Next Article in Journal
Clinical Efficacy and Safety of Lidocaine Tape for Topical Anesthesia of the Oral Mucosa: A Preliminary Controlled Trial
Next Article in Special Issue
Assessment of Dental Student Satisfaction after Internships in Collaborative Dental Practices in Saxony—A Retrospective Questionnaire Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Advances in Preventive and Therapeutic Approaches for Dental Erosion: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Implementation of a Full Digital Workflow by 3D Printing Intraoral Splints Used in Dental Education: An Exploratory Observational Study with Respect to Students’ Experiences
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Visual Tooth Color Determination with Different Reference Scales as an Exercise in Dental Students’ Education

1
Department of Prosthodontics, Gerodontology and Dental Materials, Center of Oral Health, University Medicine Greifswald, 17489 Greifswald, Germany
2
Charité, Center for Dental and Craniofacial Sciences, Department of Prosthodontics, Geriatric Dentistry and Craniomandibular Disorders, Campus Benjamin Franklin, 12203 Berlin, Germany
3
Department of Prosthodontics, Carl Gustav Carus Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, 01069 Dresden, Germany
4
Department of Prosthodontics and Material sciences, University Leipzig, 20251 Leipzig, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Dent. J. 2023, 11(12), 275; https://doi.org/10.3390/dj11120275
Submission received: 6 October 2023 / Revised: 7 November 2023 / Accepted: 27 November 2023 / Published: 29 November 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue New Trends in Dental Education and Dental Care)

Abstract

:
Visual color determination (VCD) requires color competence and individual training. The aim of this study was to evaluate the deviations in students’ VCD with two different reference scales. The research hypothesis was that none of the color references would provide a better result. Participants evaluated nine templates randomly using two reference scales (VITA-classical (VC) and 3D-Master-Toothguide (3DM_TG)). The color distance to the chosen color (ΔEab) was calculated in the CIELAB 2000. The sum’s changes in the parameters (LCh°) represented the target variable. Results were evaluated with non-parametric, rank-scaled methods, utilizing the median with a 25%-75% quartile. The significance level (α = 0.05) is determined using the Student’s t-test. The mean ± 95%CI (SD) was −1.27 ± −1.09 (3.18); the median ΔE00 was −1.49 (−1.97; 0.96) for dC3DM_TG. The determination with VC showed noticeable differences (dCVC), with a mean ΔE00 of 0.00 ± 0.00 (2.20) and a median ΔE00 of 0.00 (1.17; 1.71). The standard error was 0.19 for the dCVC and 0.27 for the dC3DM_TG. dC3DM_TG vs. dCVC showed significant differences at p < 0.001. The dental student’s VCD resulted in color deviations, regardless of the reference template used. The color deviations in hue and chroma were comparable, regardless of the reference scale. VCD’s early implementation in dental education is useful to avoid shade misjudgments and potentially expensive remakes of dentures.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Patient satisfaction and acceptance of a dental restorative, prosthodontic fixed, or removable denture are closely related to personal approval and positive expectations regarding the treatment’s outcome. This pertains not only to achieving proper functionality but also to aesthetic appearance. In this context, the reconstruction’s color and, particularly, color determination play a pivotal role. Shade determination is necessary for the reproduction and characterization of artificial restorations and represents a daily challenge for practitioners in dental practice [1,2].
Visual and/or instrumental methods are currently used in dental offices to determine the shade of teeth. They serve as a basis for shade communication with the dental technician and are later the basis for shade control of dental prostheses. The tooth shade is visually matched via direct and continuous comparison of the natural tooth (or “template” in scientific studies) with the corresponding shade reference [3,4]. The aim of color determination is to determine the reference pattern that represents the smallest possible color deviation, i.e., the least perceived color discrepancy [1,5,6,7,8,9,10]. However, it must be stated here that in visible color determination, the reference templates do not always precisely match the tooth to be determined.
The composition of the color references varies and corresponds to the most frequently occurring empirically determined tooth colors in nature. The manufacturer-specific reference scales differ from each other in the empirical selection of colors with color coordinates (L* a* b* values) in the color space, as well as in the material and distribution of the reference samples on the color scale [11]. A “true, visual color match” with a reference sample is subjectively burdened and can lead to more or less visible, significant deviations due to individual color perception [5,11]. Dental prostheses’ color corrections are time-consuming, involve additional effort and costs, require further patient appointments, and may ultimately result in resource-intensive and cost-incurring reconstruction of the dental prostheses [12,13]. In a study, Lehmann et al. investigated the accuracy and reproducibility of visual and digital color determination. They concluded that an accuracy of 72.5% and a reproducibility of 48.0% can be assumed for visual color determination [14]. The subjective perception of the observer varies significantly and depends on parameters such as age, gender [15,16,17], education [18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26] and profession (prior knowledge) [22,25,26,27,28,29,30,31]. Studies show the extent of subjective color perception [26,32,33,34].
The Cartesian coordinates L*, C*, and h° are used for the systematic description of the color parameters brightness (lightness, L*), color intensity (chroma, C*), and hue (h°) [3,35,36,37]. The brightness (L*) describes two poles in the spherical color space: “white” represents the imaginary north pole, while “black” occupies the imaginary south pole, and all gray-tone values are arranged in the vertical imaginary connection line. All other saturated, chromatic hues (h°) are represented at the equator of the sphere, with the axes in the equatorial plane, the a*- and b*-axes, representing the color valences of red–green and blue–yellow, respectively (Figure 1).
The conversion of Cartesian coordinates (L*, a*, and b*) into the mentioned cylindrical coordinates (L*, C*, and h°) is carried out using Formulas (1)–(3) [38].
L a , b = L
C a , b = a 2 + b 2
h ° a , b = atan ( b a )
To perceive color differences (ΔE), colors in the color space should ideally be arranged equidistantly. The color difference between two colors is described as the distance between the involved locations (L1, a1, and b1 as well as L2, a2, and b2) and is calculated using the Euclidean distance Formulas (4) and (5), although distributional asymmetries of colors in this system are known [13,14,35,36,37,38,39].
Δ E ab = ( L 1 L 2 ) 2 + ( a 1 a 2 ) 2 + ( b 1 b 2 ) 2
Δ E ab = Δ L 2 + Δ a 2 + Δ b 2  
For each chromatic color and its mixed color, the corresponding angle h° is given: 0° to 90° for colors between red and yellow, between 90° and 180° for colors between yellow and green. The colors between green and blue are described with the angle h° between 180° and 270°, and the angle between 270° and 360° represents the colors from blue to red. [10,39,40] Regardless of the parameters used (Lab* or L*C* h°), differences can be measured and scientifically evaluated [6,7,8,9,41,42].
An improvement in the cognitive abilities and visual color differentiation skills of the dentist or user is possible with appropriate training sequences [23,31,43,44,45]. The authors used templates that were matched in training with the appropriate reference scale [22,23,26,45,46,47,48,49,50]. McMaugh examined the differentiation ability of different cohorts: undergraduates (first and fourth clinical years), dentists and esthetic dentistry specialists, and dental technicians [22]. He found no difference between the results of the cohort of students from different semesters. Lack of student instruction and practice is discussed as a cause of the result [22]. A significant difference was found between the cohort of first-year students and specialists. He based his result on the experience of the student cohort and the enthusiasm with which a specialist implements his focus in his work. The difference between the dentist cohort and the specialists was explained by the fact that the dentists were overwhelmed by the large number of different reference shades and limited themselves to only three to four “standard shades” in their daily practice [28]. Without doubt, the good result of the dental technician cohort (dental ceramists) was due to the intensive examination of the tooth shade and the continuous checking of the (working) result. Capa et al. found in their study with dentists, dental staff, and students that experience and expertise improve and increase tooth shade determination results [28]. Haddad et al., on the other hand, could not find a significant difference in the results in the study group between the subject groups (dentists (n = 295) and students (n = 319)) and the level of experience [16]. The study group around Nakhaei et al. concurred with this result under the condition that a special reference scale (VITA 3D-Master, V3D M) was used [31]. The differences when using another reference shade guide (VITA Classical, VC) led to significant differences between the cohorts [31].
Haddad et al. demonstrated the influence of age and gender on tooth color matching in their international study with 614 participants (305 females and 309 males) from 15 universities. They found that female participants achieved significantly better results in tooth color differentiation than male participants [16]. Based on their findings, they concluded that females play an important role in shade selection and shade matching. Furthermore, in this research, they found that the level of experience does not play a crucial role in color matching. However, Haddad et al. disagree with the research of Miranda, who found that males and participants had a significantly better result in color differentiation than females [17]. Furthermore, Haddad et al. contradicted the studies of Ristic et al. and Olms et al., who found that participants achieved significantly better results in tooth color matching after participating in a curriculum of tooth color matching [49,51]. Here, the demand was postulated that color differentiation should be implemented in the training in order to avoid large color deviations, which result in a new supply or a costly improvement [49].
Capa et al. also found a positive correlation with the result, while gender and the use of visual aids (glasses or contact lenses) had no influence on the determination of tooth shade [28]. Without doubt, the good result of the dental technician cohort (ceramists) was due to the intensive examination of the tooth shade and the continuous color checking in the working progress. The authors concluded and postulated that tooth color determination should be conducted by clinicians only who participated in continuing education programs and other training courses on color competence. This can increase experience and expertise and improve tooth color identification results [28].
In summary, Wee et al. referenced studies that explored the perceptibility of a color difference, denoted as ΔEab [37,52]. Delta E (∆E) defines the perceived color distance, indicating the difference between two colors; the index “ab” refers to the reference quantity, designating the color space (CIE Lab). This measurement can be used for quantifying works related to colors. Several studies have investigated the smallest color distance still distinguishable by the average viewer (“usual user”) [53]. As a result, both ΔE = 1 and ΔE = 2.5 were found. (Table 1) Various influencing factors significantly impact visual color matching, with adherence to the observation time being one of them.
To obtain reliable results in evaluating color deviations, specific conditions must be precisely defined. These conditions include illuminance, adaptation time to illumination, and the color or brightness of the near and far surroundings in the viewing field. To ensure consistent and accurate conditions, visual sampling should be conducted in sample booths that adhere to these defined conditions [47].
Color differences (ΔEab) ranging from ΔEab = 1 [36] to 2.54 [32] and ΔEab = 2.72 [55] to 3.8 [56] were found in vitro, and ΔEab = 3.7 to 6.8 [55] were found in vivo. Baltzer et al. showed that ΔEab values of 1 to 3 were required, and higher ΔEab values of 3 to 6 are considered suboptimal [3]. (Table 1) The “perceptibility threshold” and “acceptance threshold” are crucial factors in this context. The thresholds for color differentiation differ between groups of individuals (dentists, dental students, dental assistants, dental technicians, and laypeople): the 50:50% perceptibility thresholds (ΔEab = 1.2 or ΔE00 = 0.8) and the 50:50% acceptance thresholds (ΔEab = 2.7 or ΔE00 = 1.8) [57]. These threshold values can serve as control instruments and have been implemented in the ISO standard of 2016 [57,58].
The aim of this study was to investigate the suitability of two reference scales for determining a template’s color. The null hypothesis is that none of the reference scales used leads to a significantly better result in the VCD of untrained participants.

2. Materials and Methods

Preclinical dental students (female n = 28 and male n = 10, Figure 2) from the University Medical Center Leipzig, the University of Dresden, the University of Berlin, and the Greifswald University participated in the study. The participants should be enrolled dental students; no further specific inclusion or exclusion criteria were formulated. The study participants voluntarily took part in the curriculum for tooth shade determination, which is offered annually at the participating universities as an additional course to the curriculum. Prior to study participation, the subjects underwent a color vision screening using the Ishihara test. For the screening of color blindness (Congenital Color Vision Deficiency (CCVD)), the Ishihara color charts were displayed on smartphone screens for the first time on Science Day, and the results were tested for sensitivity and specificity in comparison to the classic display on the PC monitor. These results are currently being summarized in a further publication [59].
The average age was 23.5 years ± 2.65 years, with a median age of 23.0 ± 13.0 years. (Figure 3).
The study was reviewed and approved by an Ethics Committee at the University of Greifswald (BB 175/22). The participants evaluated 9 randomly selected templates (incisors of another reference scale (Chromascop® Shade guide, Ivoclar Vivadent, Schaan (Liechtenstein)) twice using two different reference scales (VITA classical (VC) and 3D-Master Toothguide (3DM TG), both from VITA Zahnfabrik, Bad Säckingen, Germany). The VITA 3D-Master Toothguide reference scale was used to improve the comparability of the results. The linear arrangement of the reference patterns was examined by Paravina et al. and leads to a comparable, intuitive template’s matching procedure [48]. The templates were measured in advance with a calibrated spectrometer (VITA EasyShade V, VITA Zahnfabrik). The tooth shade determination was carried out under clinical conditions, i.e., neutral ambient conditions were ensured for reproducible tooth shade determination. This included uniform illumination of the environment with daylight lamps (brightness: 1750 lumens; color temperature: 4500 K) [30,60].
Differentiation was performed in a seated position at a viewing distance of 25 to 35 cm, following the recommendations of Cordel et al. [12]. The evaluation included the color distance of the template to the chosen pattern in the L* ab color space and was converted into the CIELAB 2000 color space [61]. The distance (ΔEab) was calculated using the Euclidean formula (4), the result was summarized, and the mean was calculated for both groups. The changes in the sum of the differences for each participant represented the target variable. The differences (ΔE) within the used parameters (L*C h°) were evaluated and subjected to evaluation. The results were evaluated using a statistical program (SPSS v21, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) with non-parametric, rank-scaled methods using the median and the 25%- and 75%-quartile. The Student’s t-test was used to verify the significance level (α = 0.05).

3. Results

The overall results for the 3DM TG reference scale showed for the parameters dL3DM TG, with a mean ΔE00 ± 95% confidence interval (standard deviation, SD) of 0.9 ± −1.04 (3.1) and a median ΔE00 of 0.0 (25%-; 75%-quantile: −0.7; 4.0) for VC. The determination using the VC reference scale showed noticeable color differences in dLVC, with a mean ΔE00 of 1.4 ± 1.05 (3.1) and a median ΔE00 of 0.94 (25%-; 75%-quantile: 0; 3.7). The standard error (SE) was comparable between the dLVC group (0.27) and the 3D-Master group (dL3DM TG 0.26). (Figure 4) The differences (t-test) in the results of both reference scales were not statistically significant (p = 0.683).
For the parameters dh°3DM TG, there were noticeable color differences, with a mean of 1.68 ± −1.3 (3.83) and a median ΔE00 of 1.64 (25%-; 75%-quantile: 0; 4.84) for dh°3DM TG. The determination using the VC reference scale showed noticeable color differences dh°VC, with a mean ΔE00 of 1.59 ± 1.11 (3.26) and a median ΔE00 of 0.45 (25%-; 75%-quantile: −0.45; 4.84). The standard error (SE) was approximately comparable between the dh°VC group (0.28) and the 3D-Master TG group (dh°3DM TG) (0.33) (Table 2).
The differences (t-test) in the results of both reference scales were not statistically significant; dL’ p = 0.196 and dh’ p = 0.839, but dC’ showed significant differences (p < 0.001). The mean was −1.27 ± −1.09 (3.18), and the median ΔE00 was −1.49 (25%-; 75%-quantile: −1.97; 0.96) for dC3DM TG. The determination using the VC reference scale showed noticeable color differences in dCVC, with a mean ΔE00 of 0.00 ± 0.00 (2.20) and a median ΔE00 of 0.00 (25%-; 75%-quantile: −1.17; 1.71). The standard error (SE) was 0.19 for the dCVC group and 0.27 in the 3D-Master group (dC3DM TG).

4. Discussion

Within the limitations of this study, the research hypothesis that there were no differences in the color differentiation results of the shade guides used (VITA Toothguide 3D-Master vs. VITA Classical) that demonstrated superior performance in determining the shade of a template tooth was not rejected. Determining the tooth shade is a crucial step in daily dental practice, and existing restorations near the tooth to be restored can complicate shade differentiation due to its chameleon effect. Consequently, the shade of the existing tooth may not be accurately represented on the shade scale, and the most suitable and closest tooth shade is selected to minimize shade deviation [12]. Numerous articles and studies have shown that experts (dentists, dental technicians) can determine tooth shade more accurately than patients, with shade deviations of ΔE < 2 being visually perceptible and recognizable [27,55].
In this present study, the VITA 3D-Master TG reference color scale yielded better results, possibly due to its broader color space coverage resulting from a higher number of references (n = 29) and a more evenly distributed, equidistant arrangement in the color space [57]. On the other hand, the VITA Classical reference scale, with only 16 color templates and an empirically random distribution of colors, led to faster but more challenging decisions. For better method comparison, Paravina et al. advocated a linear arrangement of shade samples, which was intuitively used by the expert group (dental technicians and dentists) [33]. The color arrangement in the reference scale’s tab leads the user intuitively and quickly to a result in direct comparison. Paravina et al. see the reason for this in the physiology of color vision. Thus, the authors assume that in color vision, a separation of brightness, saturation, and hue is not possible for the human eye, but rather a color impression consisting of all three-color dimensions and their color interactions is perceived [62].
One limitation of this study that needs to be discussed is that the results were limited to a cohort of young dental students. These students were in the pre-clinical training phase and were only taught about tooth color determination in a lecture. Up to this point, they had no clinical experience with patients. Even though McMaugh found no differences between the cohort groups of preclinical students (first and fourth year students), he did find that the misjudgment of tooth color became significantly lower in the postgraduate phase [22]. He explained this improvement by citing the increasing clinical experience of practitioners and the enthusiasm and precision with which restorative dentistry specialists in particular approach shade selection [22]. Capa et al. also found that the cohort comparison of dentists, dental assistants, and dental technicians achieved better results in tooth shade selection compared to students [28]. For this reason, Paravina et al. argued that teaching tooth shade differentiation should be introduced at a later stage in the dental curriculum [44]. Another limitation of the study could be the number of participants. Although the number of study participants should always be higher, the cohort studied here consists of students who voluntarily took part in the annual “Science Day of the Universities”.
A further limitation of the present study is the proband’s number. The results of this research are based on a cohort of preclinical students who were instructed in the performance of clinical tooth shade differentiation in a lecture. As with all clinical studies, the results should not only be broadly based, but the cohort should also have different levels of prior knowledge. In terms of gender distribution, it should be noted that almost three-quarters of the participants were female (74%), and only a quarter of the participants were male (26%). In further studies in this area of tooth shade determination, attention should also be paid to the equal distribution of the two gender groups in order to be able to work out a gender difference.
Another aspect that needs to be discussed concerning the results presented here is the use of VITA 3D-Master TG. Although the participants were instructed in the use of the reference scale in advance, in contrast to the use of the VITA Classical reference scale, the VITA 3D-Master TG shade guides the user through a step-by-step approximation of the tooth shade selection. In this process, after determining the color brightness (five color gradations), the color intensity (five categories) is determined. Finally, as the last step, the shade (in the yellowish or reddish range with three categories) is determined. Through the determination of lightness (value) at the first level, up to 60% of incorrect assessments are eliminated. Both further steps, the determination of chroma and hue, reduce color-matching errors with a compliance of almost 70% [33,34].
A curriculum during training is recommended as an essential factor for success within tooth shade differentiation [23,43,44,45,46,51,63]. There is agreement on the fact that an improvement of cognitive abilities and skills in visual color differentiation is possible through adequate training [45,47,49,50,64]. At this point, Paravina et al. stated that courses in which information about color with prosthetic or conservative emphasis, color science, and the fundamentals of color vision; the influence of environmental parameters such as color-matching lamps (color-rendering index); and color references (VITA 3D-Master Bleached Guide) are considered useful [48]. He considered the appropriate time for the curriculum to be in the post-graduate phase rather than in pre-clinical training [48].
However, the evaluation method used in this present study did not establish the superiority of the VITA Toothguide 3D-Master over the VITA Classical (p > 0.05), leading to the acceptance of the null hypothesis. It should be discussed whether the better results with the VITA Toothguide 3D-Master stem from its closer proximity to the template’s color. Another contributing factor could be the equidistant color pattern distribution of the VITA Toothguide 3D-Master in the color space, enabling better representation of color nuances compared to the VITA Classical reference scale.
The target variable in this study focused on color deviation from the original: larger L* a b coordinate differences resulted in large deviations, while narrow L* a b coordinates resulted in smaller deviations in ΔE from the template. This demonstrates the sufficiency of the subjects’ color choices and the suitability of the color references used in each case.
Visual shade matching of adjacent teeth with a shade reference remains the standard method for shade selection, despite its susceptibility to inhomogeneity. The adequacy of matching the natural tooth color in direct visual matching depends on factors like viewing distance and the distribution of colors in the reference scales. It is reasonable to assume that the sought-after color may not be exactly represented in any available reference shade guide. No significant difference was found in color differentiation when using two color reference scales for subject decisions. Similar results obtained with the evaluation method used here need further investigation [65].
In dental practice, electronic spectrophotometers significantly increase the accuracy of color determination compared to visual methods. They offer stability in results, reducing the impact of local measurement conditions and handling variability. To minimize subjective variance and ensure consistency in tooth shade determination, electronic instrumental aids have gained popularity in dental practices. The digital spectrophotometer, such as the VITA Easyshade® (VITA Zahnfabrik, Bad Säckingen, Germany), serves as a reference standard for tooth shade determination in clinical studies [8,42,66,67,68]. Various scientific studies report reliability rates of 87.4% to 99.0% and accuracy rates of 67% to 93% for digital dental colorimeters [69,70]. Despite some deviations in calculated color coordinates from the spectrophotometric reference system, the instrumental color measurement demonstrated excellent repeatability [6]. For daily practice, however, an additional visual inspection and validation of the instrumental color measurements is still recommended.

5. Conclusions

From this, the following can be concluded and postulated:
  • In view of patients’ increasing desire for highly esthetic dentures, tooth shade determination should be part of dental training;
  • In order to reduce misjudgments in tooth shade determination, training and instruction in tooth shade differentiation should be implemented in dental training or in an additional curriculum;
  • The kind of reference scale used does not play a role in tooth shade determination training;
  • Rather, the differentiation result improves through application in the dental practice or through additional training.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.U.K. and H.A.J.; methodology, T.U.K., H.A.J., K.B., W.B.H. and E.P.; investigation, H.A.J.; resources, T.U.K.; data, H.A.J. and T.U.K.; writing—original draft preparation, T.U.K.; writing—review and editing, T.U.K. and E.P.; visualization, T.U.K.; supervision, H.A.J.; project administration, T.U.K. and H.A.J.; funding acquisition, T.U.K., H.A.J., K.B. and W.B.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by VITA Zahnfabrik, Bad Säckingen, Germany.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Greifswald (BB 175/22, 10.01.2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to restrictions, e.g., privacy.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Chu, S.J.; Devigus, A.; Paravina, R.; Mieleszko, A. Fundamentals of Color: Shade Matching and Communication in Esthetic Dentistry, 2nd ed.; Quintessence Pub Co.: Batavia, IL, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  2. Chu, S.J.; Trushkowsky, R.D.; Paravina, R.D. Dental color matching instruments and systems. Review of clinical and research aspects. J. Dent. 2010, 38 (Suppl. S2), e2–e16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Baltzer, A.; Kaufmann-Jinoian, V. The Determination of the Tooth Colors. Quintessenz Zahntech 2004, 30, 726–740. [Google Scholar]
  4. Stoll, R.; Frankenberger, R. Praxis und Theorie der Zahnfarbe bei restaurativer Therapie. Quintessenz 2010, 61, 581–585. [Google Scholar]
  5. Fondriest, J. Shade matching in restorative dentistry: The science and strategies. Int. J. Periodontics Restor. Dent. 2003, 23, 467–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Lehmann, K.M.; Igiel, C.; Schmidtmann, I.; Scheller, H. Four color-measuring devices compared with a spectrophotometric reference system. J. Dent. 2010, 38 (Suppl. S2), e65–e70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Oomen, J.A.; Slager, C.J.; Lehmann, K.G.; Schuurbiers, J.C.; Serruys, P.W. Color quantification in angioscopic video images. Med. Prog. Technol. 1995, 21, 39–46. [Google Scholar]
  8. Schwabacher, W.B.; Goodkind, R.J. Three-dimensional color coordinates of natural teeth compared with three shade guides. J. Prosthet. Dent. 1990, 64, 425–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Stokes, M.; Fairchild, M.D.; Berns, R.S. Colorimetric quantified visual tolerances for pictorial images. In Comparison of Color Images Presented in Different Media; Pearson, M., Ed.; Graphic Arts and Inter-Soc. Color Council (TAGA): Warrendale, PN, USA, 1992; Volume 2, pp. 757–777. [Google Scholar]
  10. Wyszecki, G.; Stiles, W.S. Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulas; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: London, UK, 1967. [Google Scholar]
  11. DIN EN ISO 22598:2020; Dentistry—Colour Tabs for Intraoral Tooth Colour Determination. International Organization for Standardization: Vernier, Geneva, 2020.
  12. Corcodel, N.; Zenthöfer, A.; Setz, J.; Rammelsberg, P.; Hassel, A.J. Estimating costs for shade matching and shade corrections of fixed partial dentures for dental technicians in Germany: A pilot investigation. Acta Odontol. Scand. 2011, 69, 319–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Mjor, I.A.; Burke, F.J.; Wilson, N.H. The relative cost of different restorations in the UK. Br. Dent. J. 1997, 182, 286–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Lehmann, K.; Devigus, A.; Wentaschek, S.; Igiel, C.; Scheller, H.; Paravina, R. Comparison of visual shade matching and electronic color measurement device. Int. J. Esthet. Dent. 2017, 12, 396–404. [Google Scholar]
  15. Winkler, S.; Boberick, K.G.; Weitz, K.S.; Datikashvili, I.; Wood, R. Shade Matching by Dental Students. J. Oral. Implantol. 2006, 32, 256–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Haddad, H.J.; Jakstat, H.A.; Arnetzl, G.; Borbely, J.; Vichi, A.; Dumfahrt, H.; Renault, P.; Corcodel, N.; Pohlen, B.; Marada, G.; et al. Does gender and experience influence shade matching quality? J. Dent. 2009, 37, e40–e44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Milagre, V.; Teixeira, M.; Miranda, M.; Osorio Silva, C.; Pinto, J. Effect of Gender, Experience, and Value on Color Perception. Oper. Dent. 2012, 37, 228–233. [Google Scholar]
  18. Bahanan, S. Shade matching quality among dental students using visual and instrumental methods. J. Dent. 2014, 41, 48–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Sagars, J. Shade matching for today’s dentistry. Dent. Econ. 2002, 1, 62–67. [Google Scholar]
  20. Joseph, C.L.M.; Saltzgaber, J.; Havstad, S.L.; Johnson, C.C.; Johnson, D.; Peterson, E.L.; Alexander, G.; Couper, M.P.; Ownby, D.R. Comparison of early-, late-, and non-participants in a school-based asthma management program for urban high school students. Trials 2011, 12, 141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Luck, O.; Reitemeier, B.; Scheuch, K. Testing of fine motor skills in dental students. Eur. J. Dent. Educ. 2000, 4, 10–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. McMaugh, D.R. A comparative analysis of the colour matching ability of dentists, dental students, and ceramic technicians. Aust. Dent. J. 1977, 22, 165–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Llena, C.; Forner, L.; Ferrari, M.; Amengual, J.; Llambes, G.; Lozano, E. Toothguide Training Box for dental color choice training. J. Dent. Educ. 2011, 75, 360–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Samra, A.; Moro, M.; Mazur, R.; Vieira, S.; De Souza, E.; Freire, A.; Rached, R. Performance of Dental Students in Shade Matching: Impact of Training. J. Esthet. Restor. Dent. 2017, 29, e24–e32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Sinmazisik, G.; Trakyali, G.; Tarcin, B. Evaluating the ability of dental technician students and graduate dentists to match tooth color. J. Prosthet. Dent. 2014, 112, 1559–1566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Alomari, M.; Chadwick, R.G. Factors influencing the shade matching performance of dentists and dental technicians when using two different shade guides. Br. Dent. J. 2011, 211, e32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bücking, W. The correct tooth shade determination—A challenge for dentists and dental technicians. Quintessenz 2006, 53, 1199–1207. (In German) [Google Scholar]
  28. Çapa, N.; Malkondu, O.; Kazazoğlu, E.; Çalikkocaoğlu, S. Evaluating factors that affect the shade-matching ability of dentists, dental staff members and laypeople. J. Am. Dent. Assoc. 2010, 141, 71–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Douglas, R.D.; Steinhauer, T.J.; Wee, A.G. Intraoral determination of the tolerance of dentists for perceptibility and acceptability of shade mismatch. J. Prosthet. Dent. 2007, 97, 200–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gasparik, C.; Tofan, A.; Culic, B.; Badea, M.; Dudea, D. Influence of light source and clinical experience on shade matching. Clujul. Med. 2014, 87, 30–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Nakhaei, M.; Ghanbarzadeh, J.; Amirinejad, S.; Alavi, S.; Rajatihaghi, H. The Influence of Dental Shade Guides and Experience on the Accuracy of Shade Matching. J. Contemp. Dent. Pract. 2016, 17, 22–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Delta, E. Wikipedia. Available online: https://de.wikipedia.org/wiki/Delta_E:Wikipedia—Freeencyclopedia (accessed on 25 August 2005).
  33. Hassel, A.J.; Zenthofer, A.; Corcodel, N.; Hildenbrandt, A.; Reinelt, G.; Wiesberg, S. Determination of VITA Classical shades w ith the 3D-Master shade guide. Acta Odontol. Scand. 2013, 71, 721–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Zenthöfer, A.; Wiesberg, S.; Hildenbrandt, A.; Reinelt, G.; Rammelsberg, P.; Hassel, A. Selecting VITA classical shades with the VITA 3D-master shade guide. Int. J. Prosthodont. 2014, 27, 376–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Gomez-Polo, C.; Gomez-Polo, M.; Celemin Vinuela, A.; Martinez Vazquez de Parga, J.A. A clinical study relating CIELCH coordinates to the color dimensions of the 3D-Master System in a Spanish population. J. Prosthet. Dent. 2015, 113, 185–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kuehni, R.G.; Marcus, R.T. An Experiment in Visual Scaling of Small Color Differences. Color Res. Appl. 1979, 4, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lindsey, D.T.; Wee, A.G. Perceptibility and acceptability of CIELAB color differences in computer-simulated teeth. J. Dent. 2007, 35, 593–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Burkinshaw, S.M. Colour in relation to dentistry. Fundamentals of colour science. Br. Dent. J. 2004, 196, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Agoston, G.A. Color Theory and Its Application in Art and Design; Springer-Verlag: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1979; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
  40. Judd, D.B.; Wyszecki, G. Color in Business, Science, and Industry; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1975; Volume 3. [Google Scholar]
  41. Tung, F.F.; Goldstein, G.R.; Jang, S.; Hittelman, E. The repeatability of an intraoral dental colorimeter. J. Prosthet. Dent. 2002, 88, 585–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Yuan, J.C.; Brewer, J.D.; Monaco, E.A., Jr.; Davis, E.L. Defining a natural tooth color space based on a 3-dimensional shade system. J. Prosthet. Dent. 2007, 98, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Olms, C.; Klinke, T.; Pirek, P.; Hannak, W.B. Randomized multi-centre study on the effect of training on tooth shade matching. J. Dent. 2013, 41, 1259–1263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Paravina, R.D.; O’Neill, P.N.; Swift, E.J.; Nathanson, D.; Goodacre, C.J. Teaching of color in predoctoral and postdoctoral dental education in 2009. J. Dent. 2010, 38, e34–e40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Pecho, O.E.; Della Bona, A. Teaching and Training Color Determination in Dentistry. In Teaching and Training Color Determination in Dentistry; Della Bona, A., Ed.; Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2020; pp. 39–45. [Google Scholar]
  46. Zhang, L.; Fan, J.; Tian, M.; Wu, G.; Zhang, S.; Chen, J. Use of Tooth Guide Trainer on Dental Students’ Training of Shade Matching. In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Information Technology in Medicine and Education (ITME), Huangshan, China, 13–15 November 2015. [Google Scholar]
  47. Clary, J.A.; Ontiveros, J.C.; Cron, S.G.; Paravina, R.D. Influence of light source, polarization, education, and training on shade matching quality. J. Prosthet. Dent. 2016, 116, 91–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Paravina, R.D.; Powers, J.M. Esthetic Color Training in Dentistry; Elsevier Mosby: St. Louis, MI, USA, 2004; p. 272. [Google Scholar]
  49. Ristic, I.; Stankovic, S.; Paravina, R.D. Influence of Color Education and Training on Shade Matching Skills. J. Esthet. Restor. Dent. 2016, 28, 287–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Goodkind, R.J.; Loupe, M.J. Teaching of color in predoctoral and postdoctoral dental education in 1988. J. Prosthet. Dent. 1992, 67, 713–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Olms, C.; Haak, R.; Jakstat, H.A. Development and implementation of the Clinical Tooth Shade Differentiation Course–an evaluation over 3 years. GMS J. Med. Educ. 2016, 33, 8–14. [Google Scholar]
  52. Wee, A.G.; Lindsey, D.T.; Kuo, S.; Johnston, W.M. Color accuracy of commercial digital cameras for use in dentistry. Dent. Mater. 2006, 22, 553–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Has, M.; Newman, T. (Eds.) Color Management: Current Practice and the Adoption of a New Standard. Available online: http://www.color.org/wpaper1.xalter (accessed on 5 October 2023).
  54. Klinke-Wilberg, T.; Biffar, R. Influence of Surrounding Conditions on Visual Determination of Tooth Shade. J. Dent. Oral Care 2018, 4, 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Klinke, T.; Bratner, S.; Hannak, W.; Boening, K.; Jakstat, H. Influence of viewing distance on visual color differentiation in vitro. Int. J. Prosthodont. 2022, 35, 219–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Ragain, J.C., Jr.; Johnston, W.M. Color acceptance of direct dental restorative materials by human observers. Color Res. Appl. 2000, 25, 278–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. DIN ISO/TR 28642:2016; Dentistry—Guidance on Colour Measurement. International Organization for Standardization: Vernier, Geneva, 2016.
  58. Paravina, R.D.; Ghinea, R.; Herrera, L.J.; Bona, A.D.; Igiel, C.; Linninger, M.; Sakai, M.; Takahashi, H.; Tashkandi, E.; Perez Mdel, M. Color difference thresholds in dentistry. J. Esthet. Restor. Dent. 2015, 27 (Suppl. S1), 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Klinke, T.; Hannak, W.; Jakstat, H.; Boening, K. Sensitivity and specificity of Ishihara test presented on smartphone displays. Int. Dent. J. 2023. under review. [Google Scholar]
  60. Bratner, S.; Hannak, W.; Boening, K.; Klinke, T. Comparison of different illumination intensities of mobile units for tooth color differentiation: An in-vitro study. J. Prosthet. Dent. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Sharma, G.; Wu, W.; Dalal, E. The CIEDE2000 color-difference formula: Implementation notes, supplementary test data, and mathematical observations. Col. Res. Appl. 2005, 30, 21–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Paravina, R.D.; Powers, J.M.; Fay, R.-M. Dental Color Standards: Shade Tab Arrangement. J. Esthet. Restor. Dent. 2001, 13, 254–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Bratner, S.R.; Jakstat, H.A. Teaching and Learning visual tooth shade discrimination. In 12 Forum für den Lichttechnischen Nachwuchs; Lux, J., Ed.; Ilmenau University of Technology: Ilmenau, Germany, 2015; pp. 18–19. [Google Scholar]
  64. Bergen, S.F. Color Education for the Dental Profession; New York University College of Dentistry: New York, NY, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  65. Bratner, S.R.; Hannak, W.; Boening, K.; Klinke, T. Color determination with no-match-templates using two different tooth color scales—An in vitro evaluation. J. Esthet. Restor. Dent. 2020, 32, 593–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Della Bona, A.; Barrett, A.A.; Rosa, V.; Pinzetta, C. Visual and instrumental agreement in dental shade selection: Three distinct observer populations and shade matching protocols. Dent. Mater. 2009, 25, 276–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Gómez-Polo, C.; Gómez-Polo, M.; Martínez Vazquez De Parga, J.; Celemin-Viñuela, A. 3D Master Toothguide according to L*, C*, and h* coordinates. Color Res. Appl. 2015, 40, 518–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Meireless, S.; Demarco, F.; Dos, S.I.; Dumith, S.; Bona, A. Validation and reliability of visual assessment with a shade guide for tooth-color classification. Oper. Dent. 2008, 33, 121–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Olms, C.; Setz, J.M. The repeatability of digital shade measurement—A clinical study. Clin. Oral. Investig. 2013, 17, 1161–1166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Kim-Pusateri, S.; Brewer, J.D.; Davis, E.L.; Wee, A.G. Reliability and accuracy of four dental shade-matching devices. J. Prosthet. Dent. 2009, 101, 193–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The CIE-LCh system. The localization of the point “F” can be specified as the color angle (hue value, H in h°) and vector length C* (chroma) of the color saturation in the cylinder coordinate (L*, C*, and h°).
Figure 1. The CIE-LCh system. The localization of the point “F” can be specified as the color angle (hue value, H in h°) and vector length C* (chroma) of the color saturation in the cylinder coordinate (L*, C*, and h°).
Dentistry 11 00275 g001
Figure 2. Gender distribution in the proband’s cohort [%].
Figure 2. Gender distribution in the proband’s cohort [%].
Dentistry 11 00275 g002
Figure 3. The distribution of participants’ ages has a mean of 23.5 ± 2.65 years.
Figure 3. The distribution of participants’ ages has a mean of 23.5 ± 2.65 years.
Dentistry 11 00275 g003
Figure 4. Deviation (ΔE00) for color determination in hue value (h°), lightness (L), and vector length C* (chroma) with VITA Classical (VC) and with VITA 3D–Master-Toothguide (3DM) reference scale of student’s color determination.
Figure 4. Deviation (ΔE00) for color determination in hue value (h°), lightness (L), and vector length C* (chroma) with VITA Classical (VC) and with VITA 3D–Master-Toothguide (3DM) reference scale of student’s color determination.
Dentistry 11 00275 g004
Table 1. Interpretation of color distances ΔE as Euclidean distances of L*a*b* values or from polar coordinates L*C*h* [32,54].
Table 1. Interpretation of color distances ΔE as Euclidean distances of L*a*b* values or from polar coordinates L*C*h* [32,54].
ΔEValuation
0.0–0.5exact match/no difference in color
0.5–1.0very good match/small difference, visible for a trained eye
1.0–2.0non-recognizable color difference, good match/acceptable
2.0–4.0recognizable color difference, poor match
4.0–5.0noticeable color difference, hardly acceptable
>5.0mismatch/totally unacceptable, difference will be evaluated as a different color
Table 2. Distribution of the Cartesian coordinates L*, a*, and b* as a function of the reference scales used, which are used for the systematic description of the color parameters lightness (L*), color intensity (chroma, C*), and hue (hue, h°).
Table 2. Distribution of the Cartesian coordinates L*, a*, and b* as a function of the reference scales used, which are used for the systematic description of the color parameters lightness (L*), color intensity (chroma, C*), and hue (hue, h°).
VITA 3DM TGVC
dLdCdh°dLdCdh°
Mean ± SD0.97 ± 3.06−1.27 ± 3.181.68 ± 3.831.45 ± 3.09−0.00 ± 2.201.59 ± 3.26
Confidence interval (95% CI)0.45–1.49−1.81–−0.721.03–2.330.93–1.98−0.37–0.371.03–2.14
Median0.00−1.491.640.940.000.45
Standard error (SE)0.260.270.330.270.190.28
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Klinke, T.U.; Hannak, W.B.; Böning, K.; Jakstat, H.A.; Prause, E. Visual Tooth Color Determination with Different Reference Scales as an Exercise in Dental Students’ Education. Dent. J. 2023, 11, 275. https://doi.org/10.3390/dj11120275

AMA Style

Klinke TU, Hannak WB, Böning K, Jakstat HA, Prause E. Visual Tooth Color Determination with Different Reference Scales as an Exercise in Dental Students’ Education. Dentistry Journal. 2023; 11(12):275. https://doi.org/10.3390/dj11120275

Chicago/Turabian Style

Klinke, Thomas U., Wolfgang B. Hannak, Klaus Böning, Holger A. Jakstat, and Elisabeth Prause. 2023. "Visual Tooth Color Determination with Different Reference Scales as an Exercise in Dental Students’ Education" Dentistry Journal 11, no. 12: 275. https://doi.org/10.3390/dj11120275

APA Style

Klinke, T. U., Hannak, W. B., Böning, K., Jakstat, H. A., & Prause, E. (2023). Visual Tooth Color Determination with Different Reference Scales as an Exercise in Dental Students’ Education. Dentistry Journal, 11(12), 275. https://doi.org/10.3390/dj11120275

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop