The Setting of the Null Subject Parameters across (Non-)Null-Subject Languages
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Null Subject Parameter(s)
2.1. Empirical Problems
(Gilligan 1989, p. 147) |
(Jaeggli and Safir 1989, pp. 29–30) |
2.2. Conceptual Problems
2.3. Reshaping the Classical View
(Roberts 2019, pp. 237, 241) |
(Gilligan 1989, 80) |
3. Null Subject Parameter Setting under the BCC-Account
(Biberauer and Roberts 2016, p. 11) |
- The child has a template in which uninterpretable features are matched to interpretable features. This will be important, for example, for the child to match null subjects to T’s uninterpretable features;
- FFs are not pre-given in the grammar: the child postulates them according to the input;
- There are mechanisms used to parse the input and optimize the learning task. Those are: Feature Economy (FE) and Input Generalization (IG);6
- This account assumes that children can infer morphological paradigms early, incorporating phi-features into their grammar.
4. Missing Subjects in Children Acquiring NNSLs
4.1. The Diary Drop Hypothesis
(Nariyama 2004, pp. 246–47) |
Guasti (2016) |
4.2. Root Infinitives
(25) | Optional Infinitive Stage |
a. Finite sentences are sometimes used; | |
b. Nonfinite sentences are sometimes used; | |
c. Finite and nonfinite verbs show up in the appropriate positions; | |
d. Case and finiteness properties follow from the ATOM; 14 | |
e. NS/OI holds, that is, ATOM does not hold for null-subject languages (children acquiring NSLs do not go through an OI stage). |
(26) | Models explaining the Optional Infinitive Stage |
a. Underspecification of TNS Wexler (1992); | |
b. Omission of functional layers Rizzi (1994); | |
c. Underspecification of AGR/TNS Schütze andWexler (1996); | |
d. Underspecification of number Hoekstraet al. (1996). |
Berber | |
Ouhalla (1993) |
5. The Acquisition of Radical Null Subject Languages
6. The Acquisition of Partial and Consistent Null-Subject Languages
6.1. Definite Null Subjects in PNSLs
6.2. Definite Null Subjects in CNSLs
6.3. Generic Null Subjects in PNSLs
6.3.1. Spontaneous Production
(Simões 1999, p. 126) |
(Magalhães 2006, p. 137) |
(Torn-Leesik and Vija 2012, p. 252) |
(Torn-Leesik and Vija 2012, p. 259) |
6.3.2. Comprehension
As you already noticed, this school is very weird. One of the rules is that just one of the students, Joaquim, has to eat dessert during lunch time instead of regular food.
Look at Bruno! He brought a salad to eat for lunch. Look, he is eating the salad now!
Mariana brought cheese balls to eat for lunch. Look she is eating cheese balls! Look at Lucas! He is eating pasta!
At first Joaquim put a sandwich in his lunchbox, but then he remembered the rule that he had to bring dessert for lunch. So he brought a brigadeiro instead. Look, he is eating a brigadeiro.21
Now Elmo is going to tell us a part of the story. Let’s see whether he paid attention or not.
Elmo:
7. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Glossing abbreviations: 3 = third person, acc = accusative, comp = complementizer, f = feminine, imprs = impersonal, inf = infinitive, nom = nominative, pl = plural, ptcp = participle, ptv = partitive, sg = singular. | ||
2 | Here, “generic null subject” refers specifically to 3SG environments. As noted by an anonymous reviewer, CNSLs may well allow generic null subjects with a 2SG or 3PL verb, and/or with impersonal or mediopassive constructions. For present purposes, however, the key point is that CNSLs generally disallow 3SG generic null subjects (which will be analyzed as resulting from D-to-T incorporation). | ||
3 | The typology here described is not exhaustive. There are microvariations not covered by Table 1. Roberts (2019) gives as example the Northern Italo-Romance dialects, which do not quite behave as CNSLs, having a more restricted pattern of definite null subjects (which he attributes to their clitic system). | ||
4 | As noted by a reviewer, indirect evidence is not the only way children could figure out whether their language allows complementizer-trace effects violations or not: they could start with the assumption that their language does not have complementizer-trace violations and then wait for possible counterevidence in the input. However, indirect learning might be the most viable mechanism in which children acquire complementizer-trace proprieties. As shown by Chacón et al. (2015), instances of complementizer-trace effects violations are rare in the input in Spanish and Italian, contrasting with instances of postverbal subjects (a property which could lead the learner to infer that their language allows complementizer-trace violations), which are much more common in the input. If frequency is a relevant factor in language acquisition, the learner is more likely to use indirect evidence in this case. | ||
5 | An anonymous reviewer pointed out that Afrikaans and English should be a problem for the idea that NNSLs have D in D0, as these languages do not mark gender and number in determiners. However, what seems crucial to Roberts (2019) is that all NNSLs, including English and Afrikaans, have definite and indefinite determiners, which makes all them to have D in D0. | ||
6 | An anonymous reviewer was concerned with how the child would be able to parse a sentence despite having the ‘wrong’ parameter value. The concern alludes to Valian (1990) and the canonical Null Subject Parameter model. In the canonical model, the child is born with an English-like grammar or with an Italian-like grammar. Based on the Subset Principle, he/she should have an English-like grammar. The problem, according to Valian (1990), is that a child learning Italian, if born with an English-like grammar, will be unable to parse strings without subjects, because his/her parse is fed by an English-like grammar which is blind to null subjects. I believe this problem does not arise in the model I assume here, because the child is not born with a particular grammar. Children will be looking for ways to fullfill the [iF]/[uF] template, for example, by matching verbal agreement with null subjects when they are found in the input, or by not doing it when they are not found. | ||
7 | |||
8 | |||
9 | That is not entirely true. Finnish is not a NNSL, but a PNSL, and yet it has an overt expletive: sitä a partitive form of the pronoun se, classified as a there-type, pure expletive Holmberg and Nikanne (2002). | ||
10 | Under the BCC-view, ‘setting the null subject parameter’ is best translated as ‘learning the right feature values’. However, for simplicity, I will use the shorthand phrase ‘setting’ (or ‘missetting’) the null subject parameter. | ||
11 | |||
12 | However, examples of RIs occurring in wh-questions have been restricted to English. | ||
13 | Observe that for English, the percentage of missing subjects with finite verbs is higher than with nonfinite ones, which is markedly different from other languages. The English-speaking children represented in the graph are Eve and Adam. Eve produces very few missing subjects, both in finite and nonfinite clauses. Adam produces more null subjects in finite clauses than in nonfinite ones, but the difference is just 9%. For English, only third-person singular verbs were counted because other verbal forms lack inflection, and so do not inform whether the child produced a RI or not. This might have had an effect on the overall count, since what maybe was an RIs in second-, first-person or third-person plural were left aside. For more discussion, see Hoekstra and Hyams (1998). | ||
14 | ATOM stands for “AGR/TNS omission model”. Schütze and Wexler (1996) assume that AGR and/or TNS may be deleted by the child. If AGR is deleted, subjects get default case. In English, the default case is Acc, but in some languages, like German/Dutch, the default case is Nom. | ||
15 | Even though the children were very young, they were able to pass the control items, hence the performance was not affected by their cognitive stage. | ||
16 | An anonymous reviewer worries that the statement that children acquiring RNSLs know their target grammar is too strong, due to the fact that the evidence comes from production, and production is sensitive to a number of factor, such as discourse context. I agree that a comprehension study would yield more compelling data, however the fact that children acquiring RNSLs produce a higher rate of null subjects than children acquiring English already suggests that the grammar of the two groups differs. | ||
17 | Some languages classified as PNSLs license null subjects in contexts quite different from the ones described here. For example, in Shipibo third-person pronouns are optionally null, whereas second and first-person subjects are obligatory overt Camacho (2013). We can question whether PNSL is an adequate classification for these languages, or if the variation could be regarded as a microparameter (in the terms of Roberts (2019)). | ||
18 | An anonymous reviewer raised an important point: Figure 3 aggregates children’s production of null subjects across several months. In their words, maybe the aggregation disguises a parameter missetting. If we looked at narrow time frames, we might find evidence of missetting at certain points in time. I think the reviewer is correct in their observation. All we can say is that for a certain age group, there was no evidence of parameter missetting. | ||
19 | According to Simões, two other factors contributed to the number of null subjects found in André’s speech: what the author calls “ritualized repetitions” and André’s frequent use of the statement “I don’t know” (não sei) which is usually uttered with a null subject in BP even in the speech of adults. Adults, differently than children, are unlikely to constantly respond to a question with “I don’t know”. | ||
20 | Generic null subjects are also attested in RNSLs, such as Chinese and Thai, since RNLs lack -features. If we consider only this fact, without taking into account the knowledge BP-speaking children have about agreement, it is not possible to say, by this experiment, whether BP-speaking children know they are acquiring a PNSL: they could have a grammar of an RNSL. As illustrated in the sentence below, the subject in the impersonal sentence below in Mandarin only has the generic interpretation:
However, BP-speaking children exhibit stable knowledge of agreement at least by the age of 3;00 Magalhães (2006). Agreement is not present in RNSLs, but it is in PNSLs. The known facts about the acquisition of agreement in BP in conjunction with children’s interpretation of null subjects can reveal whether BP-speaking children are aware they are acquiring a PNSL. | ||
21 | Brigadeiro is a popular Brazilian dessert. | ||
22 | For the statistics and graph creation, the R R Core Team (2021) package gstatsplot Patil (2021) was used. In the x-axis, n = 60 refers to the number of sentences judged by each age group. Each age group was composed by 10 children. G4 = 4-year-olds, G5 = 5-year-olds, G6 = 6-year-olds, G7 = 7-year-olds. | ||
23 | A reviewer mentioned that hypothesis (i) directly conflicts with the proposal that children are grammatically conservative Snyder (2002, 2007, 2011). In principle, it would be possible to assume that children are conservative when acquiring syntactic aspects of their language, while assuming that the same does not hold in acquiring pragmatics. However, this assumption needs to be further investigated. |
References
- Austin, Jennifer, María Blume, David Parkinson, Zelmira Núñez del Prado, and Barbara Lust. 1997. The status of pro-drop in the initial state: Results from new analyses of Spanish. Contemporary Perspectives on the Acquisition of Spanish 1: 37–54. [Google Scholar]
- Baker, Mark. 2008. The Macroparameter in a Microparametric World. Amsterdam: Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Bertolino, Karina. 2024. The acquisition of generic null subjects under the Borer-Chomsky Conjecture. In Language Acquisition in Romance Languages. Amsterdam: Benjamins, vol. 1, pp. 58–85. [Google Scholar]
- Bertolino, Karina. 2016. Modals and Impersonal Null Subjects in Brazilian Portuguese. Master’s thesis, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Bertolino, Karina. 2020. An Experimental Study on the Acquisition of Impersonals in Brazilian Portuguese. Ph.D. thesis, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Berwick, Robert. 1985. The Acquisition of Syntactic Knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, vol. 16. [Google Scholar]
- Berwick, Robert, and Noam Chomsky. 2011. The Biolinguistic Program: The current state of its development. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 19–41. [Google Scholar]
- Biberauer, Theresa. 2010. Semi null-subject languages, expletives and expletive pro reconsidered. In Parametric Variation: Null Subjects in Minimalist Theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 153–99. [Google Scholar]
- Biberauer, Theresa. 2018. Pro-drop and Emergent Parameter. In Null Subjects in Generative Grammar: A Synchronic and Diachronic Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 94. [Google Scholar]
- Biberauer, Theresa. 2019. Factors 2 and 3: Towards a principled approach. Catalan Journal of Linguistics Special Issue, 45–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biberauer, Theresa, and Ian Roberts. 2016. Parameter typology from a diachronic perspective. Theoretical Approaches to Linguistic Variation 234: 259–91. [Google Scholar]
- Bloom, Lois, Patsy Lightbown, Lois Hood, Melissa Bowerman, Michael Maratsos, and Michael P Maratsos. 1975. Structure and variation in child language. In Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. New York: Wiley, pp. 1–97. [Google Scholar]
- Bloom, Paul. 1990. Subjectless sentences in child language. Linguistic Inquiry 21: 491–504. [Google Scholar]
- Boster, Carole Tenny. 1997. Processing and Parameter Setting in Language Acquisition: A Computational Approach. Ph.D. thesis, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Brandtler, Johan. 2004. Subject omission and discourse anchorage in early Swedish child language. In Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax. London: Scandinavian Syntax. [Google Scholar]
- Bromberg, Sara, and Kenneth Wexler. 1995. Null subjects in child wh-questions. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 26: 221–47. [Google Scholar]
- Camacho, José. 2013. Null Subjects. Cambridge Studies in Linguistics. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cardinaletti, Anna, and Michal Starke. 1999. The typology of structural deficiency. In Clitics in the Languages of Europe. Vienna: DE Gruyter, pp. 145–233. [Google Scholar]
- Chacón, Dustin Alfonso, Michael Fetters, Margaret Kandel, Eric Pelzl, and Colin Phillips. 2015. Indirect Learning and Language Variation: Reassessing the That-Trace Effect. Master’s thesis, College Park & Yale University, University of Maryland, Yale, CT, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1980. On Cognitive Structures and Their Development: A Reply to Piaget. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use. Westport: Greenwood Publishing Group. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1998. Minimalist Inquiries: The Framework. In MIT Occasional Papers in Linguistics. Cambridge, MA: MIT, Department of Linguistics, vol. 15. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2005. Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2013. Problems of projection. Lingua 130: 33–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2015. Problems of projection: Extensions. Structures, Strategies and Beyond: Studies in Honour of Adriana Belletti 223: 1–16. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam, and Michael Kenstowicz. 1999. Derivation by Phase. In An Annotated Syntax Reader. p. 482. Available online: https://as.nyu.edu/content/dam/nyu-as/linguistics/documents/Kayne%20Leu%20Zanuttini%200520%20-2013-proofs%20paper.pdf#page=494 (accessed on 12 January 2024).
- Cipriani, Paola, Pietro Pfanner, Anna Chilosi, Lorena Cittadoni, Alessandro Ciuti, Anna Maccari, Natalia Pantanon, L. Pfanner, P. Poli, S. Sarno, and et al. 1989. Protocolli Diagnostici e Terapeutici Nello Sviluppo e Nella Patologia del Linguaggio. Pisa: Italian Ministry of Health, Stella Maris Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- Clahsen, Harald. 1989. Creole Genesis, the Lexical Learning Hypothesis and the Problem of Development in Language Acquisition. In Wheels within Wheels: Papers of the Duisburg Symposium on Pidgin and Creole Languages. Frankfurt: Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
- Clahsen, Harald, Martina Penke, and Teresa Parodi. 1993. Functional categories in early child german. Language Acquisition 3: 395–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, Herbert, and Eve Clark. 1979. Psychology and language: An introduction to psycholinguistics. Language 55: 436. [Google Scholar]
- Crain, Stephen, and Cecile McKee. 1985. Acquisition of structural restrictions on anaphora. North East Linguistics Society 16: 8. [Google Scholar]
- Demuth, Katherine. 1994. Minimal words in early acquisition. Paper presented at the 19th Conference on Language, Valletta, Malta, June 14–15; Boston: Development, Boston University. [Google Scholar]
- Duarte, Mel. 1995. Brazilian Portuguese and the null subject parameter: Variation and syntax. Paper presented at Nwave 24, Philadelphia, PA, USA, October 12–15; Philadelphia: University of Philadelphia. [Google Scholar]
- Dye, Cristina, Yarden Kedar, and Barbara Lust. 2019. From lexical to functional categories: New foundations for the study of language development. First Language 39: 9–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faria, Isabel. 1993. A Aquisição da Noção Agente e a Produção de Sujeitos Sintáticos por Crianças Portuguesas até aos Dois Anos e Meio. Revista Internacional de Língua Portuguesa 10: 16–50. [Google Scholar]
- Gerken, LouAnn. 1991. The metrical basis for children’s subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language 30: 431–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerken, LouAnn. 1994. Young children’s representation of prosodic phonology: Evidence from English-speakers’ weak syllable productions. Journal of Memory and Language 33: 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilligan, Gary Martin. 1989. A Cross-Linguistic Approach to the Pro-Drop Parameter. Ph.D. thesis, University of California Press, Berkeley, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Grinstead, John. 2004. Subjects and interface delay in child Spanish and Catalan. Language 80: 40–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guasti, Maria Teresa. 1993. Verb syntax in Italian child grammar: Finite and nonfinite verbs. Language Acquisition 3: 1–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guasti, Maria Teresa. 1996. Acquisition of Italian interrogatives. In Generative Perspectives on Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Guasti, Maria Teresa. 2016. Language Acquisition: The Growth of Grammar. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Haegeman, Liliane. 1990. Non-overt subjects in diary contexts. In Grammar in Progress, GLOW Essays for Henk van Riemsdijk. Dordrecht and Providence: Foris Publications, pp. 167–74. [Google Scholar]
- Haegeman, Liliane. 1997. Register variation, truncation, and subject omission in English and in French. English Language & Linguistics 1: 233–70. [Google Scholar]
- Haegeman, Liliane, and Tabea Ihsane. 2001. Adult null subjects in the non-pro-drop languages: Two diary dialects. Language Acquisition 9: 329–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamann, Cornelia, and Kim Plunkett. 1998. Subjectless sentences in child danish. Cognition 69: 35–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoekstra, Teun, Nina Hyams, and Misha Becker. 1996. The underspecification of number and the licensing of root infinitives. In BUCLD 21: Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, pp. 293–306. [Google Scholar]
- Hoekstra, Teun, and Nina M. Hyams. 1998. Aspects of root infinitives. Lingua 106: 81–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holmberg, Anders. 2010. Null Subject Parameters. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, pp. 88–124. [Google Scholar]
- Holmberg, Anders. 2017. Linguistic typology. In The Oxford Handbook of Universal Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Holmberg, Anders, and Ian Roberts. 2010. Introduction: Parameters in Minimalist Theory. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–57. [Google Scholar]
- Holmberg, Anders, and Urpo Nikanne. 2002. Expletives, subjects, and topics in Finnish. In Subjects, Expletives, and the EPP. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 71–106. [Google Scholar]
- Huang, C.-T. James. 1984. On the distribution and reference of empty pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry 15: 531–74. [Google Scholar]
- Hyams, Nina. 1986. Language Acquisition and the Theory of Parameters. New York: Springer Science & Business Media, vol. 3. [Google Scholar]
- Hyams, Nina. 1991. A reanalysis of null subjects in child language. In Theoretical Issues in Language Acquisition. London: Psychology Press, pp. 259–78. [Google Scholar]
- Hyams, Nina, and Kenneth Wexler. 1993. On the grammatical basis of null subjects in child language. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 421–60. [Google Scholar]
- Jaeggli, Osvaldo, and Kenneth J. Safir. 1989. The null subject parameter and parametric theory. In The Null Subject Parameter. New York: Springer, pp. 1–44. [Google Scholar]
- Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 1986. Some fundamental aspects of language development after age 5. In Language Acquisition Studies in First Language Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, p. 2. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Young-Joo. 2000. Subject/object drop in the acquisition of Korean: A cross-linguistic comparison. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 9: 325–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Yun Jung, and Megha Sundara. 2021. 6–month–olds are sensitive to English morphology. Developmental Science 24: e13089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kiss, Katalin. 1995. Discourse Configurational Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lorusso, Paolo, Claudia Caprin, and Maria Teresa Guasti. 2005. Overt subject distribution in early italian children. Paper presented at the 29th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development-BUCLD 29 Online Proceedings Supplement, Boston, UK, November 5–7; Somerville: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
- MacWhinney, Brian. 2014. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, Volume II: The Database. London: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Magalhães, Telma. 2006. O Sistema Pronominal Sujeito e Objeto na Aquisição do Português Europeu e do Português Brasileiro. Ph.D. thesis, Instituto de Estudos da Linguagem, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, Brazil. [Google Scholar]
- Manzini, M. Rita, and Kenneth Wexler. 1987. Parameters, Binding Theory, and Learnability. Linguistic Inquiry 18: 413–44. [Google Scholar]
- Maratsos, Michael P. 1976. The Use of Definite and Indefinite Reference in Young Children: An Experimental Study of Semantic Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Menuzzi, Sergio. 2000. That-trace effects in portuguese. Fórum Linguístico 2: 13–39. [Google Scholar]
- Modyanova, Nadya, and Ken Wexler. 2007. Semantic and pragmatic language development: Children know ‘that’ better. Paper presented at the 2nd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (GALANA), Montréal, QC, Canada, August 17–19; pp. 297–308. [Google Scholar]
- Morgan, James L. 1996. Prosody and the roots of parsing. Language and Cognitive Processes 11: 69–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nariyama, Shigeko. 2004. Subject ellipsis in english. Journal of Pragmatics 36: 237–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newmeyer, Frederick. 2004. Against a parameter-setting approach to typological variation. Linguistic Variation Yearbook 4: 181–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nicolis, Marco. 2008. The Null Subject Parameter and correlating properties. In The Limits of Syntactic Variation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 132–271. [Google Scholar]
- Orfitelli, Robyn, and Nina Hyams. 2012. Children’s grammar of null subjects: Evidence from comprehension. Linguistic Inquiry 43: 563–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ouhalla, Jamal. 1993. Subject-extraction, negation and the anti-agreement effect. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 11: 477–518. [Google Scholar]
- Patil, Indrajeet. 2021. Visualizations with statistical details: The ’ggstatsplot’ approach. Journal of Open Source Software 6: 3167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Phillips, Colin. 1996. Root infinitives are finite. In Paper Presented at the 20th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, vol. 2, pp. 588–99. [Google Scholar]
- Piaget, Jean. 1952. The Origins of Intelligence in Children. Translated by Margaret Cook. New York: Norton & Co. [Google Scholar]
- Piaget, Jean. 1959. Language and Thought of the Child: Selected Works. New York: Routledge, vol. 5. [Google Scholar]
- Pierce, Amy. 1992. Language Acquisition and Syntactic Theory: A Comparative Study of French and English Child Grammars. Dordrecht: Kluwer. [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. 2021. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Radford, Andrew. 1990. Syntactic Theory and the Acquisition of English syntax: The Nature of Early Child Grammars of English. Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Rasetti, Lucienne. 2000. Interpretive and formal properties of null subjects in early french. Generative Grammar in Geneva 1: 241–74. [Google Scholar]
- Rasetti, Lucienne. 2003. Optional Categories in Early French Syntax: A Developmental Study of Root Infinitives and Null Arguments. Ph.D. thesis, Université de Genève, Geneve, Switzerland. [Google Scholar]
- Rizzi, Luigi. 1982. Negation, wh-movement, and the null subject parameter. In An Annotated Syntax Reader. p. 169. Available online: https://search.library.berkeley.edu/discovery/fulldisplay?docid=cdi_wiley_ebooks_10_1002_9781394260140_ch11_ch11&context=PC&vid=01UCS_BER:UCB&lang=en&search_scope=DN_and_CI&adaptor=Primo%20Central&tab=Default_UCLibrarySearch&query=null,,1982,AND&mode=advanced&offset=0 (accessed on 12 January 2024).
- Rizzi, Luigi. 1986. Null objects in Italian and the theory of pro. Linguistic Inquiry 17: 501–57. [Google Scholar]
- Rizzi, Luigi. 1994. Syntactic Theory and First Language Acquisition: Cross-Linguistic Perspectives. Binding, Dependencies, and Learnability, Chapter Early Null Subjects and Root Null Subjects. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., vol. 2, pp. 249–72. [Google Scholar]
- Rizzi, Luigi. 2005. Grammatically-Based Target-Inconsistencies in Child Language. Available online: https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Grammatically-based-Target-inconsistencies-in-Child-Rizzi/7b3f1546e598d2e9e411ce70ba838c31a7910772 (accessed on 12 January 2024).
- Roberts, Ian. 2010a. Agreement and Head Movement: Clitics, Incorporation, and Defective Goals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, vol. 59. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, Ian. 2010b. A deletion analysis of null subjects. In Parametric Variation: Null Subjects in Minimalist Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, p. 58. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, Ian. 2019. Parameter Hierarchies and Universal Grammar. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Roeper, Thomas, and Bernhard Rohrbacher. 2000. Null Subjects in Early Child English and the Theory of Economy of Projection. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, pp. 345–96. [Google Scholar]
- Saito, Mamoru. 2007. Notes on East Asian argument ellipsis. Language Research 43: 203–27. [Google Scholar]
- Saito, Mamoru. 2016. (a) case for labeling: Labeling in languages without Φ-feature agreement. The Linguistic Review 33: 129–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schütze, Carlson, and Kenneth Wexler. 1996. Subject case licensing and english root infinitives. In Paper Presented at the 20th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Boston: MIT Press, pp. 670–81. [Google Scholar]
- Shi, Rushen, and Melanie Lepage. 2008. The effect of functional morphemes on word segmentation in preverbal infants. Developmental Science 11: 407–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shi, Rushen, Janet F. Werker, and Anne Cutler. 2006. Recognition and Representation of Function Words in English-learning Infants. Infancy 10: 187–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shin, Naomi Lapidus, and Helen Smith Cairns. 2012. The development of NP selection in school-age children: Reference and Spanish subject pronouns. Language Acquisition 19: 3–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simões, Luciene Juliano. 1999. Sujeito nulo na aquisição do Português do Brasil. Cadernos de Estudos Linguísticos 36: 105–30. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder, William. 2002. Parameters: The view from child language. In Proceedings of the Third Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics. Tokyo: Hituzi Shobo, pp. 27–44. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder, William. 2007. Child Language: The Parametric Approach. Oxford Applied Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder, William. 2011. Children’s grammatical conservatism: Implications for syntactic theory. Paper presented at the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA, USA, November 5–7; Somerville: Cascadilla Press, vol. 1, pp. 1–20. [Google Scholar]
- Tomioka, Satoshi. 2003. The semantics of Japanese null pronouns and its cross-linguistic implications. The Interfaces: Deriving and Interpreting Omitted Structures 61: 18. [Google Scholar]
- Torn-Leesik, Reeli, and Maigi Vija. 2012. Acquisition of the impersonal voice by an Estonian child. Journal of Baltic Studies 43: 251–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torrens, Vincent. 1995. The acquisition of inflection in Spanish and Catalan. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 26: 451–72. [Google Scholar]
- Valian, Virginia. 1990. Null subjects: A problem for parameter-setting models of language acquisition. Cognition 35: 105–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valian, Virginia. 1991. Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition 40: 21–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valian, Virginia. 2016. Null Subjects. In The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 386–413. [Google Scholar]
- Vihman, Virve Anneli. 2004. Valency Reduction in Estonian. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Viitso, Tiit Rein. 1997. Fennic. In The Uralic Languages. New York: Routledge, pp. 96–114. [Google Scholar]
- Vija, Maigi, Reeli Torn-Leesik, and Renate Pajusalu. 2009. Tegumood Eesti Lapsekeeles. Tallinn: Eesti Keele Sihtasutus, vol. 5, pp. 329–44. [Google Scholar]
- Villa-García, Julio. 2011. On the role of children’s deterministic learning in the ’no-overt-subject’ stage in the L1 acquisition of Spanish. In Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. vol. 37, pp. 375–388. Available online: https://journals.linguisticsociety.org/proceedings/index.php/BLS/article/view/843 (accessed on 12 January 2024).
- Wang, Qi, Diane Lillo-Martin, Catherine T. Best, and Andrea Levitt. 1992. Null subject versus null object: Some evidence from the acquisition of Chinese and English. Language Acquisition 2: 221–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wexler, Kenneth. 1992. Optional Infinitives, Head Movement and the Economy of Derivation in child Grammar. Occasional Paper, 45. Cambridge, MA: Center for Cognitive Science, MIT. [Google Scholar]
- Yang, Charles. 2002. Knowledge and Learning in Natural Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
Types | Consistent | Partial | Semi | Non-Null-Subject |
---|---|---|---|---|
Examples | Italian, Greek | BP, Finnish | Kriyol, German | English, French |
Main (3SG) | null | overt | overt | overt |
Embedded (3SG) | null | null | overt | overt |
Generic | overt | null | overt | overt |
Expletive | null | null | null | overt |
Radical pro-drop | Consistent null-subject | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Chinese | Italian | EP | Spanish | |
tánlùn | parlare | falar | hablar | |
1SG | tánlùn | parlo | falo | hablo |
2SG | tánlùn | parli | falas | habla |
3SG | tánlùn | parla | fala | hablas |
1PL | tánlùn | parliamo | falamos | hablamos |
2PL | tánlùn | parlate | falam 1 | habláis |
3PL | tánlùn | parlano | falam | hablan |
Partial null-subject | ||
---|---|---|
Brazilian Portuguese | Finnish | |
falar | puhua | |
1SG | falo | puhun |
2SG | fala | puhut |
3SG | fala | puhuu |
1PL | fala | puhutaan |
2PL | falam | puhutte |
3PL | falam | puhuu |
Non-null-subject | Semi-null-subject | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
English | French) | Icelandic (4 distinctions) | Kriyol | |
talk | parler 1 | tala | fala | |
1SG | talk | paKl | tala | fala |
2SG | talk | paKl | talar | fala |
3SG | talks | paKl | talar | fala |
1PL | talk | paKlÕ | tölum | fala |
2PL | talk | parle | talið | fala |
3PL | talk | paKl | tala | fala |
Child | Null Subjects |
---|---|
Diana | 73% |
Martina | 67% |
Raffaello | 79% |
Rosa | 77% |
Children | 75% |
Adults | 74% |
Age Group | N = Participants | Percentage |
---|---|---|
G1 | 14 | 90.7% |
G2 | 18 | 73.5% |
G3 | 25 | 75.5% |
Children | 57 | 78.5% |
Adults | 9 | 63.7% |
Child | Generic Null Subjects | Age of Emergence |
---|---|---|
Maria Eliza | 34 | 3;4 |
Túlio | 42 | 1;9 |
Elias | 1 | 2;7 |
Nino | 9 | 1;8 |
Gustavo | 0 | after 2;10 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bertolino, K. The Setting of the Null Subject Parameters across (Non-)Null-Subject Languages. Languages 2024, 9, 276. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9080276
Bertolino K. The Setting of the Null Subject Parameters across (Non-)Null-Subject Languages. Languages. 2024; 9(8):276. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9080276
Chicago/Turabian StyleBertolino, Karina. 2024. "The Setting of the Null Subject Parameters across (Non-)Null-Subject Languages" Languages 9, no. 8: 276. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9080276
APA StyleBertolino, K. (2024). The Setting of the Null Subject Parameters across (Non-)Null-Subject Languages. Languages, 9(8), 276. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9080276