic review. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 63: 1093–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cychosz, Margaret, Anele Villanueva, and Adriana Weisleder. 2021. Efficient estimation of children’s language exposure in two bilingual communities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 64: 3843–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Cat, Cécile, Draško Kašćelan, Philippe Prévost, Ludovica Serratrice, Laurie Tuller, and Sharon Unsworth. 2022. Quantifying Bilingual EXperience (Q-BEx): Questionnaire Manual and Documentation. Charlottesville: Open Science Framework. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Houwer, Annick. 2009. Bilingual First Language Acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, vol. 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Houwer, Annick. 2011. Language input environments and language development in bilingual acquisition. Applied Linguistics Review 2: 221–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diedenhofen, Birk, and Jochen Musch. 2015. cocor: A comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. PLoS ONE 10: e0121945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dijkstra, Jelske, Folkert Kuiken, René J. Jorna, and Edwin L. Klinkenberg. 2016. The role of majority and minority language input in the early development of a bilingual vocabulary. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19: 191–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doğruöz, A. Seza, and Ad Backus. 2010. Turkish in the Netherlands: Development of a new variety? In Language Contact: New Perspectives. Edited by Muriel Norde, Bob de Jonge and Cornelius Hasselblatt. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing, vol. 28, pp. 87–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duursma, Elisabeth, Silvia Romero-Contreras, Anna Szuber, Patrick Proctor, Catherine Snow, Diane August, and Margarita Calderón. 2007. The role of home literacy and language environment on bilinguals’ English and Spanish vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 171–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller, and Enlli Môn Thomas. 2009. Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12: 213–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gershkoff-Stowe, Lisa, and Erin R. Hahn. 2013. Word comprehension and production asymmetries in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 114: 489–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gibson, Todd A., Elizabeth D. Peña, and Lisa M. Bedore. 2014. The relation between language experience and receptive-expressive semantic gaps in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 17: 90–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gollan, Tamar H., Rosa I. Montoya, Cynthia Cera, and Tiffany C. Sandoval. 2008. More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language 58: 787–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gollan, Tamar H., Timothy J. Slattery, Diane Goldenberg, Eva Van Assche, Wouter Duyck, and Keith Rayner. 2011. Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: The frequency-lag hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 140: 186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greenwood, Charles R., Kathy Thiemann-Bourque, Dale Walker, Jay Buzhardt, and Jill Gilkerson. 2011. Assessing children’s home language environments using automatic speech recognition technology. Communication Disorders Quarterly 32: 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haman, Ewa, Magdalena Łuniewska, and Barbara Pomiechowska. 2015. Designing cross-linguistic lexical tasks (CLTs) for bilingual preschool children. In Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, pp. 196–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haman, Ewa, Magdalena Łuniewska, Pernille Hansen, Hanne Gram Simonsen, Shula Chiat, Jovana Bjekić, Agnė Blažienė, Katarzyna Chyl, Ineta Dabašinskienė, Sharon Armon-Lotem, and et al. 2017. Noun and verb knowledge in monolingual preschool children across 17 languages: Data from Cross-linguistic Lexical Tasks (LITMUS-CLT). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 31: 818–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hammer, Carol Scheffner, Megan Dunn Davison, Frank R. Lawrence, and Adele W. Miccio. 2009. The effect of maternal language on bilingual children’s vocabulary and emergent literacy development during Head Start and kindergarten. Scientific Studies of Reading 13: 99–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hart, Betty, and Todd R. Risley. 1995. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Towson: Paul H Brookes Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Heine, Steven J., Darrin R. Lehman, Kaiping Peng, and Joe Greenholtz. 2002. What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales?: The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82: 903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heine, Steven J., Toshitake Takata, and Darrin R. Lehman. 2000. Beyond self-presentation: Evidence for self-criticism among Japanese. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26: 71–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika. 2003. The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development 74: 1368–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoff, Erika. 2006. How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26: 55–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika, and Cynthia Core. 2013. Input and language development in bilingually developing children. In Seminars in Speech and Language. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers, vol. 34, No. 04. pp. 215–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika, Cynthia Core, Sylvia Place, Rosario Rumiche, Melissa Señor, and Marisol Parra. 2012. Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language 39: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huttenlocher, J., W. Haight, A. Bryk, M. Seltzer, and T. Lyons. 1991. Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27: 236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kašćelan, Draško, Philippe Prévost, Ludovica Serratrice, Laurie Tuller, Sharon Unsworth, and Cécile De Cat. 2022. A review of questionnaires quantifying bilingual experience in children: Do they document the same constructs? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 25: 29–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lalwani, Ashok K., Sharon Shavitt, and Timothy Johnson. 2006. What is the relation between cultural orientation and socially desirable responding? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90: 165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levin-Asher, Bonnie, Osnat Segal, and Liat Kishon-Rabin. 2023. The validity of LENA technology for assessing the linguistic environment and interactions of infants learning Hebrew and Arabic. Behavior Research Methods 55: 1480–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lieven, Elena, Vibeke Grover, Paola Uccelli, and Meredith L. Rowe. 2019. Input, interaction and learning in early language development. In Learning through Language: Towards an Educationally Informed Theory of Language Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marasli, Zeynep, and Jessica L. Montag. 2023. Optimizing random sampling of daylong audio. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Sydney, Australia, July 26–29; 45. No. 45. [Google Scholar]
- Marchman, Virginia A., Lucía Z. Martínez, Nereyda Hurtado, Theres Grüter, and Anne Fernald. 2017. Caregiver talk to young Spanish-English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent-report measures of dual-language exposure. Developmental Science 20: e12425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Orena, Adriel John, Krista Byers-Heinlein, and Linda Polka. 2019. Reliability of the language environment analysis recording system in analyzing French–English bilingual speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 62: 2491–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Orena, Adriel John, Krista Byers-Heinlein, and Linda Polka. 2020. What do bilingual infants actually hear? Evaluating measures of language input to bilingual-learning 10-month-olds. Developmental Science 23: e12901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pearson, Barbara Z., Sylvia C. Fernandez, Vanessa Lewedeg, and D. Kimbrough Oller. 1997. The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics 18: 41–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearson, Karl, and Louis Napoleon George Filon. 1898. VII. Mathematical contributions to the theory of evolution.—IV. On the probable errors of frequency constants and on the influence of random selection on variation and correlation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A, Containing Papers of a Mathematical or Physical Character 191: 229–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Place, Sylvia, and Erika Hoff. 2011. Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development 82: 1834–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Place, Sylvia, and Erika Hoff. 2016. Effects and noneffects of input in bilingual environments on dual language skills in 2 ½-year-olds. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19: 1023–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Potter, Christine E., Eva Fourakis, Elizabeth Morin-Lessard, Krista Byers-Heinlein, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2019. Bilingual toddlers’ comprehension of mixed sentences is asymmetrical across their two languages. Developmental Science 22: e12794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- R Core Team. 2020. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.r-project.org/ (accessed on 1 August 2023).
- Ramírez-Esparza, Nairán, Adrián García-Sierra, and Patricia K. Kuhl. 2014. Look who’s talking: Speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. Developmental Science 17: 880–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramírez-Esparza, Nairán, Adrián García-Sierra, and Patricia K. Kuhl. 2017. The impact of early social interactions on later language development in Spanish–English bilingual infants. Child Development 88: 1216–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramírez-Esparza, Nairán, Samuel D. Gosling, and James W. Pennebaker. 2008. Paradox lost: Unraveling the puzzle of Simpatía. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 39: 703–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richards, Jeffrey A., Jill Gilkerson, Dongxin Xu, and Keith Topping. 2017. How much do parents think they talk to their child? Journal of Early Intervention 39: 163–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, Meredith L. 2012. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development 83: 1762–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siow, Serene, Nicola A. Gillen, Irina Lepădatu, and Kim Plunkett. 2023. Double it up: Vocabulary size comparisons between UK bilingual and monolingual toddlers. Infancy 28: 1030–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, Sharon. 2016. Quantity and Quality of Language Input in Bilingual Language Development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, Sharon, Vicky Chondrogianni, and Barbora Skarabela. 2018. Experiential measures can be used as a proxy for language dominance in bilingual language acquisition research. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Wonderen, Elise, and Sharon Unsworth. 2021. Testing the validity of the Cross-Linguistic Lexical Task as a measure of language proficiency in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language 48: 1101–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weisleder, Adriana, and Anne Fernald. 2013. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science 24: 2143–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Williams, Evan J. 1959. The comparison of regression variables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological) 21: 396–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
n | M | SD | Range | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Minority language | ||||
Polish | 21 | |||
Turkish | 31 | |||
Age (months) | 52 | 55 | 10 | 36–72 |
Gender | ||||
Female | 27 | |||
Male | 25 | |||
Language Dominance | ||||
>60% Dutch | 24 | |||
40–60% Dutch | 11 | |||
<40% Dutch | 17 | |||
Parents’ educational level | ||||
University degree | 44 | |||
Post-secondary school | 7 | |||
Secondary school | 1 |
Subject | Segment | Speaker | Language | Dominance | CDS | Activity |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
00001 | 1 | SIB, FAT | Dutch | Dutch | ODS | Mealtime |
00001 | 2 | CHI, FAT | Dutch, Polish | Polish | CDS-ADULT | Mealtime |
00001 | 3 | CHI, FAT | Dutch | Dutch | CDS-ADULT | Mealtime |
00001 | 4 | CHI, FAT, MOT | Dutch, Polish | Both | CDS-ADULT | Playtime |
Q-BEx | LENA | Combined Method |
Step 1 (Q-BEx) | Step 1 (LENA) | Step 1 (Q-BEx Data) |
Determine who spends time with the child during a regular week and holiday week. | Code the sampled recording (270 30-s segments) for speaker, language, and child-directed speech. | Determine who spends time with the child during a regular week. |
Step 2 (Q-BEx) | Step 2 (LENA) | Step 2 (LENA data) |
Parents estimate the proportion of languages used by each interlocutor with the child. | Remove segments that do not contain child-directed speech. | Code the sampled recording (270 30-s segments) for speaker, language, and child-directed speech, and calculate the proportion of observed language use in each unique speaker context. |
Step 3 (Q-BEx) | Step 3 (LENA) | Step 3 (LENA data) |
Multiply the time spent by each interlocutor with their estimated language use. This is achieved automatically by the Q-BEx interface. | Divide the number of segments in one language by the total number of segments. | Calculate the mean adult word count (AWC) per waking hour to quantify the amount of speech the child receives during the recorded day. |
Step 4 (Q-BEx and LENA) | ||
Multiply the time spent in each context with the quantity of speech (mean AWC) and the observed language use. |
Variable | Majority Language (Dutch) | Minority Language (Polish/Turkish) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | min | max | M | SD | min | max | |
Language input | ||||||||
Q-BEx | 0.52 | 0.21 | 0.14 | 0.96 | 0.47 | 0.21 | 0.04 | 0.86 |
LENA | 0.36 | 0.34 | 0.00 | 0.97 | 0.63 | 0.34 | 0.03 | 1.00 |
Combined method | 54,202 | 34,916 | 7377 | 145,115 | 62,651 | 43,786 | 1321 | 182,797 |
Combined proportion | 0.48 | 0.28 | 0.09 | 0.98 | 0.52 | 0.28 | 0.02 | 0.91 |
Vocabulary scores | ||||||||
Expressive | 22.6 | 11.2 | 1 | 44 | 25.8 | 17.0 | 0 | 55 |
Receptive | 50.4 | 10.2 | 16 | 62 | 50.1 | 11.0 | 16 | 64 |
Method | Majority Language (Dutch) | Minority Language (Polish/Turkish) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Expressive | Receptive | Expressive | Receptive | |||||
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
Q-BEx | 0.66 | <0.001 | 0.35 | 0.01 | 0.74 | <0.001 | 0.56 | <0.001 |
LENA | 0.62 | <0.001 | 0.43 | 0.001 | 0.62 | <0.001 | 0.44 | 0.001 |
Combined method | 0.49 | <0.001 | 0.28 | 0.04 | 0.59 | <0.001 | 0.48 | <0.001 |
Hypotheses | Majority Language (Dutch) | Minority Language (Polish/Turkish) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Expressive | Receptive | Expressive | Receptive | |
Combined > Q-BEx | 0.96 | 0.71 | 0.96 | 0.79 |
Combined > LENA | 0.95 | 0.94 | 0.63 | 0.35 |
LENA ≠ Q-BEx | 0.60 | 0.37 | 0.08 | 0.16 |
Method | Expressive Vocabulary | Receptive Vocabulary | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
z | p | z | p | |
Q-BEx | 0.92 | 0.18 | 1.41 | 0.08 |
LENA | −0.04 | 0.52 | 0.05 | 0.48 |
Combined | 0.78 | 0.21 | 1.26 | 0.10 |
Method | Majority Language (Dutch) | Minority Language (Polish/Turkish) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
t | p | Cohen’s d | t | p | Cohen’s d | |
Q-BEx | 3.31 | <0.001 | 0.33 | 2.50 | 0.008 | 0.26 |
LENA | 1.94 | 0.03 | 0.19 | 2.21 | 0.02 | 0.22 |
Combined | 1.91 | 0.03 | 0.19 | 1.23 | 0.11 | 0.12 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Verhoeven, E.; van Witteloostuijn, M.; Oudgenoeg-Paz, O.; Blom, E. Comparing Different Methods That Measure Bilingual Children’s Language Environment: A Closer Look at Audio Recordings and Questionnaires. Languages 2024, 9, 231. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9070231
Verhoeven E, van Witteloostuijn M, Oudgenoeg-Paz O, Blom E. Comparing Different Methods That Measure Bilingual Children’s Language Environment: A Closer Look at Audio Recordings and Questionnaires. Languages. 2024; 9(7):231. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9070231
Chicago/Turabian StyleVerhoeven, Emma, Merel van Witteloostuijn, Ora Oudgenoeg-Paz, and Elma Blom. 2024. "Comparing Different Methods That Measure Bilingual Children’s Language Environment: A Closer Look at Audio Recordings and Questionnaires" Languages 9, no. 7: 231. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9070231
APA StyleVerhoeven, E., van Witteloostuijn, M., Oudgenoeg-Paz, O., & Blom, E. (2024). Comparing Different Methods That Measure Bilingual Children’s Language Environment: A Closer Look at Audio Recordings and Questionnaires. Languages, 9(7), 231. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9070231